Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T23:34:12.309Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Purpose in life and cognitive health: a 28-year prospective study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 March 2024

Angelina R. Sutin*
Affiliation:
Florida State University College of Medicine, Tallahassee, FL, USA
Martina Luchetti
Affiliation:
Florida State University College of Medicine, Tallahassee, FL, USA
Yannick Stephan
Affiliation:
Euromov, University of Montpellier, Montpellier, France
Antonio Terracciano
Affiliation:
Florida State University College of Medicine, Tallahassee, FL, USA
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: A. R. Sutin, Florida State University College of Medicine, 1115 W. Call Street, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA. E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Objectives:

To examine the prospective association between purpose in life measured at three points across middle and older adulthood and cognitive outcomes assessed 8–28 years later.

Design:

Prospective Study.

Setting:

Wisconsin Longitudinal Study of Aging (WLS).

Participants:

WLS participants who reported on their purpose in life at Round 4 (1992–1994; Mage = 52.58), Round 5 (2003–2007; Mage = 63.74), and/or Round 6 (2010–2012; Mage = 70.25) and were administered a cognitive battery at Round 7 (2020; Mage = 79.94) were included in the analysis (N = 4,632).

Measurements:

Participants completed the Ryff measure of purpose in life and were administered the telephone interview for cognitive status and measures of verbal fluency, digit ordering, and numeric reasoning.

Results:

Purpose in life measured at age 52 was related to better global cognitive function and verbal fluency but unrelated to dementia at age 80. In contrast, purpose in life at ages 63–70 was associated with lower likelihood of dementia, as well as better global cognitive function and verbal fluency at age 80. The effect sizes were modest (median Beta coefficient = .05; median odds ratio = .85). A slightly steeper decline in purpose in life between ages 52 and 70 was found for individuals with dementia at age 80.

Conclusions:

Purpose in life is associated with healthier cognitive function measured up to 28 years later. Individuals with lower purpose, especially in their 60s or older, and with steeper declines in purpose, are more likely to have dementia at age 80.

Type
Original Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Psychogeriatric Association

Purpose in life is the feeling that one’s life is goal-oriented and has direction (Ryff, Reference Ryff1989). It is a component of eudaimonic well-being that has been associated with better health outcomes in older adulthood (Musich et al., Reference Musich, Wang, Kraemer, Hawkins and Wicker2018). Among these outcomes, purpose is associated consistently with better cognitive health (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti and Terracciano2021c). Individuals who feel more purposeful tend to perform better on cognitive tasks (Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira, Zeng, Marquine, Lipton, González, Daviglus, Lamar and Zuelsdorff2021), report fewer cognitive failures (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Aschwanden, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023a), are less likely to develop pre-dementia syndromes (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2021b), and, ultimately, are less likely to develop dementia (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Buchman, Barnes and Bennett2010). That is, among individuals who are cognitively healthy at baseline, purpose in life is associated with lower risk of incident dementia over time, an association that has replicated in at least eight samples and is independent of clinical (e.g., diabetes) and behavioral (e.g., physical inactivity) risk factors (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Stephan, Sesker and Terracciano2023b).

Previous research on purpose in life and risk of dementia has used samples that were, on average, in their 60s at the assessment of purpose in life and had, on average, 5.94 years of follow-up (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Stephan, Sesker and Terracciano2023b). Thus, less is known about whether purpose in life measured in middle adulthood is associated with dementia at older ages. Purpose tends to be associated with better cognitive health across adulthood, including better performance on cognitive tasks, regardless of age (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan, Strickhouser and Terracciano2022b). In addition, the association between purpose and dementia risk is not moderated by age, which suggests that this association is similar across relatively younger (including middle-aged adults) and relatively older adults (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Stephan, Sesker and Terracciano2023b).

There is some evidence, however, that purpose during middle adulthood may not be associated prospectively with global cognitive function during the transition to older adulthood: purpose in life measured at age 52 was unrelated to cognitive performance at age 69 in a longitudinal sample of women (Nakanishi et al., Reference Nakanishi, Yamasaki, Nishida, Richards and Ernstsen2019). If replicated, and extended to dementia, this pattern would suggest that midlife purpose in life is associated concurrently with better cognitive function but that it may not help to maintain cognition into older adulthood. The present research addresses this prospective association with dementia, as well as global cognition and other cognitive domains that have been previously associated with purpose: verbal fluency, working memory, and numeric reasoning (Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira, Zeng, Marquine, Lipton, González, Daviglus, Lamar and Zuelsdorff2021; Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023d; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Guimond, Kubzansky, Zhang, Bennett, Barnes, Grodstein and Lincoln2022).

The association between purpose in life and dementia has been examined primarily in the direction of purpose as a prospective predictor of cognitive decline (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Shin, Scicolone and Parmelee2019b) and dementia risk (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Stephan, Sesker and Terracciano2023b). The opposite direction, however, is also possible: purpose in life may change with the onset of cognitive deficits. And, indeed, recent evidence suggests that purpose in life has steeper declines in individuals who develop cognitive impairment, particularly after the onset of cognitive deficits (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023c). The present research extends this literature by testing whether dementia status in older adulthood is associated with the trajectory of purpose in life across middle-to-older adulthood.

The present research uses nearly 30 years of data from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (Herd et al., Reference Herd, Carr and Roan2014) to address key questions about the relation between purpose in life, dementia, and cognitive function between middle and older adulthood. First, with three assessments of purpose when participants were on average in their early 50s, mid-60s, and early 70s, we examine the prospective association between purpose measured at each time point and likelihood of dementia assessed 8–28 years later, when participants were on average about 80 years old. Second, we examine the prospective association between purpose in life and several cognitive tasks to evaluate purpose as a prospective correlate of cognitive function, as well as surpassing the threshold for dementia. Third, we test whether these associations are moderated by age, sex, or education to evaluate generalizability across these sociodemographic groups. Finally, we model the trajectory of purpose in life over nearly 20 years to examine whether the intercept and slope of purpose differ between people without and with dementia at age 80.

Method

Participants and procedure

Participants were from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study (WLS), an ongoing study of graduates from Wisconsin high schools in 1957 and a selected sibling (Herd et al., Reference Herd, Carr and Roan2014). Graduates and siblings have been followed periodically since high school, with the most recent assessment in 2020. Participants (graduates and siblings) reported on their purpose in life at Round 4 (1992–1994), Round 5 (2003–2007), and Round 6 (2010–2012) and were administered a cognitive battery at Round 7 (2020). All participants with relevant cognitive data from Round 7 (N = 4,632) were included in the analyses. Multiple imputation was used to impute missing data on purpose in life at each round before analysis (n = 519 values imputed at Round 4, n = 403 values imputed at Round 5, and n = 562 values imputed at Round 6; results based on unimputed data at each round are in supplemental material). Some analyses limited the analytic sample to participants who were the focal graduates and had data on purpose in life at all three rounds (n = 2393) to facilitate comparisons across ages.

Measures

Purpose in life

Items from Ryff’s Purpose in Life scale (Ryff, Reference Ryff1989) were used to measure purpose at Rounds 4, 5, and 6. The exact items varied somewhat across rounds, but four items were the same at every round (I am an active person carrying out the plans I set for myself; I do not have a good sense of what it is I'm trying to accomplish in life; I sometimes feel as if I've done all there is to do in life; I used to set goals for myself, but now it seems like a waste of time). Participants rated these items from 1 (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree). Items were reverse scored when necessary, and the mean was computed across the four items at each round (alpha = 0.67 at each round). Higher scores indicated greater purpose.

Dementia

Interviewers administered the Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS; Crimmins et al., Reference Crimmins, Kim, Langa and Weir2011) at Round 7. The TICS included tasks that measured orientation, attention, working memory, conceptual reasoning, language skills, and episodic memory that were summed into a total score (possible range 0–50). Dementia can be classified based on three specific tasks from the TICS: total recall, serial 7s, and backward counting (possible range 0–27). A score of 6 or less across these tasks has been validated as a threshold for dementia (Crimmins et al., Reference Crimmins, Kim, Langa and Weir2011; Langa et al., Reference Langa, Larson, Crimmins, Faul, Levine, Kabeto and Weir2017). Dementia was coded into no (>6 = 0) and yes (≤6 = 1).

Cognitive function

In addition to the total TICS score, participants also completed verbal fluency, digit ordering, and numeric reasoning tasks at Round 7. Verbal fluency was measured by asking participants to list as many words that start with the letter F or L as they could in 60 s. For digit ordering, participants were read a list of numbers and had to repeat the numbers back in order from lowest to highest. The list started with three numbers and increased one number at a time up to eight numbers. The task ended after two failed attempts of the same list length. Numeric reasoning was measured with a number series task. Participants had to fill in one missing number in a series of numbers (e.g., 17 __ 12 8 [answer = 15]). Participants completed the first block and then the difficulty of the second block varied by how well the participant did on the first block. WLS applied Rasch scoring to the number series task to account for planned differences in the difficulty of the second block across participants.

Covariates

Sociodemographic factors were age in years concurrent with each purpose assessment, sex (0 = male, 1 = female), and education in years. A variable that distinguished graduates from siblings (0 = graduates, 1 = sibling) was also included in the analyses. Because of differences in performance between the letters F and L, a dummy-coded variable that indicated task letter (0 = F, 1 = L) was included as an additional covariate for analyses of verbal fluency.

Analytic strategy

Logistic regression was used to examine the association between purpose in life and likelihood of dementia at Round 7, controlling for the sociodemographic factors. Three logistic regressions were tested, one for each round that had a measurement of purpose in life (i.e., 4, 5, and 6). We then did a sensitivity analysis that limited participants to graduates who reported on their purpose at all three rounds to evaluate the estimates on the same subsample and make comparisons across waves based on the same participants who were about the same age within each round. Linear regression was used to examine the association between purpose in life and performance on the four cognitive tasks (TICS, verbal fluency, digit ordering, numeric reasoning), controlling for sociodemographic factors and letter for verbal fluency. We then did the same sensitivity analysis as for dementia. Moderation by age, sex, and education was tested with an interaction between purpose and each of these factors in the full sample. For the interpretation of moderation, we focused on interactions that replicated across at least two rounds. Finally, multilevel modeling was used to model change in purpose across Rounds 4–6 and to correlate the intercept and slope of purpose with dementia at Round 7. Thus, the multilevel analyses examined whether the trajectory of purpose in life across Rounds 4–6 varied by dementia status at Round 7. Maximum likelihood estimation was used, and all participants were included in the analysis because multilevel modeling can handle missing data for participants who may have only reported on their purpose at one or two rounds. Age, sex, education, and sample were included as covariates. We first tested an intercept-only model and then added the slope.

Results

Descriptive statistics are in Table 1. Correlations among study variables are in Supplemental Table S1. Across the full sample, 411 participants (8.9%) had a TICSm score in the dementia range at Round 7. Results of the logistic regressions are in Table 2 (for comparison, results of the logistic regressions for the full sample based on unimputed data are in Supplemental Table S2). Purpose in life measured in Round 4, at about age 52, was not associated significantly with likelihood of dementia measured 26–28 years later, at about age 80. Purpose in life was assessed at Round 5, at about age 63, and Round 6, at about age 70, however, was associated with greater likelihood of dementia at Round 7, at about age 80: For every standard deviation higher score in purpose at either Round 5 or Round 6, there was an approximately 20% reduced likelihood of dementia measured 13–17 or 8–10 years later.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for all study variables in the total sample and by dementia status at wave 7 in the full and restricted samples

Note. Descriptive statistics are either mean (standard deviation) or percentage (n). The full sample includes all participants who contributed data to the analyses. The restricted sample is limited to participants who reported on their purpose in life at all three rounds and who were in the graduate sample.

Table 2. Association between purpose in life at Round 4, Round 5, and likelihood of dementia at Round 7

Note. Full Sample: N = 4632, n = 411 with dementia. Restricted Sample N = 2393, n = 202 with dementia. OR = odds ratio. CI = Confidence Interval.

a The restricted sample is limited to participants who reported on their purpose in life at all three rounds and who were in the graduate sample (age was not included in this model because participants were all about the same age at each wave in the restricted sample).

The results of the sensitivity analyses that limited the sample to participants who had data on purpose at all three rounds and who were the focal graduates are also in Table 2. The pattern was the same as the full sample: Purpose at Round 4 was unrelated to dementia, whereas purpose at both Round 5 and Round 6 was associated with reduced likelihood of dementia at Round 7. The association was slightly stronger for purpose at Round 6 than at Round 5, but the confidence intervals overlapped more than 50%, and thus the estimates were not statistically different from each other.

The associations between purpose at each round and the four cognitive function tasks are in Table 3 (for comparison, results of the linear regressions for the full sample based on unimputed data are in Supplemental Table S3). A different pattern emerged than for dementia. Specifically, purpose in life measured at Round 4, Round 5, and Round 6 was consistently associated with better performance on the full TICS and the verbal fluency task at Round 7; this pattern was similar when the sample was restricted to participants in the graduate sample who reported on their purpose at each of the three rounds. The only exception was the association between purpose at Round 4 and the full TICS was not significant. Purpose was not associated consistently with digit ordering or numeric reasoning across the full and restricted samples.

Table 3. Association between purpose in life at Round 4, Round 5, and Round 6 and cognitive function at Round 7

Note. Full Sample N = 4632. Restricted Sample N = 2,393. Beta coefficients are standardized beta coefficients from linear regression controlling for Age (full sample only), sex, education, and sample (full sample only). Verbal fluency also controls for letter used in the task (F, L). TICS = Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. aThe restricted sample is limited to participants who reported on their purpose in life at all three rounds and who were in the graduate sample (age was not included in this model because participants were all about the same age at each wave in the restricted sample).

There was little evidence that the associations were moderated by sociodemographic factors (Supplemental Table S4 and Table S5). The one significant interaction between purpose and age on likelihood of dementia at Round 5 was not significant at Rounds 4 or 6. None of the other interaction terms was significant. Age, sex, and education likewise did not consistently moderate the association between purpose and the four cognitive tasks, with two exceptions. First, the association between purpose and the full continuous TICS score was stronger among females than males. This interaction was significant at all three rounds. Second, the association between purpose and numeric reasoning was moderated by education, such that purpose was associated with better numeric reasoning among participants with relatively less education, whereas it was unrelated among participants with relatively higher education. This interaction was significant with purpose measured at Round 4 and Round 5 but not at Round 6.

Finally, the associations between the intercept and slope of purpose between Round 4 to Round 6 and dementia at Round 7 from the multilevel models are in Table 4. Consistent with the logistic regressions, the intercept of purpose was associated negatively with dementia, which indicated that higher average purpose was associated with lower likelihood of having dementia. The addition of a time × dementia interaction suggested that decreases in purpose between Rounds 4 and 6 were also associated with dementia at Round 7, with slightly steeper declines in individuals with dementia at Round 7. This model, however, was only a slightly better fit than the intercept-only model (i.e., Δ in AIC > 2).

Table 4. Multilevel models of change in purpose between Rounds 4 and 6 and dementia at Round 7

Note. N = 4,632. Model 1 is the intercept-only model. Model 2 is Model 1 with further inclusion of the slope. Bs are standardized coefficients from the multilevel model analysis.

Discussion

The present research used nearly 30 years of longitudinal data to examine the prospective associations between purpose in life and cognitive function and dementia. With the longest follow-up to date, purpose in life measured on average when participants were in their early 50s was unrelated to likelihood of dementia about 28 years later. In contrast, purpose in life measured when participants were either in their mid-60s or early 70s was associated with lower likelihood of dementia between eight and 17 years later. Purpose measured at all three rounds was associated with better global cognitive function and verbal fluency, whereas it was largely unrelated to digit ordering and numeric reasoning. The effect sizes for both dementia and the cognitive function measures were modest but consistent with the broader literature on purpose and cognitive health (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023d). There was only modest evidence for moderation by sociodemographic factors. Finally, the multilevel model suggested that individuals with lower levels and steeper declines in purpose over about 20 years were more likely to have dementia in their 80s.

Previous research on purpose in life and dementia risk has relied on samples that were, on average, about 69 years old and that had on average nearly six years of follow-up (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Stephan, Sesker and Terracciano2023b). The present research suggests that age might matter: The average age of 52 at Round 4 was about 15 years younger than the average age across the eight samples in the previous meta-analysis. This null association is consistent with one previous study of women that found purpose measured in the early 50s was unrelated to cognitive function about 17 years later (Nakanishi et al., Reference Nakanishi, Yamasaki, Nishida, Richards and Ernstsen2019).

One potential explanation for this pattern of findings is reverse causality. That is, the neurodegeneration prior to dementia onset could lead to lower purpose; thus, the observed association at older ages could be due to disease processes that reduce purpose rather than purpose acting as a protective factor against the development of dementia. While some findings in the WLS are consistent with reverse causality, other evidence is not. For example, if reverse causality accounts for the relation between purpose and lower risk of dementia, the association between purpose and dementia should get stronger the shorter the interval between the measurement of purpose and dementia. In the present sample, however, the association between purpose at Round 6 and dementia was not statistically stronger than the association between purpose at Round 5 and dementia, which is what to be expected if the association was due solely to reverse causality. A more parsimonious interpretation of the weaker and null association between the age 52 purpose and age 80 dementia is that associations generally weaken over time, as changes in life circumstances, events, and idiosyncratic processes are more likely to occur the longer the follow-up time.

Purpose measured earlier in middle adulthood may be too distal of a predictor of dementia to be detected. Purpose in middle adulthood, however, may set in motion a cascade that helps protect cognitive health in older adulthood. Individuals with more purpose, for example, tend to have fewer clinical risk factors for poor cognitive health: They are less likely to have cardiovascular disease (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Delaney and Kubansky2019a), diabetes (Hafez et al., Reference Hafez, Heisler, Choi, Ankuda, Winkelman and Kullgren2018), inflammation (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Stephan, Luchetti and Terracciano2023f), obesity (Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Feldman, Tsai, Harpaz-Rotem, McCutcheon, Schulenberg and Pietrzak2023a), or depression (Boreham and Schutte, Reference Boreham and Schutte2023) than individuals with less purpose. Purpose is likewise associated with healthier lifestyles, including greater engagement in physical activity (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Stephan, Kekäläinen, Luchetti and Terracciano2023e) and less substance use (Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Tsai, Harpaz-Rotem, McCutcheon, Schulenberg and Pietrzak2023b). These clinical and behavioral profiles help support healthier cognitive aging (Norton et al., Reference Norton, Matthews, Barnes, Yaffe and Brayne2014). Purpose in life is also associated with greater engagement (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023d) and social integration (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Aschwanden, Lee, Sesker, Stephan and Terracciano2022a) that likewise tend to support cognitive health (Fancourt et al., Reference Fancourt, Steptoe and Cadar2020; Luchetti et al., Reference Luchetti, Terracciano, Aschwanden, Lee, Stephan and Sutin2020). It is possible that these processes are more relevant as adults transition from middle-to-older adulthood than earlier in middle adulthood.

It is of note that purpose in life at each round was prospectively associated with better global cognitive function and better verbal fluency. These findings contrast with Nakanishi et al. (Reference Nakanishi, Yamasaki, Nishida, Richards and Ernstsen2019) who did not find a significant prospective association between purpose and global cognitive function over a 17-year follow-up. The present analysis does, however, replicate previous research that found a positive association between purpose and better global cognitive function (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Shin, Scicolone and Parmelee2019b) and higher verbal fluency (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan, Strickhouser and Terracciano2022b). This pattern suggests that purpose, even measured in the early 50s, does support better cognitive health nearly 30 years later.

Although purpose tends to be associated with better cognitive function (Estrella et al., Reference Estrella, Durazo-Arvizu, Gallo, Tarraf, Isasi, Perreira, Zeng, Marquine, Lipton, González, Daviglus, Lamar and Zuelsdorff2021; Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023d), not all studies find it is associated with performance in specific domains (Cattaneo et al., Reference Cattaneo, Solana-Sánchez, Abellaneda-Pérez, Portellano-Ortiz, Delgado-Gallén, Alviarez Schulze, Pachón-García, Zetterberg, Tormos, Pascual-Leone and Bartrés-Faz2022; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne, Watson, Seehra and Martinez2018). And, indeed, the current analysis failed to find a consistent association between purpose and either digit ordering or numeric reasoning. The latter null association was somewhat surprising, given that some previous work had found a positive association between purpose and numeric reasoning (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023d). There is a need to test the association between purpose and multiple domains of cognitive function to identify whether differences across studies are a failure to replicate or whether there are meaningful differences in the association between purpose and specific cognitive domains.

There was little evidence that sociodemographic factors moderated the association between purpose and either dementia or cognitive function. Previous research finds that the association between purpose and cognitive health is generally similar across sociodemographic factors (Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Turiano, Payne and Hill2017; Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Aschwanden, Luchetti, Stephan, Terracciano and Rouch2021a; Wilson et al., 2021). Surprisingly, the association between purpose and global cognitive function was stronger among females than males, an association apparent at each measurement of purpose. Given that previous research on purpose and cognitive function has not found such a sex difference, this moderation should be considered with caution until replicated. There is some evidence that purpose has a stronger association with cognitive function in less economically robust environments (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan, Strickhouser and Terracciano2022b). The present research found some support that purpose may be more helpful for numeric reasoning among individuals with relatively less education. Again, this moderation should be interpreted with caution until replicated, particularly because the interaction was specific to reasoning and did not extend to the other cognitive tasks.

There was some evidence from the multilevel models that change in purpose over about 20 years was associated with dementia. This evidence could be interpreted as supporting the reverse causality hypothesis, such that those who develop dementia may have neurodegeneration that leads to loss of purpose that is apparent before cognitive deficits. Across this time period, however, there are many life events that could decrease purpose that are related to dementia and possibly unrelated to neurodegeneration. Retirement, for example, is a significant life event associated with subsequent declines in purpose (Lewis and Hill, Reference Lewis and Hill2020). Widowhood, which the likelihood increases with age, is also associated with lower purpose (Koren and Lowenstein, Reference Koren and Lowenstein2008). Such events may precipitate both declines in purpose and hasten the development of dementia. Recent research also suggests that purpose in life decreases more following the onset of cognitive impairment than before impairment (Sutin et al., Reference Sutin, Luchetti, Stephan and Terracciano2023c). The present research, however, did not have data on the timing of cognitive impairment; more research is needed to better evaluate the timing of changes in purpose and dementia.

The present research builds on recently published literature in International Psychogeriatrics on purpose in life and neuropsychiatric conditions. For example, Fischer et al. (Reference Fischer, Feldman, Tsai, Harpaz-Rotem, Lucas, Schulenberg and Pietrzak2023c) reported on the cross-sectional association between purpose in life and aspects of social and psychological functioning in veterans. The present research extends the positive associations to aspects of cognitive functioning measured years later. In addition, apathy is common in dementia (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Henley and Suárez-González2023), and the decline in purpose associated with dementia found in the present research may contribute to the development of apathy after disease onset. Purpose may also be related to apathy in preclinical stages, which further increases risk of incident dementia (Connors et al., Reference Connors, Teixeira-Pinto, Ames, Woodward and Brodaty2023).

The present study has several strengths, including three assessments of purpose in life and nearly 30 years of follow-up. There are also limitations that need to be considered. Dementia, for example, was only measured once and not concurrently with the purpose assessments. However, it is unlikely that participants had dementia in their 50s or 60s. If they had dementia 20–30 years earlier, it is likely that they would have progressed too far to be able to participate in the Round 7 assessment. Relatedly, with one assessment of dementia, it was not possible to determine the timing of its development, particularly regarding change in purpose over time. In addition, mortality in the WLS likely underestimated the association between purpose and cognitive function because participants with lower purpose may have been more likely to die younger and thus were not included in the Round 7 cognitive assessment. Finally, the WLS sample is white and from one state within the United States. As such, the associations may not generalize to other populations. The findings should thus be interpreted in the context of these limitations.

Despite these limitations, the overall findings are consistent with the broader literature on purpose in life and cognitive health and add new information on the prospective association between purpose and dementia. Future research would benefit from more frequent assessments of both purpose in life and cognitive function and dementia to better pinpoint when purpose is most protective and when purpose could be leveraged for interventions to improve cognitive outcomes in older adulthood.

Conflict of interest

None.

Source of funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01AG074573. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Description of author(s)’ roles

A. Sutin designed the study, obtained, and analyzed the data and wrote the paper. M. Luchetti, Y. Stephan, and A. Terracciano provided critical feedback throughout the project and assisted with writing the paper.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the use of WLS. Since 1991, the WLS has been supported principally by the National Institute on Aging (AG-9775, AG-21079, AG-033285, and AG-041868), with additional support from the Vilas Estate Trust, the National Science Foundation, the Spencer Foundation, and the Graduate School of the University of Wisconsin-Madison. Since 1992, data have been collected by the University of Wisconsin Survey Center.

Data availability

The data used in this research is available to the public from the WLS website (https://researchers.wls.wisc.edu/).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610224000383.

References

Boreham, I. D., & Schutte, N. S. (2023). The relationship between purpose in life and depression and anxiety: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 79(12), 27362767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P. A., Buchman, A. S., Barnes, L. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2010). Effect of a purpose in life on risk of incident Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older persons. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(3), 304310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattaneo, G., Solana-Sánchez, J., Abellaneda-Pérez, K., Portellano-Ortiz, C., Delgado-Gallén, S., Alviarez Schulze, V., Pachón-García, C., Zetterberg, H., Tormos, J. M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Bartrés-Faz, D. (2022). Sense of coherence mediates the relationship between cognitive reserve and cognition in middle-aged adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 835415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, J. D., Henley, S. M. D., & Suárez-González, A. (2023). A systematic review of the prevalence of depression, anxiety, and apathy in frontotemporal dementia, atypical and young-onset Alzheimer’s disease, and inherited dementia. International Psychogeriatrics, 35(9), 457476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connors, M. H., Teixeira-Pinto, A., Ames, D., Woodward, M., & Brodaty, H. (2023). Apathy and depression in mild cognitive impairment: Distinct longitudinal trajectories and clinical outcomes. International Psychogeriatrics, 35(11), 633642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crimmins, E. M., Kim, J. K., Langa, K. M., & Weir, D. R. (2011). Assessment of cognition using surveys and neuropsychological assessment: The health and retirement study and the aging, demographics, and memory study. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 66(Suppl 1), 162171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estrella, M. L., Durazo-Arvizu, R. A., Gallo, L. C., Tarraf, W., Isasi, C. R., Perreira, K. M., Zeng, D., Marquine, M. J., Lipton, R. B., González, H. M., Daviglus, M. L., Lamar, M., & Zuelsdorff, M. (2021). Psychosocial factors associated with cognitive function among middle-aged and older hispanics/Latinos: The hispanic community health study/Study of latinos and its sociocultural ancillary study. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 79(1), 433449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fancourt, D., Steptoe, A., & Cadar, D. (2020). Community engagement and dementia risk: Time-to-event analyses from a national cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 74(1), 7177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, I. C., Feldman, D. B., Tsai, J., Harpaz-Rotem, I., McCutcheon, V. E., Schulenberg, S. E., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2023a). Purpose in life and physical health in older U.S. military veterans: Cross-sectional results from the national health and resilience in veterans study. Preventive Medicine, 170, 107495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, I. C., Tsai, J., Harpaz-Rotem, I., McCutcheon, V. E., Schulenberg, S. E., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2023b). Perceived purpose in life, mental health, and suicidality in older U.S. military veterans: Results from the national health and resilience in veterans study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(2), 8793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, I. C., Feldman, D. B., Tsai, J., Harpaz-Rotem, I., Lucas, K. A., Schulenberg, S. E., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2023c). Identifying significant correlates of purpose in life in older US military veterans: Results from the national health and resilience in veterans study. International Psychogeriatrics, 35(10), 560565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafez, D., Heisler, M., Choi, H., Ankuda, C. K., Winkelman, T., & Kullgren, J. T. (2018). Association between purpose in life and glucose control among older adults. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 52(4), 309318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herd, P., Carr, D., & Roan, C. (2014). Cohort profile: Wisconsin longitudinal study (WLS). International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(1), 3441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, E. S., Delaney, S. W., & Kubansky, L. D. (2019a). Sense of purpose in life and cardiovascular disease: Underlying mechanisms and future directions. Current Cardiology Reports, 21(11), 135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, G., Shin, S. H., Scicolone, M. A., & Parmelee, P. (2019b). Purpose in life protects against cognitive decline among older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(6), 593601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koren, C., & Lowenstein, A. (2008). Late-life widowhood and meaning in life. Ageing International, 32(2), 140155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langa, K. M., Larson, E. B., Crimmins, E. M., Faul, J. D., Levine, D. A., Kabeto, M. U., & Weir, D. R. (2017). A comparison of the prevalence of dementia in the United States in 2000 and 2012. JAMA Internal Medicine, 177(1), 5158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, N. A., & Hill, P. L. (2020). Does being active mean being purposeful in older adulthood? Examining the moderating role of retirement. Psychology and Aging, 35(7), 10501057.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, N. A., Turiano, N. A., Payne, B. R., & Hill, P. L. (2017). Purpose in life and cognitive functioning in adulthood. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24(6), 662671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luchetti, M., Terracciano, A., Aschwanden, D., Lee, J. H., Stephan, Y., & Sutin, A. R. (2020). Loneliness is associated with risk of cognitive impairment in the survey of health, ageing and retirement in Europe. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 35(7), 794801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musich, S., Wang, S. S., Kraemer, S., Hawkins, K., & Wicker, E. (2018). Purpose in life and positive health outcomes among older adults. Population Health Management, 21(2), 139147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakanishi, M., Yamasaki, S., Nishida, A., Richards, M., & Ernstsen, L. (2019). Midlife psychological well-being and its impact on cognitive functioning later in life: An observational study using a female British birth cohort. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 72(3), 835843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, S., Matthews, F. E., Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K., & Brayne, C. (2014). Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: An analysis of population-based data. The Lancet Neurology, 13(8), 788794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 10691081.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutin, A. R., Aschwanden, D., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., Terracciano, A., & Rouch, I. (2021a). Sense of purpose in life is associated with lower risk of incident dementia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 83(1), 249258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Aschwanden, D., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2023a). Sense of purpose in life and subjective cognitive failures. Personality and Individual Differences, 200, 111874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Aschwanden, D., Lee, J. H., Sesker, A. A., Stephan, Y., Terracciano, A. (2022a). Sense of purpose in life and concurrent loneliness and risk of incident loneliness: An individual-participant meta-analysis of 135,227 individuals from 36 samples. Journal of Affective Disorders, 309, 211220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Aschwanden, D., Stephan, Y., Sesker, A. A., & Terracciano, A. (2023b). Sense of meaning and purpose in life and risk of incident dementia: New data and meta-analysis. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 105, 104847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., Strickhouser, J. E., & Terracciano, A. (2022b). The association between purpose/meaning in life and verbal fluency and episodic memory: A meta-analysis of >140,000 participants from up to 32 countries. International Psychogeriatrics, 34(3), 263273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2021b). Purpose in life and motoric cognitive risk syndrome: Replicable evidence from two national samples. Journal of The American Geriatrics Society, 69(2), 381388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2023c). Change in purpose in life before and after onset of cognitive impairment. JAMA Network Open, 6(9), e2333489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., Stephan, Y., & Terracciano, A. (2023d). Purpose in life and cognitive performance and informant ratings of cognitive decline, affect, and activities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 19.Google ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Luchetti, M., & Terracciano, A. (2021c). Sense of purpose in life and healthier cognitive aging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(11), P917P919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Kekäläinen, T., Luchetti, M., & Terracciano, A. (2023e). Purpose in life and accelerometer-measured physical activity among older adults. Psychology Health, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y., Luchetti, M., & Terracciano, A. (2023f). Purpose in life and markers of immunity and inflammation: Testing pathways of episodic memory. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 174, 111487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, M., Guimond, A.-J.ée, Kubzansky, L. D., Zhang, Y., Bennett, D. A., Barnes, L. L., Grodstein, F., & Lincoln, K. D. (2022). Negative and positive psychosocial factors in relation to cognitive health in older African Americans. Innovation in Aging, 6(3), igac019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S., Capuano, A. W., Sampaio, C., Leurgans, S. E., Barnes, L. L., Boyle, P. A., Farfel, J. M., & Bennett, D. A. (2022). Relationship of purpose in life to dementia in older black and white Brazilians. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 28(10), 9971002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L. B., Watson, C. W., Seehra, S., & Martinez, M. N. (2018). Positive psychosocial factors and cognition in ethnically diverse older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 24(3), 294304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for all study variables in the total sample and by dementia status at wave 7 in the full and restricted samples

Figure 1

Table 2. Association between purpose in life at Round 4, Round 5, and likelihood of dementia at Round 7

Figure 2

Table 3. Association between purpose in life at Round 4, Round 5, and Round 6 and cognitive function at Round 7

Figure 3

Table 4. Multilevel models of change in purpose between Rounds 4 and 6 and dementia at Round 7

Supplementary material: File

Sutin et al. supplementary material

Sutin et al. supplementary material
Download Sutin et al. supplementary material(File)
File 22.8 KB