Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:49:18.147Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pitfall Intention Explanation Task with Clue Questions (Pitfall task): assessment of comprehending other people's behavioral intentions in Alzheimer's disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 July 2012

Tomoharu Yamaguchi*
Affiliation:
Gunma University Graduate School of Health Sciences, Gunma, Japan Department of Rehabilitation, Gunma University of Health and Welfare, Gunma, Japan
Yohko Maki
Affiliation:
Gunma University Graduate School of Health Sciences, Gunma, Japan Geriatrics Research Institute and Hospital, Gunma, Japan
Haruyasu Yamaguchi
Affiliation:
Gunma University Graduate School of Health Sciences, Gunma, Japan
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Tomoharu Yamaguchi, 3-39-15 Showa-machi, Maebashi, Gunma, 371-8514, Japan. Phone: +81-27-220-8946; Fax: +81-27-220-8946. Email: [email protected].
Get access

Abstract

Background: In Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, deficits in contextual understanding and intentions/beliefs of other people (theory of mind; ToM) cause communication problems between patients and caregivers. To evaluate deficits of contextual understanding/ToM, we developed the Pitfall Intention Explanation Task with Clue Questions (Pitfall task).

Methods: We recruited 26 healthy older adults in clinical dementia rating (CDR) 0, and 62 outpatients: 12 with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) in CDR 0.5; 36 mild AD in CDR 1; and 14 moderate AD in CDR 2. The Pitfall task consists of a single-frame cartoon that shows a character's intention and seven serial questions that provide clues for contextual understanding/ToM.

Results: The total score (0–7) was decreased with progression of AD (CDR 0, 5.4 ± 2.6; CDR 0.5, 3.7 ± 2.7; CDR 1, 1.9 ± 3.1; CDR 2, 0.0 ± 0.0; respectively). In CDR 0, two-third of the participants responded correctly without clue questions. In CDR 0.5, one-third of the participants responded correctly without clue questions, and half of them understood with the help of the clue questions. In CDR 1, one-fourth of the participants responded correctly without clue questions, and the clue questions did not increase the correct response. In CDR 2, none responded correctly. Additionally, the Pitfall task provided the chance for patients’ families to observe patients’ responses.

Conclusion: Contextual understanding/ToM, a kind of social cognition, was impaired with progression of AD. The Pitfall task evaluates the function quickly with low burden for memory function, and may provide helpful clues for caregivers to achieve good communication with AD patients.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.Google Scholar
Bschor, T., Kühl, K. P. and Reischies, F. M. (2001). Spontaneous speech of patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type and mild cognitive impairment. International Psychogeriatrics, 13, 289298.Google Scholar
Carlomagno, S., Santoro, A., Menditti, A., Pandolfi, M. and Marini, A. (2005). Referential communication in Alzheimer's type dementia. Cortex, 41, 520534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuetos, F., Arango-Lasprilla, J. C., Uribe, C., Valencia, C. and Lopera, F. (2007). Linguistic changes in verbal expression: a preclinical marker of Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 433439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lira, J. O., Ortiz, K. Z., Campanha, A. C., Bertolucci, P. H. and Minett, T. S. (2011). Microlinguistic aspects of the oral narrative in patients with Alzheimer's disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 23, 404412.Google Scholar
Dubois, B. et al. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurology, 6, 734746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A. and Black, S. E. (2009). False–belief understanding in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 489497.Google Scholar
Folstein, M., Folstein, S. and McHugh, P. (1975). “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.Google Scholar
Forbes-McKay, K. E. and Venneri, A. (2005). Detecting subtle spontaneous language decline in early Alzheimer's disease with a picture description task. Neurological Sciences, 26, 243254.Google Scholar
Gitlin, L. N., Winter, L., Dennis, M. P. and Hauck, W. W. (2007). A non-pharmacological intervention to manage behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia and reduce caregiver distress: design and methods of project ACT3. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 2, 695703.Google Scholar
Goodglass, H. and Kaplan, E. (1983). The Assessment of Aphasia and Related Disorders. Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.Google Scholar
Gregory, C. et al. (2002). Theory of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease: theoretical and practical implications. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 125, 752764.Google Scholar
Grundman, M. et al. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment can be distinguished from Alzheimer disease and normal aging for clinical trials. Archives of Neurology, 61, 5966.Google Scholar
Huberle, E. and Karnath, H. O. (2010). Saliency modulates global perception in simultanagnosia. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 595603.Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R., Greenwood, P. M. and Alexander, G. E. (2000). Alzheimer disease constricts the dynamic range of spatial attention in visual search. Neuropsychologia, 38, 11261135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C. (2007). Mild cognitive impairment: current research and clinical implications. Seminars in Neurology, 27, 2231.Google Scholar
Potkins, D. et al. (2003). Language impairment in dementia: impact on symptoms and care needs in residential homes. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 10021006.Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., Kluger, A., Franssen, E., Wegiel, J. and den Leon, M. J. (2008). Mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a historical perspective. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 1831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ripich, D. N., Ziol, E. and Lee, M. M. (1998). Longitudinal effects of communication training on caregivers of persons with Alzheimer's disease. Clinical Gerontologist, 19, 3755.Google Scholar
Rösler, A., Mapstone, M., Hays-Wicklund, A., Gitelman, D. R. and Weintraub, S. (2005). The “zoom lens” of focal attention in visual search: changes in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 41, 512519.Google Scholar
Savundranayagam, M. Y., Hummert, M. L. and Montgomery, R. J. (2005). Investigating the effects of communication problems on caregiver burden. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, S48S55.Google Scholar
Vasse, E., Vernooij-Dassen, M., Spijker, A., Rikkert, M. O. and Koopmans, R. (2010). A systematic review of communication strategies for people with dementia in residential and nursing homes. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 189200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamaguchi, H., Maki, Y. and Yamaguchi, T. (2011). A figurative proverb test for dementia: rapid detection of disinhibition, excuse and confabulation, causing discommunication. Psychogeriatrics, 11, 205211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaitchik, D., Koff, E., Brownell, H., Winner, E. and Albert, M. (2004). Inference of mental states in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 9, 301313.Google Scholar