Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:23:13.448Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive stimulation therapy (CST): neuropsychological mechanisms of change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 November 2012

Louise Hall
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Martin Orrell
Affiliation:
Research Department of Mental Health Sciences, University College London, 67–73 Riding House Street, London, W1W 7EJ, UK
Joshua Stott
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK
Aimee Spector*
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Aimee Spector, PhD, DClinPsy, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. Phone: +44 (0)20 7679 1844. Email: [email protected].
Get access

Abstract

Background: Cognitive stimulation therapy (CST) is an evidence-based psychosocial intervention for people with dementia consisting of 14 group sessions aiming to stimulate various areas of cognition. This study examined the effects of CST on specific cognitive domains and explored the neuropsychological processes underpinning any effects.

Methods: A total of 34 participants with mild to moderate dementia were included. A one-group pretest–posttest design was used. Participants completed a battery of neuropsychological tests in the week before and after the manualised seven-week CST programme.

Results: There were significant improvement pre- to post-CST group on measures of delayed verbal recall (WMS III logical memory subtest – delayed), visual memory (WMS III visual reproduction subtest – delayed), orientation (WMS III information and orientation subscale), and auditory comprehension (Token Test). There were no significant changes on measures of naming (Boston Naming Test-2), attention (Trail Making Test A/Digit Span), executive function (DKEFS verbal fluency/Trail Making Test B), praxis (WMS III visual reproduction – immediate) or on a general cognitive screen (MMSE).

Conclusions: Memory, comprehension of syntax, and orientation appear to be the cognitive domains most impacted by CST. One hypothesis is that the language-based nature of CST enhances neural pathways responsible for processing of syntax, possibly also aiding verbal recall. Another is that the reduction in negative self-stereotypes due to the de-stigmatising effect of CST may impact on language and memory, domains that are the primary focus of CST. Further research is required to substantiate these hypotheses.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Breuil, V., De Rotrou, J., Forette, F., Tortrar, D., Ganancia-Ganem, A. and Frambourt, A. (1994). Cognitive stimulation of patients with dementia: preliminary results. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 9, 211–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clare, L. (2008). Neuropsychological assessment of the older person. In Woods, R. and Clare, L. (eds.), Handbook of the Clinical Psychology of Ageing. UK: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Clare, L. and Woods, R. T. (2004). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer's disease: a review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clare, L., Wilson, B. A., Carter, G., Breen, K., Gosses, A. and Hodges, J. R. (2000). Intervening with everyday memory problems in dementia of Alzheimer type: an errorless learning approach. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 132146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, D. B.et al. (2001) Effects of practice on category fluency in Alzheimer's disease. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 125128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E. and Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test, 2nd edn.San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E. and Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G. and Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E. and Baresi, B. (2001). Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, 3rd edn.Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T. and Statham, J. A. (2004). Explicit and implicit stereotype activation effects on memory: do age and awareness moderate the impact of priming? Psychology and Aging, 19, 495505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, J. R. (2007). Cognitive Assessment for Clinicians. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H. and Weintraub, S. (2001). The Boston Naming Test, 2nd edn.Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
Mohs, R. C.et al. (1997). Development of cognitive instruments for use in clinical trials of antidementia drugs: additions to the Alzheimer's disease assessment scale that broaden its scope. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 11 (Suppl 2), 1321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. C.et al. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 39, 11591165.Google Scholar
NICE-SCIE (2007). Dementia: Supporting People with Dementia and their Carers in Health and Social Care. NICE Clinical Guideline 42. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (www.nice.org.uk).Google Scholar
Reitan, R. M. and Wolfson, D. (1992). The Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation, 2nd edn.Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rosen, W. G., Mohs, R. C. and Davis, K. L. (1984). A new rating scale for Alzheimer's disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 13561364.Google ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 6887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spector, A.et al. (2003). Efficacy of an evidence-based cognitive stimulation therapy programme for people with dementia: Randomised Controlled Trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 183, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spector, A., Thorgrimsen, L., Woods, B. and Orrell, M. (2006). Making a Difference: An Evidence-Based Group Programme to Offer Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (CST) to People with Dementia: Manual for Group Leaders. UK: Hawker Publications.Google Scholar
Spector, A., Orrell, M. and Woods, B. (2010). Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (CST): effects on different areas of cognitive function for people with dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 12531258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S. and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 3rd edn.Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Swaab, D. F., Dubelaar, E. J. G., Hofman, M. A., Scherder, E. J. A., van Someren, E. J. W. and Verwer, R. W. H. (2002). Brain aging and Alzheimer's disease; use it or lose it. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 345375.Google ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edn.San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Woods, B., Thorgrimsen, L., Spector, A., Royan, L. and Orrell, M. (2006). Improved quality of life and cognitive stimulation therapy in dementia. Aging and Mental Health, 10, 219226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, B., Aguirre, E., Orrell, M. and Spector, A. (2012). Cognitive stimulation for people with dementia (review). The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar