Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T13:22:43.393Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Clinical characteristics of early-onset versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 July 2023

Paige Seath
Affiliation:
Academic Psychiatry Division, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Luis Enrique Macedo-Orrego
Affiliation:
Departamento de Psiquiatría, Universidad Nacional Mayor de San Marcos, Lima, Peru Departamento de atencion especializada de adultos mayores, Instituto Nacional de Salud Mental, Lima, Peru
Latha Velayudhan*
Affiliation:
Academic Psychiatry Division, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Latha Velayudhan, Academic Psychiatry Division, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, 16 De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Phone: 020 7848 0508. Email: [email protected].

Abstract

Objectives:

A number of studies have compared Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the commonest form of dementia, based on their age of onset, i.e. before the age of 65 years (early-onset AD, EO-AD) to those developing after 65 years of age (late-onset AD, LO-AD), but the differences are not clear. We performed a systematic review and meta-analysis to compare clinical characteristics between EO-AD and LO-AD.

Design, measurements, and participants:

Medline, Embase, PsycINFO, and CINAHL databases were systematically searched for studies comparing time to diagnosis, cognitive scores, annual cognitive decline, activities of daily living (ADLs), neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS), quality of life (QoL), and survival time for EO-AD and LO-AD patients.

Results:

Forty-two studies were included (EO-AD participants n = 5,544; LO-AD participants n = 16,042). An inverse variance method with random effects models was used to calculate overall effect estimates for each outcome. People with EO-AD had significantly poorer baseline cognitive performance and faster cognitive decline but longer survival times than people with LO-AD. There was no evidence that EO-AD patients differ from people with LO-AD in terms of symptom onset to diagnosis time, ADLs, and NPS. There were insufficient data to estimate overall effects of differences in QoL in EO-AD compared to LO-AD.

Conclusions:

Our findings suggest that EO-AD differs from LO-AD in baseline cognition, cognitive decline, and survival time but otherwise has similar clinical characteristics to LO-AD. Larger studies using standardized questionnaires focusing on the clinical presentations are needed to better understand the impact of age of onset in AD.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Psychogeriatric Association

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common form of dementia, accounting for approximately 60–70% of the current 55 million dementia cases worldwide (WHO, 2021). Most often, symptoms develop after 65 years of age and known as late-onset AD (LO-AD) (WHO, 1992). However, it can also develop before 65 years of age, called early-onset AD (EO-AD), which accounts for approximately 5.5% of all AD cases (Zhu et al., Reference Zhu2015).

There is evidence that EO-AD and LO-AD differ in clinical presentation. For instance, many researchers have observed that a significantly larger proportion of EO-AD patients exhibit non-amnestic presentations in which their main symptoms involve language deficits, apraxia, and visuospatial deficits, more so than memory complaints, which are typical in LO-AD patients (Gumus et al., Reference Gumus, Multani, Mack and Tartaglia2021). However, non-memory symptoms are not exclusive to EO-AD. Licht et al. (Reference Licht, McMurtray, Saul and Mendez2007) observed poorer verbal fluency and motor-executive scores in LO-AD compared to EO-AD. Differences in the clinical characteristics and disease course have been compared between EO-AD and LO-AD; however, there are no reviews that compared EO-AD and LO-AD. Better understanding of the characteristics of EO-AD relative to LO-AD could facilitate early recognition of EO-AD in clinical settings and, therefore, enable appropriate management. It could also provide patients and their caregivers or families with more information regarding the symptoms and prognostic course for planning care. We undertook a systematic review and meta-analysis to examine if there were any differences in clinical characteristics such as time to diagnosis, cognition, neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS), activities of daily living (ADLs), quality of life (QoL), and survival time between patients with EO-AD and LO-AD.

Method

Study selection

The review was undertaken according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) reporting guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman and The2009). A systematic search was conducted in the databases Medline, EMBASE, PsycINFO, and CINAHL for papers published until October 26, 2021. More relevant references were identified by the snowballing method, with manual search of reference lists of identified papers. The search terms were “Alzheimer’s disease” (as a sub-heading) AND (“early-onset Alzheimer* disease” OR “young*-onset Alzheimer* disease”) AND “late-onset Alzheimer* disease” AND “age of onset” (sub-heading) OR “Mini-Mental State Examination” OR “cognitive decline” OR “Neuropsychiatric Inventory” OR “Activities of Daily Living” (sub-heading) OR “survival” (sub-heading) OR “patient outcome assessment” (sub-heading).

Studies were included if (1) they included original research comparing diagnosed EO-AD and LO-AD patients in terms of time to diagnosis, cognitive status, cognitive decline, NPS, functional status, quality of time, or survival; (2) they defined EO-AD as any age below 65-years-old at onset and LO-AD as any age from over 65-years-old at onset; and (3) they were published as peer-reviewed journal articles in English language. Studies were excluded if they (1) had EO dementias without sub-typing into EO-AD; (2) were non-English; or (3) published as any publication type other than peer-reviewed journal article such as conference abstracts.

The process of selecting the final studies is demonstrated in the PRISMA chart (Figure 1) (Page et al., Reference Page2021). Studies were independently assessed by two researchers and disagreements resolved through consensus or discussions with a senior researcher.

Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram.

Data extraction

A modified version of the Checklist for Critical Appraisal and Data Extraction for Systematic Reviews of Prediction Modelling Studies, Adapted for Prognostic Factor Studies (CHARMS-PF) was used to guide information extraction (Moons et al., Reference Moons2014; Riley et al., Reference Riley2019) (Table 1). Information relating to statistical techniques and model development, study dates, and missing data is addressed in the assessment of risk of bias (Moons et al., Reference Moons2014). Extracted data were then inputted into the software program Review Manager (RevMan) 5.4 (The Cochrane Collaboration, 2020).

Table 1. Table of the included studies’ characteristics

Values are presented as n or mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise specified.

AD = Alzheimer’s disease, EO-AD = Early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, LO-AD = Late-onset Alzheimer’s disease, MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination, ADL = Activities of Daily Living, NPI = Neuropsychiatric Inventory, I-ADL = Instrumental Activities of Daily Living, QoL-AD = Quality of Life-Alzheimer’s Disease, NINCDS-ARDRA = National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke/Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association, IWG = International Working Group, DSM-IV (or DSM-4) = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition, DSM-IV-TR = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition text revision, NIA-AA = National Institute of Aging-Alzheimer’s Association, CREDOS = Clinical Research Center for Dementia of South Korea, ADNI GO/2 = Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Grand Opportunity/2, CAMD = Coalition Against Major Diseases, MCI = Mild cognitive impairment, PET = Positron emission tomography, UK = United Kingdom, USA = United States of America.

Quality assessment

The risk of bias assessment of individual studies was done according to the PROGRESS framework by the application of the quality in prognostic factor studies (QUIPS) tool (Hayden et al., Reference Hayden, van der Windt, Cartwright, Côté and Bombardier2013; Riley et al., Reference Riley2019). The prognostic factor in this review is considered the age (at onset) variable that is used to separate the groups into EO-AD and LO-AD, where EO-AD is before 65 years and LO-AD is after 65 years. The authors made a priori decisions as to which QUIPS domains are most important to this review (described in the supplementary material).

Instruments

Time to diagnosis was defined as the time from symptom onset to diagnosis (Brück et al., Reference Brück, Wolters, Ikram and de Kok2021). Any studies that reported time to diagnosis in months were converted into years. Cognitive scores as obtained using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) assessed baseline cognition and cognitive decline. In AD populations specifically, there is an abundance of evidence suggesting that the MMSE is appropriate (Kørner et al., Reference Kørner, Lauritzen and Bech1996). This, as well as the measure’s wide use in the literature (Stanley and Walker, Reference Stanley and Walker2014; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Whitfield, Kuchenbaecker, Sanders, Stevens and Walker2019), is the rationale for selecting this measure for the current review. NPS are psychological and behavioral disturbances that are core features of AD and measured commonly using Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (Cummings et al., Reference Cummings, Mega, Gray, Rosenberg-Thompson, Carusi and Gornbein1994). Another variable of interest in the current review was functionality, defined as the ability to independently engage in and complete ADLs (Lawton and Brody, Reference Lawton and Brody1969). We also looked at QoL, which, in elderly populations, concerns physical condition, mood, relationships, ability to participate in meaningful activities, financial well-being, and the individual’s perceptions of their QoL, and, in this review, is operationalized as scores on the Quality of Life in Alzheimer’s disease (QoL-AD) scale (Logsdon et al., Reference Logsdon, Gibbons, McCurry and Teri1999). We extracted both self- and informant-reported QoL-AD scores. Finally, the survival time was defined as time from symptom onset or diagnosis to death (Brodaty et al., Reference Brodaty, Seeher and Gibson2012).

Data synthesis and analysis

For continuous variables (time to diagnosis, MMSE scores, mean annual change in MMSE scores, total NPI scores, ADL scores, QoL-AD scores, and survival times), an inverse variance statistical method with a random effects model was used. Random effects models allow for variation in studies’ true effect sizes due to differences in sample characteristics. Standardized mean difference (SMD) was the effect measure to account for differences in scales between studies (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins2022), for example modified or translated versions. RevMan 5.4 automatically calculated the SMD when means, standard deviations (SDs), and sample sizes for each group for each relevant study were inputted. Heterogeneity is assessed in each analysis using the Chi square test and the I 2 statistic. Interpretations of I 2 are based on the cutoff values suggested in the Cochrane Handbook (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins2022). The results of these analyses are illustrated in forest plots (Figures 2 and 3).

Figure 2. (a) and (b) Forest plots comparing symptom onset to diagnosis and cognitive decline of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EO-AD) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LO-AD). (c) Forest plot comparing cognitive performance of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EO-AD) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LO-AD).

Figure 3. (a–c) Forest plots comparing neuropsychiatric symptoms, functionality, and survival times of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EO-AD) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LO-AD).

The Cochrane Group suggests that two studies are adequate to perform a meta-analysis given that those studies are compatible and found similar results (Ryan, Reference Ryan2016). As some of the studies included in the current review were compatible but not with similar results, we performed a meta-analysis where there were three or more studies that reported a mean and SD. When a meta-analysis could not be conducted, the studies were reviewed and reported narratively. When a study had multiple results from the same sample, for example before and after propensity score matching (PSM), analysis was conducted with the original cohort only, but, when a study investigated an outcome after PSM only, the post-PSM result was included in the analysis. Sensitivity analyses were conducted only when the original meta-analysis result is significant (Tawfik et al., Reference Tawfik, Dila, Mohamed, Tam, Ahmed and Huy2019). The factors considered in sensitivity analyses were decided a priori and included study setting, country, and samples’ disease severities when they were reported (Brück et al., Reference Brück, Wolters, Ikram and de Kok2021).

Where variables or measures relevant to the current review were mentioned but no data were reported in a paper, authors were contacted to request for the data, and if we received no response and unable to access the information, the study was not included in the relevant analysis. Where studies used samples stratified by variables in conjunction with age at onset, the data re-stratified by only age at onset with a cutoff value of 65-years-old were requested for from the authors. If provided with raw datasets without summary statistics, we calculated the means and SDs as appropriate. Relevant data from newly stratified versions were only extracted if the equivalent data were published in the original paper. When no inferential test results were provided and the full dataset was available, we used the relevant tests in SPSS version 25 to compare EO- and LO-AD group means. In the case of missing summary statistics data in published papers, they were computed where possible according to the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins et al., Reference Higgins2022). Mean and SDs were obtained upon request from authors for the survival time variable (Rhodius-Meester et al., Reference Rhodius-Meester2019). If sample size data were missing and unable to be provided, the study was excluded from the analysis (Koedam et al., Reference Koedam, Lauffer, van der Vlies, van der Flier, Scheltens and Pijnenburg2010). All meta-analyzed means and SDs are presented in Supplementary Table 1.

Results

Data selection

Figure 1 (PRISMA flow chart) summarizes the study selection procedure. A total of 42 studies (EO-AD participants, n = 5,544, age = 61.32 ± 2.47; LO-AD participants, n = 16,042, age = 77.45 ± 3.27) were included. Main characteristics and outcome measures of each study are included in Table 1. Overall quality assessment of the 42 studies showed 23 (54.8%) had overall low risk of bias, 16 (38.1%) had overall moderate risk of bias, and 3 (7.1%) had overall high risk of bias (Table 1, Supplementary Figures 1 and 2).

Time to diagnosis

Six studies (EO-AD, n = 1,093; LO-AD, n = 3,000) compared the time to diagnosis of AD from symptom onset to diagnosis in EO and LO participants (Falgàs et al., Reference Falgàs2019; Mendez et al., Reference Mendez, Lee, Joshi and Shapira2012; Park et al., Reference Park2015; Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Whitfield, Kuchenbaecker, Sanders, Stevens and Walker2019; van Vliet et al., Reference van Vliet2013; van der Vlies et al., Reference van der Vlies, Koedam, Pijnenburg, Twisk, Scheltens and van der Flier2009). The overall effect estimate was not statistically significant (SMD = 0.20, 95% confidence interval (CI) [−0.12, 0.52], Z = 1.21, p = 0.23) (Figure 2a). These data therefore do not provide evidence of a difference in the time period between onset of symptoms and diagnosis for EO-AD and LO-AD patients.

Cognition at presentation

Of the 36 studies that compared EO-AD and LO-AD cognitive performance using the MMSE, Koedam et al. (Reference Koedam, Lauffer, van der Vlies, van der Flier, Scheltens and Pijnenburg2010) did not provide exact sample size data for the two groups so was excluded. The remaining 35 studies (EO-AD, n = 3,059; LO-AD, n = 11,524) (Figure 2c) showed a statistically significant overall effect estimate (SMD = −0.22, 95% CI [−0.35, −0.09], Z = 3.37, p < 0.001), implying that EO-AD patients had lower MMSE scores than LO-AD patients at initial presentation. However, there was high heterogeneity (I 2 = 85%, χ 2(34) = 231.32, p < 0.001).

Cognitive decline

Ten of the included studies compared the rate of annual cognitive decline in EO-AD versus LO-AD patients using MMSE. Although Jacobs et al. (Reference Jacobs1994) compared six-monthly rate of change over 2 years, the exact change in MMSE scores for the annual period was not available; hence, we could not include them. However, they did report that EO-AD patients showed a more rapid decline than LO-AD (F = 12.50, p < 0.001). We also could not include Stanley et al. (Reference Stanley, Whitfield, Kuchenbaecker, Sanders, Stevens and Walker2019) as they did not report or provide information on means and SDs. The mean difference of MMSE from the analysis of the remaining seven studies (EO-AD, n = 356; LO-AD, n = 995) was statistically significant (SMD = −0.45, 95% CI [−0.74, −0.17], Z = 3.13, p = 0.002), suggesting that EO-AD patients had a greater decrease in their MMSE score per year than LO-AD patients (Figure 2b). There was however significant heterogeneity (I 2 = 66%, χ 2(7) = 17.40, p = 0.008).

Neuropsychiatric symptoms

Six studies (EO-AD, n = 1,049; LO-AD, n = 2,050) compared mean NPI scores of EO-AD and LO-AD participants. LO-AD participants had a greater total NPI score than the EO-AD group; however, this difference was not statistically significant (SMD = −0.42, 95% CI [−0.91, 0.08], Z = 1.66, p = 0.10) (Figure 3a). There was considerable heterogeneity in the effect estimates (I 2 = 96%; χ 2(5) = 132.74, p < 0.001).

There were three studies that compared EO-AD and LO-AD’s mean scores for the NPI subdomains (Baillon et al., Reference Baillon, Gasper, Wilson-Morkeh, Pritchard, Jesu and Velayudhan2019; Mushtaq et al., Reference Mushtaq2016; Toyota et al., Reference Toyota2007). Toyota et al. (Reference Toyota2007) compared only 10 of the subdomains, so we conducted meta-analysis for the original 10 subdomains (EO-AD, n = 110; LO-AD, n = 357). Overall effect estimates for agitation, disinhibition, and irritability could not be calculated in RevMan because SDs were reported as zero by Mushtaq et al. (Reference Mushtaq2016), leaving too few studies to conduct a meta-analysis for these domains. Of the remaining seven subdomains, there were no significant effect estimates, suggesting that EO-AD and LO-AD do not differ in these subdomains (Supplementary Figure 3) or for the total NPS.

Functional status

A number of studies compared ADL scores in EO-AD and LO-AD patients. However, they used different questionnaires such as Lawton ADL (Lawton and Brody, Reference Lawton and Brody1969), FAQ (Pfeffer et al., Reference Pfeffer, Kurosaki, Harrah, Chance and Filos1982), Bristol Activities of Daily Living (BADL) (Bucks et al., Reference Bucks, Ashworth, Wilcock and Siegfried1996), Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study-Activities of Daily Living (ADCS-ADL) (Galasko et al., Reference Galasko1997), and Barthel Index (Mahoney and Barthel, Reference Mahoney and Barthel1965).

Analysis conducted on the three studies that used FAQ (EO-AD, n = 588; LO-AD, n = 1,454) showed the effect estimate was not statistically significant (SMD = 0.16, 95% CI [−0.09, 0.40], Z = 1.25, p = 0.21), suggesting that EO-AD and LO-AD patients have similar independence in instrumental ADLs (Figure 3b). The heterogeneity was not significant (I 2 = 63%, χ 2(2) = 5.39, p = 0.07) (Figure 3b). LO-AD patients had greater functional independence using Lawton I-ADL scores (Wattmo and Wallin, Reference Wattmo and Wallin2017), whereas EO-AD were shown to have less severe functional impairment on the ADCS-ADL scores (Grønning et al., Reference Grønning, Rahmani, Gyllenbuorg, Dessau and Høgh2012). On the other hand, some studies showed no difference between the groups using Lawton I-ADL (Carotenuto et al., Reference Carotenuto2012), BADL (Baillon et al., Reference Baillon, Gasper, Wilson-Morkeh, Pritchard, Jesu and Velayudhan2019), and ADCS-ADL (Kaiser et al., Reference Kaiser2012). Two studies compared Barthel Index scores (basic ADLs) between EO-AD and LO-AD patients. Park et al. (Reference Park2015) reported that EO-AD patients (n = 616) scored higher on the Barthel ADL scale than the LO-AD patients (n = 2,351), without any reference to values. Chang, Reference Chang2017) suggested EO-AD had significantly higher ADL scores (n = 331, mean (± SD) = 18.7 (± 3.2)) compared to LO-AD (n = 3,280, mean (± SD) = 17.9 (± 4.1) (p < 0.001)).

Quality of life

Two studies compared EO-AD and LO-AD and reported both the patient-reported and informant-reported QoL-AD scores separately. For the patient-reported data, EO-AD patients’ (n = 52) mean score was 33.2 (± 6.5), similar to LO-AD (n = 155, 34.3 ± 6.2) (Dourado et al., Reference Dourado, Laks and Mograbi2016). For the informant-reported data, EO-AD patients’ (n = 52) mean score of 29.6 (± 6.2) was similar to LO-AD patients (n = 155, 30.4 ± 7.6). A similarly non-significant finding was reported by Kimura et al. (Reference Kimura2018), wherein EO-AD patients had a self-reported QoL-AD score (n = 53, 33.6 ± 6.5) versus LO-AD (n = 57, 32.9 ± 5.8, p = 0.540). The difference between EO-AD and LO-AD’s caregiver-reported QOL-AD scores was also non-significant (p = 0.895).

Survival time

Three studies examined survival in EO-AD and LO-AD patients. Rhodius-Meester et al. (Reference Rhodius-Meester2019) defined survival as time in years from diagnosis to death, whereas Smirnov et al. (Reference Smirnov, Galasko, Hiniker, Edland and Salmon2021) and Spina et al. (Reference Spina2021) defined it as time in years from symptom onset to death. A meta-analysis could be conducted with these studies after Smirnov et al. (Reference Smirnov, Galasko, Hiniker, Edland and Salmon2021) provided summary statistics stratified by the cutoff of 65-years-old upon request (D. Smirnov, personal communication, December 5, 2021). The overall effect estimate was significant (SMD = 0.28, 95% CI [0.14, 0.42], Z = 3.97, p < 0.001) with moderate but non-significant heterogeneity (I 2 = 65%, χ 2(2) = 5.70, p = 0.06), suggesting that EO-AD patients survive for longer than LO-AD patients (Figure 3c).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review and meta-analysis comparing clinical characteristics of EO-AD to LO-AD. We found people with EO-AD had poorer baseline cognitive scores and faster cognitive decline but longer survival times. They did not differ from people with LO-AD in terms of time to diagnosis, ADLs, and NPS. There were insufficient data to estimate overall effects in QoL.

EO-AD participants showed significantly poorer cognitive performance than LO-AD participants. LO-AD patients typically present mainly with episodic memory complaints, whereas a higher proportion of EO-AD patients exhibit non-amnestic cognitive syndromes that affect domains such as language and visuospatial abilities (Mendez et al., Reference Mendez, Lee, Joshi and Shapira2012), which could explain their lower MMSE scores. EO-AD presents with more non-cognitive symptoms such as depression and anxiety (Gumus et al., Reference Gumus, Multani, Mack and Tartaglia2021), and they may use association areas to compensate for their cognitive difficulties (Solé-Padullés et al., Reference Solé-Padullés2009) and it is not until later when the cognitive symptoms are more obvious and impacting their day-to-day life that they approach for assessment, which may also be a reason for lower scores at presentation. Although the effect size was small, this is of potential theoretical interest and an area to study further.

People with EO-AD showed a more rapid rate of annual decline in global cognitive function compared to LO-AD, with a medium effect size, which is potentially clinically meaningful and needs to be established in future studies. This may be due to the faster, more severe neuropathological changes that have been observed in EO-AD patients (Sakai et al., Reference Sakai2013). The faster EO-AD decline has been observed with multiple measures of cognition (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Kennedy, Wang and Cutter2015), so it would not have been affected by our choice of limiting the cognitive measure to the MMSE. The faster cognitive decline has also been noted beyond the period of 24 months from baseline. Sakai et al. (Reference Sakai2013) found that EO-AD patients’ MMSE decreased at an average rate of 1.9 (SD = 1.0) points per year, up to 90 months from baseline, which was significantly greater than LO-AD patients whose MMSE decreased by 1.1 (SD = 0.8) points on average per year (p < 0.001).

We did not see a difference in time to diagnosis from symptom onset to getting diagnosed between the two groups. Previous evidence that EO dementia has longer time to diagnosis may be influenced by the other subtypes of EO-dementias such as frontotemporal dementia which are more prevalent in younger age, more non-amnestic presentations, total number of specialist services consulted which increased the time to diagnosis, and maybe also lack of competence even in specialist services (Kvello-Alme et al., Reference Kvello-Alme, Bråthen, White and Sando2021; Loi et al., Reference Loi2022). However, when we focussed on EO-AD versus LO-AD there seems to be no difference, as also seen when time to diagnosis was compared for different types of young onset dementia, with shorter time to diagnosis in people with AD compared to the “other” dementia group (Loi et al., Reference Loi2022). This needs to be further investigated and compared between different dementia subtypes for the age groups. A recent study found delay in time to diagnosis of people with EO-AD who were mostly diagnosed using biomarkers (Kvello-Alme et al., Reference Kvello-Alme, Bråthen, White and Sando2021). This study however did not compare with LO-AD group. It would be useful and important for future studies to have accurate diagnosis using biomarkers which are now more available to compare EO-AD versus LO-AD.

Our meta-analysis of studies investigating EO-AD and LO-AD’s ADL using FAQ suggested that there was no difference in functional dependence, indicating the illness affects both younger and older adults similarly. In the literature, there is high variability in use of measures to assess functional status in AD. Future studies should aim to use consistent measures.

There was no significant difference in NPS between EO-AD and LO-AD. This is consistent with recent studies that also found no significant difference between LO-AD and EO-AD for overall NPI scores (Altomari et al., Reference Altomari2022; Falgàs et al., Reference Falgàs2022). We also found no significant group differences in any of the NPI sub-domain scores. Most NPI sub-domain difference effect sizes were negligible to small; however, the effect size for the group difference in aberrant motor behavior was large, but because of the small sample of studies limited the analysis’ power to detect statistical significance.

Two identified studies suggested that QoL for EO-AD and LO-AD patients was not significantly different regardless of who was reporting it, patient or the caregiver. More research in QoL comparing EO-AD versus LO-AD is needed (Ducharme et al., Reference Ducharme, Lachance, Kergoat, Coulombe, Antoine and Pasquier2016).

The lack of difference in NPS, functional dependence, and QoL possibly demonstrates that AD impacts people in similar ways irrespective of age at onset.

There were three studies comparing survival times (Rhodius-Meester et al., Reference Rhodius-Meester2019; Smirnov et al., Reference Smirnov, Galasko, Hiniker, Edland and Salmon2021; Spina et al., Reference Spina2021). Contrary to the assumption that EO-AD progresses quickly with a short survival period, the effect estimate suggested that EO-AD patients survive longer than LO-AD patients. The explanation could be that younger people are in better physical health and medical conditions compared to elderly with LO-AD (Spina et al., Reference Spina2021). EO-AD patients may also have protective factors, such as more commonly experiencing atypical presentations such as language symptoms or executive dysfunction, which are associated with longer survival (Pavisic et al., Reference Pavisic2020). It could also be because the studies included in the review were sporadic EO-AD cases which are unlike familial EO-AD which is known to have rapid progression (Loeffler, Reference Loeffler2021). Future research could investigate survival in different types of EO-AD cases compared to LO-AD.

It is interesting that our meta-analysis showed that EO-AD had lower cognitive performance at presentation and greater cognitive decline but longer survival rate. Higher age at diagnosis, higher number of medical comorbidities, and greater disability have been shown to predict shortened survival better than cognitive impairment (Lichtenstein et al., Reference Lichtenstein2018). Later age at diagnosis and greater disease severity at presentation have been associated with shorter survival time (Brodaty et al., Reference Brodaty, Seeher and Gibson2012; Schaffert et al., Reference Schaffert2022). Future studies should examine the role of cognitive impairment in predicting life expectancy in those with milder dementia using more sensitive neuropsychological measures (Schaffert et al., Reference Schaffert2022).

Our review and meta-analysis are limited by methodological weaknesses in the included studies as identified during quality assessment, specifically pertaining to sample size, selective outcome reporting, and variability of questionnaires used in the studies. Of the included studies, 18 were investigating neuroimaging and/or biomarkers and, although we excluded studies that matched groups by MMSE scores, there may have been potential confounders in some studies that may have clinically homogenous groups.

Our review was limited to studies that directly compared EO-AD and LO-AD. Some studies identified in the literature search divided their samples into EO-AD, middle-onset AD (MO-AD), and LO-AD (Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Whitfield, Kuchenbaecker, Sanders, Stevens and Walker2019). Their findings suggest that MO-AD may be another separate sub-type of AD, as found that MO-AD patients have a significantly different monthly rate of MMSE decline compared to EO-AD and LO-AD patients (Stanley et al., Reference Stanley, Whitfield, Kuchenbaecker, Sanders, Stevens and Walker2019). Future meta-analyses could also compile research comparing EO-AD, MO-AD, and LO-AD in order to provide a further elucidated and more specific understanding of the effects of age of onset on AD characteristics if there are enough studies. We conducted a meta-analysis for the survival time including studies which defined them either as diagnosis to death or symptom onset to death similar to the criteria previously used (Brodaty et al., Reference Brodaty, Seeher and Gibson2012). However, future studies should have uniform and similar definitions for better comparisons. More research comparing EO-AD and LO-AD patients, particularly in the domains of basic ADLs and QoL, is needed to improve clinical knowledge of how these conditions differ based on age of onset. We did not test the influences of prevalence of APOE ϵ4 allele, presence of co-pathologies, or compare biomarkers such as blood and CSF markers, or neuroimaging (brain volumes). Future reviews should examine influences of these too.

Conclusion

Our meta-analysis suggests that people with EO-AD have poorer cognitive performance at presentation, faster cognitive decline, and longer survival times compared to people with LO-AD but did not differ in time to diagnosis, functional dependence, and total NPS. This implies that the AD condition is similar and affects people in similar way irrespective of age of onset. However, further research is warranted for clarification about differences in EO-AD and LO-AD’s QoL. It is important to understand EO-AD’s characteristics to facilitate early identification, diagnosis, and management and to alleviate the burdensome social, emotional, and financial, as well as medical consequences. A greater understanding of EO-AD symptoms, course, and prognosis will also aid patients and their caregivers and families for future care needs and planning. Better understanding of clinical features along with their underlying neuropathologies would also support precision medicine with appropriate pharmacological and non-pharmacological interventions.

Conflict of interest

None.

Description of author(s)’ roles

LM and PS did the literature review. PS did data extraction, assessed risk of bias, carried out the analysis, and drafted the manuscript. LV conceived the project, was involved in study selection, assessed risk of bias, supervised data extraction and analysis, interpretation of the results, and finalized the manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Sara Pisani for help with R codes.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610223000509.

References

Altomari, N. et al. (2022). A comparison of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) and BPSD sub-syndromes in early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 85, 691699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillon, S., Gasper, A., Wilson-Morkeh, F., Pritchard, M., Jesu, A. and Velayudhan, L. (2019). Prevalence and severity of neuropsychiatric symptoms in early- versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 34, 433438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brodaty, H., Seeher, K. and Gibson, L. (2012). Dementia time to death: a systematic literature review on survival time and years of life lost in people with dementia. International Psychogeriatrics, 24, 10341045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brück, C. C., Wolters, F. J., Ikram, M. A. and de Kok, I. M. C. M. (2021). Heterogeneity in reports of dementia disease duration and severity: a review of the literature. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 84, 15151522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucks, R., Ashworth, D., Wilcock, G. and Siegfried, K. (1996). Assessment of activities of daily living in dementia: development of the Bristol Activities of Daily Living Scale. Age and Ageing, 25, 113120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carotenuto, A. et al. (2012). Late and early onset dementia: what is the role of vascular factors? A retrospective study. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 322, 170175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chagué, P. et al. (2021). Radiological classification of dementia from anatomical MRI assisted by machine learning-derived maps. Journal of Neuroradiology, 48, 412418. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurad.2020.04.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, K. J. et al. (2017). Mortality risk after diagnosis of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease: a propensity score matching analysis. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 56, 13411348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chishiki, Y. et al. (2020). Different patterns of gray matter volume reduction in early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer disease. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 33, 253258. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000245 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cho, H. et al. (2013). Longitudinal changes of cortical thickness in early- versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 34, 1921.e9e1921.e15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Contador, J. et al. (2021). Longitudinal brain atrophy and CSF biomarkers in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage: Clinical, 32, 102804. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102804 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, J. L., Mega, M., Gray, K., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., Carusi, D. A. and Gornbein, J. (1994). The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology, 44, 23082314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dourado, M. C. N., Laks, J. and Mograbi, D. (2016). Functional status predicts awareness in late-onset but not in early-onset Alzheimer disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 29, 313319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ducharme, F., Lachance, L., Kergoat, M.-J., Coulombe, R., Antoine, P. and Pasquier, F. (2016). A comparative descriptive study of characteristics of early-and late-onset dementia family caregivers. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 31, 4856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eckerström, C., Klasson, N., Olsson, E., Selnes, P., Rolstad, S. and Wallin, A. (2018). Similar pattern of atrophy in early‐ and late‐onset Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 10, 253259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2018.02.001 Google ScholarPubMed
Elahi, F. M. et al. (2020). Plasma biomarkers of astrocytic and neuronal dysfunction in early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 16, 681695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2019.09.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falgàs, N. et al. (2019). Hippocampal atrophy has limited usefulness as a diagnostic biomarker on the early onset Alzheimer’s diseases patients: a comparison between visual and quantitative assessment. NeuroImage: Clinical, 23, 101927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falgàs, N. et al. (2022). The severity of neuropsychiatric symptoms is higher in early-onset than late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. European Journal of Neurology, 29, 957967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, M. C., Abreu, M. J., Machado, C., Santos, B., Machado, Á. and Costa, A. S. (2018). Neuropsychiatric profile in early versus late onset Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 33, 9399. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317517744061 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisoni, G. B., Testa, C., Sabattoli, F., Beltramello, A., Soininen, H. and Laakso, M. P. (2005). Structural correlates of early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease: Voxel-Based morphometric study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 76, 112114. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2003.029876 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galasko, D. et al. (1997). An inventory to assess activities of daily living for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 11, S33S39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerritsen, A. A. J. et al. (2016). Prevalence of comorbidity in patients with young-onset Alzheimer disease compared with late-onset: a comparative cohort study. Journal of the American Medical Directors Association, 17, 318323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jamda.2015.11.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gour, N. et al. (2014). Functional connectivity changes differ in early and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Human Brain Mapping, 35, 29782994. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22379 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grønning, H., Rahmani, A., Gyllenbuorg, J., Dessau, R. B. and Høgh, P. (2012). Does Alzheimer’s disease with early onset progress faster than with late onset? A case-control study of clinical progression and cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 33, 111117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gumus, M., Multani, N., Mack, M. L. and Tartaglia, M. C. (2021). Progression of neuropsychiatric symptoms in young-onset versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. GeroScience, 43, 213223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guven, G. et al. (2020). Peripheral TREM2 mRNA levels in early and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease patients. Molecular Biology Reports, 47, 59035909. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11033-020-05661-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayden, J., van der Windt, D., Cartwright, J., Côté, P. and Bombardier, C. (2013). Assessing bias in studies of prognostic factors. Annals of Internal Medicine, 158, 280286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T. et al. (2022). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Cochrane. Available at: www.training.cochrane.org/handbook Google Scholar
Jacobs, D. et al. (1994). Age at onset of Alzheimer’s disease: relation to pattern of cognitive dysfunction and rate of decline. Neurology, 44, 12151220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, N. C. et al. (2012). Neuropsychological and neuroimaging markers in early versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 27, 520529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, N. R. S. et al. (2018). Caregivers’ perspectives of quality of life of people with young- and late-onset Alzheimer disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 31, 7683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koedam, E. L. G. E., Lauffer, V., van der Vlies, A. E., van der Flier, W. M., Scheltens, P. and Pijnenburg, Y. A. L. (2010). Early- versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease: more than age alone. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 19, 14011408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kørner, A., Lauritzen, L. and Bech, P. (1996). A psychometric evaluation of dementia rating scales. European Psychiatry, 11, 185191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kvello-Alme, M., Bråthen, G., White, L. R. and Sando, S. B. (2021). Time to diagnosis in young onset alzheimer’s disease: a population-based study from central Norway. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 82, 965974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawton, M. P. and Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activity of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9, 179186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Licht, E. A., McMurtray, A. M., Saul, R. E. and Mendez, M. F. (2007). Cognitive differences between early- and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 22, 218222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenstein, M. L. et al. (2018). 16-year survival of the Canadian collaborative cohort of related dementias. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 45, 367374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loeffler, D. A. (2021). Modifiable, non-modifiable, and clinical factors associated with progression of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 80, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logsdon, R. G., Gibbons, L. E., McCurry, S. M. and Teri, L. (1999). Quality of life in Alzheimer’s disease: patient and caregiver reports. Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 5, 2132.Google Scholar
Loi, S. M. et al. (2022). Time to diagnosis in younger-onset dementia and the impact of a specialist diagnostic service. International Psychogeriatrics, 34, 367375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahoney, F. I. and Barthel, D. W. (1965). Functional evaluation: the Barthel index. Maryland State Medical Journal, 14, 6165.Google ScholarPubMed
Mendez, M. F., Lee, A. S., Joshi, A. and Shapira, J. S. (2012). Nonamnestic presentations of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 27, 413420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Migliaccio, R. et al. (2015). Mapping the progression of atrophy in early and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 46, 351364. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-142292 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G. and The, P. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6, e1000097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moons, K. G. M. et al. (2014). Critical appraisal and data extraction for systematic reviews of prediction modelling studies: the CHARMS checklist. PLoS Medicine, 11, e1001744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mushtaq, R. et al. (2016). A comparison of the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) in early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s – a study from South East Asia (Kashmir, India). Cureus, 8, e625.Google ScholarPubMed
Page, M. J. et al. (2021). The PRISMA. 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. PLoS Medicine, 18, e1003583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palasí, A. et al. (2015). Differentiated clinical presentation of early and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease: is 65 years of age providing a reliable threshold? Journal of Neurology, 262, 12381246. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-015-7698-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panegyres, P. K. and Chen, H. Y. (2013). Differences between early and late onset Alzheimer’s disease. American Journal of Neurodegenerative Disease, 2, 300306.Google ScholarPubMed
Park, H. K. et al. (2015). Cognitive profiles and neuropsychiatric symptoms in Korean early-onset Alzheimer’s disease patients: a CREDOS study. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 44, 661673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavisic, I. M. et al. (2020). Disease duration in autosomal dominant familial Alzheimer disease: a survival analysis. Neurology Genetics, 6, e507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfeffer, R. I., Kurosaki, T. T., Harrah, C. H., Chance, J. M., Filos, S. (1982). Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. Journal of Gerontology, 37, 323329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picard, C., Pasquier, F., Martinaud, O., Hannequin, D. and Godefroy, O. (2011). Early onset dementia: characteristics in a large cohort from academic memory clinics. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Disorders, 25, 203205. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0b013e3182056be7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rhodius-Meester, H. F. M. et al. (2019). Survival in memory clinic cohort is short, even in young-onset dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 90, 726728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riley, R. D. et al. (2019). A guide to systematic review and meta-analysis of prognostic factor studies. The British Medical Journal, 364, k4597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, C. B. et al. (2021). Assessing the retinal microvasculature in individuals with early and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Opthalmic Surgery, Lasers, & Imaging Retina, 52, 336344. https://doi.org/10.3928/23258160-20210528-06 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R. (2016). Cochrane consumers and communication review group: Meta-analysis . Available at: http://cccrg.cochrane.org/ Google Scholar
, F. et al. (2012). Differences between early and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease in neuropsychological tests. Frontiers in Neurology, 3, 81. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2012.00081 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakai, M. et al. (2013). Rate of progression of Alzheimer’s disease in younger versus older patients: a longitudinal single photon emission computed tomography study. Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 13, 555562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaffert, J. et al. (2022). Predictors of life expectancy in autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 86, 271281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, L. S., Kennedy, R. E., Wang, G. and Cutter, G. R. (2015). Differences in Alzheimer disease clinical trial outcomes based on age of the participants. Neurology, 84, 11211127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shinotoh, H. et al. (2000). Progressive loss of cortical acetylcholinesterase activity in association with cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease: a positron emission tomography study. Annals of Neurology, 48, 194200. https://doi.org/10.1002/1531-8249(200008)48:2<194::AID-ANA9>3.0.CO;2-X 3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smirnov, D. S., Galasko, D., Hiniker, A., Edland, S. D. and Salmon, D. P. (2021). Age-at-onset and APOE-related heterogeneity in pathologically confirmed sporadic Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 96, e2272e2283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solé-Padullés, C. et al. (2009). Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 30, 11141124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spina, S. et al. (2021). Comorbid neuropathological diagnoses in early versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 144, 21862198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stage, E. C. Jr et al. (2020). Neurodegenerative changes in early- and late-onset cognitive impairment with and without brain amyloidosis. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 12, 93. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-020-00647-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, K. and Walker, Z. (2014). Do patients with young onset Alzheimer’s disease deteriorate faster than those with late onset Alzheimer’s disease? A review of the literature. International Psychogeriatrics, 26, 19451953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, K., Whitfield, T., Kuchenbaecker, K., Sanders, O., Stevens, T. and Walker, Z. (2019). Rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease stratified by age. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 69, 11531160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tawfik, G. M., Dila, K. A. S., Mohamed, M. Y. F., Tam, D. N. H., Ahmed, A. M., and Huy, N. T. (2019). A step by step guide for conducting a systematic review and meta-analysis with simulation data. Tropical Medicine and Health, 47, 46. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41182-019-0165-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The Cochrane Collaboration (2020). Review manager.Google Scholar
Toyota, Y. et al. (2007). Comparison of behavioral and psychological symptoms in early-onset and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 896901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Vlies, A. E., Koedam, E. L. G. E., Pijnenburg, Y. A. L., Twisk, J. W. R., Scheltens, P. and van der Flier, W. M. (2009). Most rapid cognitive decline in APOE ϵ4 negative Alzheimer’s disease with early onset. Psychological Medicine, 39, 19071911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Vliet, D. et al. (2012). Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in young-onset compared to late-onset Alzheimer’s disease - part 1: findings of the two-year longitudinal NeedYD-study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 34, 319327. https://doi.org/10.1159/000342824 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Vliet, D. et al. (2013). Time to diagnosis in young-onset dementia as compared with late-onset dementia. Psychological Medicine, 43, 423432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wattmo, C. and Wallin, Å.K. (2017). Early-versus late-onset Alzheimer’s disease in clinical practice: cognitive and global outcomes over 3 years. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 9, 70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders. Switzerland: WHO Library Cataloguing.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2021). Dementia.Google Scholar
Zhu, X.-C. et al. (2015). Rate of early onset Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Annals of Translational Medicine, 3, 38.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram.

Figure 1

Table 1. Table of the included studies’ characteristics

Figure 2

Figure 2. (a) and (b) Forest plots comparing symptom onset to diagnosis and cognitive decline of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EO-AD) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LO-AD). (c) Forest plot comparing cognitive performance of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EO-AD) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LO-AD).

Figure 3

Figure 3. (a–c) Forest plots comparing neuropsychiatric symptoms, functionality, and survival times of early-onset Alzheimer’s disease (EO-AD) and late-onset Alzheimer’s disease (LO-AD).

Supplementary material: File

Seath et al. supplementary material

Seath et al. supplementary material

Download Seath et al. supplementary material(File)
File 481.5 KB