Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T12:21:25.295Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An investigation into early-life stress and cognitive function in older age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2019

Sarah A. Grainger*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
John D. Crawford
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Nicole A. Kochan
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Karen A. Mather
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia Neuroscience Research Australia (NeuRA), Sydney, Australia
Russell J. Chander
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Brian Draper
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Henry Brodaty
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Perminder S. Sachdev
Affiliation:
Centre for Healthy Brain Ageing (CHeBA), School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia Neuropsychiatric Institute, the Prince of Wales Hospital, Sydney, Australia
Julie D. Henry
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Sarah A. Grainger, PhD, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, St Lucia QLD 4072, Australia. Email: [email protected].

Abstract

Early-life stress (ELS) has previously been identified as a risk factor for cognitive decline, but this work has predominantly focused on clinical groups and indexed traditional cognitive domains. It, therefore, remains unclear whether ELS is related to cognitive function in healthy community-dwelling older adults, as well as whether any effects of ELS also extend to social cognition. To test each of these questions, the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) was administered to 484 older adults along with a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery and a well-validated test of social cognitive function. The results revealed no differences in global cognition according to overall experiences of ELS. However, a closer examination into the different ELS subscales showed that global cognition was poorer in those who had experienced physical neglect (relative to those who had not). Social cognitive function did not differ according to experiences to ELS. These results indicate that the relationship between ELS and cognition in older age may be dependent on the nature of the trauma experienced.

Type
Brief Report
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Germine, L., Dunn, E. C., McLaughlin, K. A. and Smoller, J. W. (2015). Childhood adversity is associated with adult theory of mind and social affiliation, but not face processing. PLoS One, 10, e0129612. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, F., Clarke, J., Heim, C., Harvey, P. D., Majer, M. and Nemeroff, C. B. (2012). The effects of child abuse and neglect on cognitive functioning in adulthood. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46, 500506. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. D., von Hippel, W., Molenberghs, P., Lee, T. and Sachdev, P. S. (2016). Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nature Reviews Neurology, 12, 2839. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korten, N. C., Penninx, B. W., Pot, A. M., Deeg, D. J. and Comijs, H. C. (2014). Adverse childhood and recent negative life events: contrasting associations with cognitive decline in older persons. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 27, 128138. doi: 10.1177/0891988714522696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majer, M., Nater, U. M., Lin, J. M., Capuron, L. and Reeves, W. C. (2010). Association of childhood trauma with cognitive function in healthy adults: a pilot study. BMC Neurology, 10, 6170. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-10-61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petkus, A. J., Lenze, E. J., Butters, M. A., Twamley, E. W. and Wetherell, J. L. (2018). Childhood trauma is associated with poorer cognitive performance in older adults. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 79. doi: 10.4088/JCP.16m11021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, K., Jaussent, I., Stewart, R., Dupuy, A. M., Courtet, P., Malafosse, A. and Ancelin, M. L. (2011). Adverse childhood environment and late-life cognitive functioning. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 26, 503510. doi: 10.1002/gps.2553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachdev, P. S. et al. (2010). The Sydney Memory and Ageing Study (MAS): Methodology and baseline medical and neuropsychiatric characteristics of an elderly epidemiological non-demented cohort of Australians aged 70–90 years. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 12481264. doi: 10.1017/S1041610210001067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H. and Samson, J. A. (2013). Childhood maltreatment and psychopathology: A case for ecophenotypic variants as clinically and neurobiologically distint subtypes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 11141133. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar