Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:57:21.987Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The association of timing of retirement with cognitive performance in old age: the role of leisure activities after retirement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2016

Andreas Ihle*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Swiss National Center of Competences in Research LIVES–Overcoming vulnerability: life course perspectives
Catherine Grotz
Affiliation:
Psychology of Aging Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
Stéphane Adam
Affiliation:
Psychology of Aging Unit, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
Michel Oris
Affiliation:
Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Swiss National Center of Competences in Research LIVES–Overcoming vulnerability: life course perspectives
Delphine Fagot
Affiliation:
Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Swiss National Center of Competences in Research LIVES–Overcoming vulnerability: life course perspectives
Rainer Gabriel
Affiliation:
Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Swiss National Center of Competences in Research LIVES–Overcoming vulnerability: life course perspectives
Matthias Kliegel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Center for the Interdisciplinary Study of Gerontology and Vulnerability, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Swiss National Center of Competences in Research LIVES–Overcoming vulnerability: life course perspectives
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Andreas Ihle, CIGEV, University of Geneva, route des Acacias 54, 1227 Carouge, Switzerland. Phone: +41 22 37 98308. Email: [email protected].
Get access

Abstract

Background:

The role of timing of retirement on cognitive functioning in old age is inconclusive so far. Therefore, the present study set out to investigate the association of timing of retirement with cognitive performance and its interplay with key correlates of cognitive reserve in a large sample of older adults.

Methods:

Two thousand two hundred and sixty three older adults served as sample for the present study. Different psychometric tests (Trail Making Test part A (TMT A), Trail Making Test part B (TMT B), Mill Hill) were administered. In addition, individuals were interviewed on their retirement, occupation, educational attainment, and regarding 18 leisure activities that have been carried out after retirement.

Results:

Earlier retirement (compared to retirement at legal age) was significantly associated with better performance in the TMT A, the TMT B, and the Mill Hill vocabulary test. Moderation analyses showed that in individuals with a moderate number of leisure activities in old age, earlier retirement was related to better cognitive performance, but not in those with a relatively large number of leisure activities. Hierarchical regression analyses revealed that entering leisure activities as additional predictor significantly increased explained variance in the cognitive measures over and above all other investigated markers of cognitive reserve (i.e. occupation and education).

Conclusions:

Present data further corroborate the view that leisure activities even in old age may lead to further enrichment effects and thereby may be related to better cognitive functioning. The role of engaging in activities in the context of major life events such as retirement is discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adam, S., Bonsang, E., Grotz, C. and Perelman, S. (2013). Occupational activity and cognitive reserve: implications in terms of prevention of cognitive aging and Alzheimer's disease. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 8, 377390.Google Scholar
Andel, R., Silverstein, M. and Kareholt, I. (2015). The role of midlife occupational complexity and leisure activity in late-life cognition. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70, 314321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonsang, E., Adam, S. and Perelman, S. (2012). Does retirement affect cognitive functioning?. Journal of Health Economics, 31, 490501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coe, N. B., Von Gaudecker, H. M., Lindeboom, M. and Maurer, J. (2012). The effect of retirement on cognitive functioning. Health Economics, 21, 913927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deltour, J. (1993). Echelle de vocabulaire Mill Hill de J. C. Raven. [Raven Mill Hill Vocabulary Scale]. Braine-le Chateau, Belgium: Editions l'Application des Techniques Modernes.Google Scholar
Dufouil, C. et al. (2014). Older age at retirement is associated with decreased risk of dementia. European Journal of Epidemiology, 29, 353361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fritsch, T., Larsen, J. D. and Smyth, K. A. (2007). The role of adolescent IQ and gender in the use of cognitive support for remembering in aging. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 394416.Google Scholar
Gabriel, R., Oris, M., Studer, M. and Baeriswyl, M. (2015). The persistence of social stratification? A life course perspective on old-age poverty in Switzerland. Swiss Journal of Sociology, 41, 465487.Google Scholar
Gatz, M., Svedberg, P., Pedersen, N. L., Mortimer, J. A., Berg, S. and Johansson, B. (2001). Education and the risk of Alzheimer's disease: findings from the study of dementia in Swedish Twins. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 56B, 292300.Google Scholar
Grotz, C. et al. (2015). Retirement age and the age of onset of Alzheimer's disease: results from the ICTUS study. Plos One, 10, e0115056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grotz, C. et al. (2016). Why is later age at retirement beneficial for cognition? Results from a French population-based study. The Journal of Nutrition, Health and Aging, 20, 514519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hacker, W. (2003). Action regulation theory: a practical tool for the design of modern work processes. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 12, 105130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S. and Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced?. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karp, A., Paillard-Borg, S., Wang, H. X., Silverstein, M., Winblad, B. and Fratiglioni, L. (2006). Mental, physical and social components in leisure activities equally contribute to decrease dementia risk. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21, 6573.Google Scholar
Kröger, E., Andel, R., Lindsay, J., Benounissa, Z., Verreault, R. and Laurin, D. (2008). Is complexity of work associated with risk of dementia?. American Journal of Epidemiology, 167, 820830.Google Scholar
Lachman, M. E., Agrigoroaei, S., Murphy, C. and Tun, P. A. (2010). Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupton, M. K. et al. (2010). Education, occupation and retirement age effects on the age of onset of Alzheimer's disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 3036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzonna, F. and Peracchi, F. (2012). Ageing, cognitive abilities and retirement. European Economic Review, 56, 691710.Google Scholar
McDowell, I., Xi, G. L., Lindsay, J. and Tierney, M. (2007). Mapping the connections between education and dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29, 127141.Google Scholar
Nucci, M., Mapelli, D. and Mondini, S. (2012). Cognitive reserve index questionnaire (CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 218226.Google Scholar
Opdebeeck, C., Martyr, A. and Clare, L. (2016). Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: a meta-analysis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 23, 4060.Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J. and Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437448.Google Scholar
Pulopulos, M. M., Hidalgo, V., Almela, M., Puig-Perez, S., Villada, C. and Salvador, A. (2014). Hair cortisol and cognitive performance in healthy older people. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 100111.Google Scholar
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271276.Google Scholar
Sanchez-Cubillo, I. et al. (2009). Construct validity of the trail making test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 438450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S. and Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, 483506.Google Scholar
Soubelet, A. (2011). Engaging in cultural activities compensates for educational differences in cognitive abilities. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 18, 516526.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia, 47, 20152028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurology, 11, 10061012.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. S., Boyle, P. A., Yu, L., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A. and Bennett, D. A. (2013). Life-span cognitive activity, neuropathologic burden, and cognitive aging. Neurology, 81, 314321.Google Scholar