Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T17:29:46.407Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do aliens dream of offworld sheep?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2018

Rodrick Wallace*
Affiliation:
Division of Epidemiology, The New York State Psychiatric Institute, Box 47, 1051 Riverside Drive, New York, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Rodrick Wallace, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The Stanley Miller experiment suggests that amino acid-based life is ubiquitous in our universe, although its varieties will not have followed the particular, highly contingent and path-dependent, evolutionary trajectory found on Earth. Are many alien organisms likely to be individually conscious in ways we would recognize? Almost certainly. Will alien consciousness require a ‘sleep cycle’? A strong argument suggests it will. Can some species develop analogs to culture and high-order technology? Less likely, but still fairly probable. If so, will we be able to communicate with them? Only on a basic level, and only with profound difficulty. The reasoning is fairly direct and involves convolution of a learned heritage system with individual and collective consciousness.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Appleby, J, Mao, X and Rodkina, A (2008) Stabilization and destabilization of nonlinear differential equations by noise. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control 53, 126132.Google Scholar
Ash, R (1990) Information Theory. New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Atlan, H and Cohen, I (1998) Immune information, self-organization, and meaning. International Immunology 10, 711717.Google Scholar
Avital, E and Jablonka, E (2000) Animal Traditions: Behavioral Inheritance in Evolution. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B (1988) A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B (2005) Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research 150, 4553.Google Scholar
Bjelakovic, I, Kruger, T, Siegmund-Schultz, R and Szkola, A (2003) Chained typical subspaces – a quantum version of Brieman's theorem, ArXiv, quant-ph/0301177.Google Scholar
Bjelakovic, I, Kruger, T, Siegmund-Schultze, R and Szkola, A (2004) The Shannon-McMillan theorem for ergodic quantum lattice systems. Inventiones Mathematicae 155, 203222.Google Scholar
Booth, V and Diniz Behn, C (2014) Physiologically-based modeling of sleep-wake regulatory networks. Mathematical Biosciences 250, 5468.Google Scholar
Borbely, A (1982) A two process model of sleep regulation. Human Neurobiology 1, 195204.Google Scholar
Brown, R (1987) From groups to groupoids: a brief survey. Bulletin of the London Mathematical Society 19, 113134.Google Scholar
Cartmill, M (2000) Animal consciousness: some philosophical, methodological, and evolutionary problems. American Zoologist 40, 835846.Google Scholar
Cohen, I (2000) Tending Adam's Garden: Evolving the cognitive immune self. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cover, T and Thomas, J (2006) Elements of Information Theory, 2nd Edn. New York: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Daan, S, Beersma, D and Borbely, A (1984) Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. American Journal of Physics 246, 161178.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S and Naccache, L (2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79, 137.Google Scholar
Dretske, F (1994) The explanatory role of information. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A 349, 5970.Google Scholar
Edelman, D, Baars, B and Seth, A (2005) Identifying hallmarks of consciousness in non-mammalian species. Consciousness and Cognition 14, 169187.Google Scholar
El Gamal, A and Kim, Y (2010) Lecture notes on network information theory. arXiv:1001.3404v4 [cs.IT].Google Scholar
Evans, B (2003) Sleep, consciousness and the spontaneous and evoked electrical activity in the brain. Is there a cortical integrating mechanism? Neurophysiiologie Clinique 33, 110.Google Scholar
Feynman, R (2000) Lectures on Computation. New York: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Griffin, D and Speck, G (2004) New evidence of animal consciousness. Animal Cognition 7, 518.Google Scholar
Harris, J, Jolivet, R and Attwell, D (2012) Synaptic energy use and supply. Neuron 75, 762777.Google Scholar
Heine, S (2001) Self as cultural product: an examination of East Asian and North American selves. Journal of Personality 69, 881906.Google Scholar
Hobson, JA (2009) REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10, 803813.Google Scholar
Hubbard, T (2003) Some Correspondences and Similarities of Shamanism and Cognitive Science: Interconnectedness, Extension of Meaning, and Attribution of Mental States. Anthropology of Consciousness 14, 2645.Google Scholar
Hudson, A, Calderon, D, Pfaff, D and Proekt, A (2014) Recovery of consciousness is mediated by a network of discrete metastable activity states. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111, 92839288.Google Scholar
Jin, H, Hu, Z and Zhou, X (2008) A convex stochastic optimization problem arising from portfolio selection. Mathematical Finance 18, 171183.Google Scholar
Khinchin, A (1957) Mathematical Foundations of Information Theory. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Laidler, K (1987) Chemical Kinetics, 3rd Edn. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Landau, L and Lifshitz, E (2007) Statistical Physics, Part I. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lu, J, Sherman, D and Saper, C (2006) A putative flip-flop switch for control of REM sleep. Nature 441, 589594.Google Scholar
Madsen, P and Vorstrup, S (1991) Cerebral blood flow and metabolism during sleep. Cerebrovascular Brain Metabolism Review 3, 281296.Google Scholar
Markus, H and Kitayama, S (1991) Culture and self-implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Reviews 98, 224253.Google Scholar
Matsuda, T and Nisbett, R (2006) Culture and change blindness. Cognitive Science 30, 381399.Google Scholar
Maturana, H and Varela, F (1980) Autopoiesis and Cognition. Dordrecht: Riedel.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R, Peng, K, Incheol, C and Norenzayan, A (2001) Culture and the system of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. Psycholical Review 108, 291310.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R and Miyamoto, Y (2005) The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Science 10, 467473.Google Scholar
Penrose, R (1994) Shadows of the Mind: A Search for the Missing Science of Consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pettini, M (2007) Geometry and Topology in Hamiltonian Dynamics and Statistical Mechanics. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Phillips, A and Robinson, P (2007) A quantitative model of sleep-wake dynamics based on the physiology of the brainstem ascending arousal system. Journal of Biological Rhythms 22, 167179.Google Scholar
Protter, P (2005) Stochastic Integration and Differential Equations, 2nd Edn. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Richerson, P and Boyd, R (2004) Not by Genes Alone; How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Saper, C, Cano, G and Scammell, T (2005) Homeostatic, circadian, and emotional regulation of sleep. Journal of Comparative Neurology 493, 9298.Google Scholar
Sergeant, C and Dehaene, S (2004) Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-nothing bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science 15, 720728.Google Scholar
Skeldon, A, Dijk, D and Derks, G (2014) Mathematical models for sleep-wake dynamics: comparison of the two-process and a mutual inhibition neuronal model PlosOne 9,e103877.Google Scholar
Spenser, J (2010) The giant component: the golden anniversary. Notices of the American Mathematical Society 57, 720724.Google Scholar
Tegmark, M (2000) Importance of quantum decoherence in brain processes. Physical Review E 61, 41944206.Google Scholar
Tegmark, M (2015) Consciousness as a state of matter. Chaos, Solitons, and Fractals 76, 238270.Google Scholar
Tononi, G (2012) Phi: A voyate from the Brain to the Soul. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2000) Language and coherent neural amplification in hierarchical systems: renormalization and the dual information source of a generalized spatiotemporal stochastic resonance. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos 10, 493502.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2005) Consciousness: A Mathematical Treatment of the Global Neuronal Workspace Model. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2007) Culture and inattentional blindness. Journal of Theoretical Biology 245, 378390.Google Scholar
Wallace, R and Fullilove, M (2008) Collective Consciousness and its Discontents. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Wallace, RG and Wallace, R (2009) Evolutionary radiation and the spectrum of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 18, 160167.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2010) Expanding the modern synthesis. Comptes Rendus Biologies 333, 701709.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2012) Consciousness, crosstalk, and the mereological fallacy: an evolutionary perspective. Physics of Life Reviews 9, 426453.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2013) A new formal approach to evolutionary processes in socioeconomic systems. Journal of Evolutionary Economics 23, 115.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2015) Closed-system ‘economic’ models for psychiatric disorders: Western atomism and its culture-bound syndromes. Cognitive Processing 16, 279290.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2016a) High metabolic demand in neural tissues: information and control theory perspectives on the synergism between rate and stability. Journal of Theoretical Biology 409, 8696.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2016b) The metabolic economics of environmental adaptation. Ecological Modelling 322, 4853.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2017) Computational Psychiatry: A systems approach to the epigenetics of mental disorders. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2018a) New statistical models of nonergodic cognitive systems and their pathologies. Journal of Theoretical Biology 436, 7278.Google Scholar
Wallace, R (2018b) Culture and the trajectories of developmental pathology: insights from control and information theories. Acta Biotheoretica 66, 79112.Google Scholar
Worthman, C and Melby, M (2002) Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep. In Carskadon, M (ed.), Adolescent Sleep Patterns: Biological, Social, and Psychological Influences. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 69117.Google Scholar