Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T11:40:54.423Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Some New (and Old) Suggestions for Improving Personnel Selection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 January 2015

Nathan R. Kuncel*
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
*
E-mail: [email protected] , Address: Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology 2008 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota

References

Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.10.4159/9780674029019Google Scholar
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1967). The genesis of popular but erroneous psychodiagnostic observations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 193204.10.1037/h0024670Google Scholar
Dawes, R. (1979). The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making. American Psychologist, 34, 571582.10.1037/0003-066X.34.7.571Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1978). Confidence in judgment: Persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 85, 395406.10.1037/0033-295X.85.5.395Google Scholar
Gaeth, G. J., & Shanteau, J. (1984). Reducing the influence of irrelevant information on experienced decision makers. Organizational Behavior and Organizational Performance, 33, 263282.10.1016/0030-5073(84)90024-2Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1970). Man versus model of man: A rationale, plus some evidence, for a method of improving on clinical inference. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 422432.10.1037/h0029230Google Scholar
Grove, W. M., & Meehl, P. E. (1996). Comparative efficiency of informal (subjective, impressionistic) and formal (mechanical, algorithmic) prediction procedures: The clinical-statistical controversy. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 2, 293323.10.1037/1076-8971.2.2.293Google Scholar
Highhouse, S. (2008). Stubbon reliance on intuition and subjectivity in employee selection. Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 1, 333342.Google Scholar
Kleinmuntz, B. (1990). Why we still use our heads instead of formulas: Toward an integrative approach. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 296310.10.1037/0033-2909.107.3.296Google Scholar
Kuncel, N. R., & Hezlett, S. A. (2007). Standardized tests predict graduate student’s success. Science, 315, 10801081.Google Scholar
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., et al. (2001). Psychological testing and psychological assessment. American Psychologist, 56, 128165.10.1037/0003-066X.56.2.128Google Scholar
Sackett, P. R., & Roth, L. (1991). A Monte Carlo examination of banding and rank order methods of test score use in personnel selection. Human Performance, 4, 279295.Google Scholar
Sawyer, J. (1966). Measurement and prediction, clinical and statistical. Psychological Bulletin, 66, 178200.10.1037/h0023624Google Scholar
Schmidt, F. L., Mack, M. J., & Hunter, J. E. (1984). Selection utility in the occupation of U.S. park ranger for three modes of test use. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 490497.Google Scholar
Smither, J. W., Reilly, R. R., Millsap, R. E., Pearlman, K., & Stoffey, R. W. (1993). Applicant reactions to selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 46, 4976.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 11241131.Google Scholar