Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:10:41.744Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infant Mental Health Research in Africa: a call for action for research in the next 10 years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2015

Mark Tomlinson*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Cape Town, Matieland, Stellenbosch, South Africa
Barak Morgan
Affiliation:
Global Risk Governance Program, Department of Public Law, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
*
* Address for correspondence: M. Tomlinson, Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Private Bag X1, Matieland, Stellenbosch 7602, South Africa. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background.

Less than 3% of articles published in the peer reviewed literature include data from low- and middle-income countries – where 90% of the world's infants live.

Methods.

In this paper, we discuss the context of infancy in Africa and the conditions of adversity obtaining in Africa.

Results.

We discuss the implications of poverty on parenting, and linked to this outline the impact of maternal depression on infant development.

Conclusions.

We outline three features of the field of infant mental health research in Africa, and issue a call for action about what we believe is needed in order to develop the field in the next decade.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2015

Introduction

At first blush – at least in the public imagination – ‘infantFootnote Footnote 1 mental health’ is not an easy concept to understand. How can an infant get depressed? Is there such a thing as an anxious infant? One of the founding fathers of psychology, William James, described the infant's world as a ‘blooming, buzzing confusion’ (p. 488) (James, Reference James1890). As late as the 1940s there were still doubts about whether infants could distinguish colours and whether they could see faces. These false beliefs have been replaced by a wealth of data detailing how (for example) infants are immaculately attuned to the emotions of others (Trevarthen & Aitken, Reference Trevarthen and Aitken2001), have basic numeracy skills (Van de Rijt & Van Luit, Reference Van De Rijt and Van Luit1999) and are co-creators of relationships with caregivers and others (Nagy et al. Reference Nagy, Liotti, Brown, Waiter, Bromiley, Trevarthen and Bardos2010). The notion of infants being fundamentally in relation with another is aptly formulated by British paediatrician and psychoanalyst Winnicott when he stated ‘There is no such thing as an infant… without maternal care one would find no infant’ (p. 39) (Winnicott, Reference Winnicott1966). The extreme dependency of infants on other people for their survival immediately places the infant (as well as the child) in relationship with others. From an evolutionary perspective it would therefore make sense that infants are born with the potential for sophisticated social, gestural and imitative interaction in order to ensure their own survival, and equally are vulnerable to mental health difficulties.

More recently, with the development of sophisticated brain measurement techniques and technologies, we now know the extent to which early experiences (even prenatally) are crucial in brain development (Webb et al. Reference Webb, Monk and Nelson2001). While there is considerable data detailing the development of infants and children in high-income countries (HIC), the same is not true for evidence on the diverse experiences and conditions that promote or impede infant development in low- and middle-income countries (LMIC), and more specifically in Africa. In this paper, we discuss the context of infancy in Africa, the implications of poverty on parenting, and linked to this provide a brief outline of the impact of maternal depression on infant and child development. We will then outline three priorities for research on infant mental health and infant development in Africa.

Infants in Africa

In the global popular imagination Africa is frequently conceptualised in single country terms – people are referred to as African whether they come from Botswana or Egypt. In fact, Africa has a population of over a billion people, and high levels of diversity with up to 3000 languages and 54 states. Ethiopia alone has five religions and over 80 ethnic groups, while there are over 500 ethnic groups and 522 languages in Nigeria. This diversity should present fertile research ground – particularly in human development. The reality, however, is one of limited research outputs – the result of poor research infrastructure, lack of human resources, coupled with endemic poverty.

Africa is the poorest continent and has 16 of the top 20 poorest nations in the world, and is characterised by extremely high levels of infant mortality and morbidity. Globally, significant gains have been made in infant and child survival in the last two decades, with a child born today having half the risk of dying than if they were born in 1990 (Liu et al. Reference Liu, Oza, Hogan, Perin, Rudan, Lawn, Cousens, Mathers and Black2014). Unfortunately, this is not true for Africa – Africa's share of child deaths has in fact increased since 1990. More than 50% of maternal and child deaths now occur in Africa although Africa comprises only 14% of the world's population (Lawn et al. Reference Lawn, Blencowe, Oza, You, Lee, Waiswa, Lalli, Bhutta, Barros, Christian, Mathers and Cousens2014). Approximately one in eight children under the age of five continues to die in sub-Saharan Africa; with 121 deaths per 1000 live births. Poverty and lack of education are the key factors determining under-five mortality rates, manifesting as high rates of teenage pregnancies, low contraceptive use and lack of access to skilled birth attendance and undernourishment among children (Barros et al. Reference Barros, Ronsmans, Axelson, Loaiza, Bertoldi, Franca, Bryce, Boerma and Victora2012).

Despite the high levels of mortality, the African countries in which children are dying are the same ones in which many more children are surviving (Irwin et al. Reference Irwin, Siddiq and Hertzman2007). Approximately 200 million children in developing countries do not achieve their developmental and cognitive potential because of living in conditions of chronic poverty and their compounding lack of education (Grantham-McGregor et al. Reference Grantham-Mcgregor, Cheung, Cueto, Glewwe, Richter and Strupp2007). Lack of resources, particularly in early child development services impairs potential in children (Shonkoff et al. Reference Shonkoff, Richter, Van Der Gaag and Bhutta2012).

Parenting in contexts of poverty

Parenting is particularly sensitive to perturbations in the psychosocial context (Tomlinson, Reference Tomlinson, Tyano, Keren, Herman and Cox2010). From a public health perspective, it has been argued that parenting difficulties are the proximal mechanism by which broader structural and societal factors operate (Link & Phelan, Reference Link and Phelan1995). Another way of describing this is to conceptualise socio-economic status as an upstream determinant, while disease and parenting are downstream determinants (Gehlert et al. Reference Gehlert, Sohmer, Sacks, Mininger, Mcclintock and Olopade2008). There are three predominant influences on parental functioning – parental factors and the personal psychological resources of the caregiver; child characteristics such as temperament and gender; and finally contextual conditions such as poverty, stress and support (Belsky, Reference Belsky1984). Parenting is embedded in a myriad of social factors that may affect child development (O'Connor & Scott, Reference O'Connor and Scott2007). An important premise of this model is that there is a potentially inexorable co-variation of risk factors in the environment of any child (O'Connor & Scott, Reference O'Connor and Scott2007). The seminal work of Sameroff has shown the limitations of considering only single risk factors in infant and child development (Sameroff & Seifer, Reference Sameroff and Seifer1983). It is the severity and chronicity of cumulative risk factors combined with persistent poor environmental conditions such as poverty and family instability that are more important than any single risk factor (Sameroff & Seifer, Reference Sameroff and Seifer1983).

Poverty and living in a violent community impact on the choices parents make to protect their children. For example, attempts at increased monitoring in order to protect the child from dangerous environments or from associating with an ‘inappropriate peer group’ often result in the escalation of the harsh and coercive aspects of caregiving (Beyers et al. Reference Beyers, Bates, Pettit and Dodge2003). In this scenario, poverty exerts its influence on infant and child outcome in a distal way via parenting practices. Poverty may also contribute to familial stress and maternal depression which in turn affects parenting, and by extension child outcome. Biological factors such as nutrition and intrauterine growth restriction may affect outcome in a proximal way (increased mortality and morbidity), but also in a distal way by increasing the vulnerability of the infant to an already stressed and adverse environment (Tomlinson, Reference Tomlinson, Tyano, Keren, Herman and Cox2010). Factors such as foetal alcohol syndrome may result in mental impairment which may result in heightened levels of abuse and neglect. Parenting is rendered more difficult with an infant who has foetal alcohol syndrome, but it is the mental impairment that compromises parenting practices over and above the more immediate direct effects of poverty.

High-risk environments characteristic of much of Africa, and where community relationships may be less positive, lead to more stressful day-to-day interactions for families (Kochanska, Reference Kochanska1997), resulting in parents that are depleted and unable to muster the necessary energy to parent (Halpern, Reference Halpern and Zeanah1993). Ghate and Hazel have argued that while many parents do well (even those living in poverty), parenting in environments characterised by poverty is more ‘risky’ than parenting elsewhere, and that the poorer the environment the more difficult parenting becomes (Garbarino & Kostelny, Reference Garbarino and Kostelny1992).

Maternal depression

In conditions of extreme poverty and instability, the demands on parents differ markedly from those facing parents in communities that have typically been the focus of research in infant mental health. Preoccupation with external problems (e.g. poverty and lack of partner support), as well as more immediate difficulties (e.g. trauma and losses), may directly affect the parent's capacity to be responsive to their infant. This difficulty may be further compounded by maternal mental health problems, in particular, by the occurrence of depression. Warm, sensitive and non-intrusive caregiving helps infants to regulate their behaviour and emotions. Disruptions such as maternal depression, absence or illness, may also however impact on infant and child development. Accumulating evidence suggests that maternal mental health may be contributing to a failure to improve key public health outcomes for women and infants (Rahman et al. Reference Rahman, Surkan, Cayetano, Rwagatare and Dickson2013). For the purposes of this paper, a powerful case could be made for focusing on other important influences on infant mental health such as physical maltreatment or psychological control. However, given the increasing global burden of disease attributable to depression (Lim et al. Reference Lim, Vos, Flaxman, Danaei, Shibuya, Adair-Rohani, Amann, Anderson, Andrews, Aryee, Atkinson, Bacchus, Bahalim, Balakrishnan, Balmes, Barker-Collo, Baxter, Bell, Blore, Blyth, Bonner, Borges, Bourne, Boussinesq, Brauer, Brooks, Bruce, Brunekreef, Bryan-Hancock, Bucello, Buchbinder, Bull, Burnett, Byers, Calabria, Carapetis, Carnahan, Chafe, Charlson, Chen, Chen, Cheng, Child, Cohen, Colson, Cowie, Darby, Darling, Davis, Degenhardt, Dentener, Des Jarlais, Devries, Dherani, Ding, Dorsey, Driscoll, Edmond, Ali, Engell, Erwin, Fahimi, Falder, Farzadfar, Ferrari, Finucane, Flaxman, Fowkes, Freedman, Freeman, Gakidou, Ghosh, Giovannucci, Gmel, Graham, Grainger, Grant, Gunnell, Gutierrez, Hall, Hoek, Hogan, Hosgood, Hoy, Hu, Hubbell, Hutchings, Ibeanusi, Jacklyn, Jasrasaria, Jonas, Kan, Kanis, Kassebaum, Kawakami, Khang, Khatibzadeh, Khoo, Kok, Laden, Lalloo, Lan, Lathlean, Leasher, Leigh, Li, Lin, Lipshultz, London, Lozano, Lu, Mak, Malekzadeh, Mallinger, Marcenes, March, Marks, Martin, McGale, McGrath, Mehta, Mensah, Merriman, Micha, Michaud, Mishra, Mohd Hanafiah, Mokdad, Morawska, Mozaffarian, Murphy, Naghavi, Neal, Nelson, Nolla, Norman, Olives, Omer, Orchard, Osborne, Ostro, Page, Pandey, Parry, Passmore, Patra, Pearce, Pelizzari, Petzold, Phillips, Pope, Pope, Powles, Rao, Razavi, Rehfuess, Rehm, Ritz, Rivara, Roberts, Robinson, Rodriguez-Portales, Romieu, Room, Rosenfeld, Roy, Rushton, Salomon, Sampson, Sanchez-Riera, Sanman, Sapkota, Seedat, Shi, Shield, Shivakoti, Singh, Sleet, Smith, Smith, Stapelberg, Steenland, Stockl, Stovner, Straif, Straney, Thurston, Tran, Van Dingenen, van Donkelaar, Veerman, Vijayakumar, Weintraub, Weissman, White, Whiteford, Wiersma, Wilkinson, Williams, Williams, Wilson, Woolf, Yip, Zielinski, Lopez, Murray, Ezzati, AlMazroa and Memish2012), coupled with the fact that depression is a multigenerational disorder, (England & Sim, Reference England and Sim2009), we have decided to focus on maternal depression in this paper.

In South Africa, up to 35%–47% of women have been diagnosed with major depressive disorder (Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999; Rochat et al. Reference Rochat, Tomlinson, Barnighausen, Newell and Stein2011; Tsai & Tomlinson, Reference Tsai and Tomlinson2012). Not only is major depressive disorder accompanied by substantial morbidity and disability (Lim et al. Reference Lim, Vos, Flaxman, Danaei, Shibuya, Adair-Rohani, Amann, Anderson, Andrews, Aryee, Atkinson, Bacchus, Bahalim, Balakrishnan, Balmes, Barker-Collo, Baxter, Bell, Blore, Blyth, Bonner, Borges, Bourne, Boussinesq, Brauer, Brooks, Bruce, Brunekreef, Bryan-Hancock, Bucello, Buchbinder, Bull, Burnett, Byers, Calabria, Carapetis, Carnahan, Chafe, Charlson, Chen, Chen, Cheng, Child, Cohen, Colson, Cowie, Darby, Darling, Davis, Degenhardt, Dentener, Des Jarlais, Devries, Dherani, Ding, Dorsey, Driscoll, Edmond, Ali, Engell, Erwin, Fahimi, Falder, Farzadfar, Ferrari, Finucane, Flaxman, Fowkes, Freedman, Freeman, Gakidou, Ghosh, Giovannucci, Gmel, Graham, Grainger, Grant, Gunnell, Gutierrez, Hall, Hoek, Hogan, Hosgood, Hoy, Hu, Hubbell, Hutchings, Ibeanusi, Jacklyn, Jasrasaria, Jonas, Kan, Kanis, Kassebaum, Kawakami, Khang, Khatibzadeh, Khoo, Kok, Laden, Lalloo, Lan, Lathlean, Leasher, Leigh, Li, Lin, Lipshultz, London, Lozano, Lu, Mak, Malekzadeh, Mallinger, Marcenes, March, Marks, Martin, McGale, McGrath, Mehta, Mensah, Merriman, Micha, Michaud, Mishra, Mohd Hanafiah, Mokdad, Morawska, Mozaffarian, Murphy, Naghavi, Neal, Nelson, Nolla, Norman, Olives, Omer, Orchard, Osborne, Ostro, Page, Pandey, Parry, Passmore, Patra, Pearce, Pelizzari, Petzold, Phillips, Pope, Pope, Powles, Rao, Razavi, Rehfuess, Rehm, Ritz, Rivara, Roberts, Robinson, Rodriguez-Portales, Romieu, Room, Rosenfeld, Roy, Rushton, Salomon, Sampson, Sanchez-Riera, Sanman, Sapkota, Seedat, Shi, Shield, Shivakoti, Singh, Sleet, Smith, Smith, Stapelberg, Steenland, Stockl, Stovner, Straif, Straney, Thurston, Tran, Van Dingenen, van Donkelaar, Veerman, Vijayakumar, Weintraub, Weissman, White, Whiteford, Wiersma, Wilkinson, Williams, Williams, Wilson, Woolf, Yip, Zielinski, Lopez, Murray, Ezzati, AlMazroa and Memish2012), but also new-born children of mothers with depression have poorer health (Rahman et al. Reference Rahman, Lovel, Bunn, Iqbal and Harrington2004) and socio-emotional development (Weissman et al. Reference Weissman, Pilowsky, Wickramaratne, Talati, Wisniewski, Fava, Hughes, Garber, Malloy, King, Cerda, Sood, Alpert, Trivedi and Rush2006), with adverse implications for their psychosocial, cognitive and economic outcomes (Heckman, Reference Heckman2007). Maternal depression is a public health concern in and of itself, but also has intergenerational effects due to its association with disturbances in the mother–infant relationship, poor new-born health (Gavin et al. Reference Gavin, Gaynes, Lohr, Meltzer-Brody, Gartlehner and Swinson2005) and poorer psychosocial, cognitive and economic outcomes (Heckman, Reference Heckman2007). A caregiver's responsiveness to the attachment needs of a child has been shown to be strongly associated with whether the child develops a secure or insecure attachment (Tomlinson et al. Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005). A secure attachment in turn benefits the child in terms of lower chances of behavioural problems, improved social functioning, better relationships with their peers and enhanced school performance (van Ijzendoorn et al. Reference Van Ijzendoorn, Dijkstra and Bus1995, Young et al. Reference Young, Beitchman, Johnson, Douglas, Atkinson, Escobar and Wilson2002). All of these caregiver tasks may be compromised in the contexts of poverty and high adversity, and certainly in the case of maternal depression.

Having briefly sketched some of the demands on parents in contexts of poverty, we will now outline three features of the field of infant mental health research in Africa, and linked to each of these issues a call for action for research in the next 10 years.

State of the knowledge base

Current context

Over 90% of the world's infants are born in LMIC (Population Reference Bureau, 2013). The so-called ‘10/90 gap’ (Saxena et al. Reference Saxena, Paraje, Sharan, Karam and Sadana2006), refers to the fact that only 10% of the worldwide spending on health research is directed towards the problems that primarily affect the poorest 90% of the world's population. A study conducted in 2003 surveying articles on infancy between 1996 and 2001 from major international journals, reported that a meagre 5% of articles emanated from parts of the world other than North America, Europe or Australasia (Tomlinson & Swartz, Reference Tomlinson and Swartz2003). In a follow-up study reviewing articles published between 2002 and 2012 only 2.3% of articles published in 11 years included data from LMIC – where 90% of the world's infants live (Tomlinson et al. Reference Tomlinson, Bornstein, Marlow and Swartz2014). The knowledge that we do have about infant mental health from Africa and other LMIC is predominantly from studies with small sample sizes and in a single setting (Walker et al. Reference Walker, Wachs, Gardner, Lozoff, Wasserman, Pollitt and Carter2007). This narrow participant database and restricted range continues to constrain our understanding of infant mental health, and impedes our ability to begin untangling the many idiosyncrasies of infant development and caregiving (Bornstein et al. Reference Bornstein, Britto, Nonoyama-Tarumi, Ota, Petrovic and Putnick2012), and affects our ability to identify which domains of development are susceptible to which experiences.

Call for action: increased funding for research and research capacity development in Africa

First and foremost, in order to deepen the knowledge base on infant mental health in Africa, there needs to be a substantial increase in research funding. While significant funding for mental health research broadly (not simply infant mental health) has not been forthcoming from funders, there have been a number of recent initiatives from Grand Challenges Canada, the Department for International Development (UK Aid) and the National Institute of Mental Health (USA) that have injected significant funding into mental health research. Much of this funding however is targeted at adult mental health research. A notable exception is that of the Grand Challenges Canada Saving Brains initiative that has provided funding to 11 studies in LMIC for the follow-up of cohorts that received an intervention in the first 1000 days with the aim of increasing the evidence base about child cognitive development.

Linked to the need for increased funding, concerted efforts need to be made to improve research capacity throughout Africa. Initiatives such as the Centre for Public Mental Health (joint initiative between Stellenbosch University and the University of Cape Town in South Africa) that offers a distance M. Phil. degree for applicants throughout Africa is an example of one such initiative. Other multi-country research studies such as the Programme for Improving Mental Health Care which is being conducted in three African countries (Ethiopia, South Africa and Uganda) have large components of capacity building (with six Ph. D. students in Ethiopia alone) will also contribute to improving research capacity on the continent. Increased resources for research, and focus on capacity building in Africa are essential if the 10/90 gap is to be meaningfully reversed.

Longitudinal and early intervention research

Current context

To the best of our knowledge there is only one longitudinal African study (tracking infant and child development) that includes data on at least three generations (original mother, child and then child of the child) – Birth to Twenty Study (Richter et al. Reference Richter, Norris and De Wet2004). As has already been shown, data on infant mental health and development from Africa are limited, and that which exist are largely cross-sectional in nature. Longitudinal studies in Africa are essential if we are to be in a position to make causal attributions across the lifespan. Increasing investment in longitudinal studies in a variety of contexts in Africa will allow us to begin unravelling the dynamics of infant and child development across the life cycle, and allow us to move from complicated correlations to developing causal models of prediction in one of the most diverse continents on earth (Butz & Torrey, Reference Butz and Torrey2006).

In addition, while the Birth to Twenty has produced seminal data on infant and child development across the lifespan (and will continue to do so), it is epidemiological and did not include an intervention (early or late). Given the adversity that characterises much of infant and child development in Africa, exploring the long-term impact of interventions across the lifespan using gold standard randomised controlled trials is essential.

Call for action: increased funding for longitudinal and early intervention research

We call for increased investment in longitudinal research as well as early intervention and prevention programmes, to improve infant mental health and optimal infant and child development. We need to establish an evidence-base from LMIC to identify the most promising interventions specifically for infant and child mental health. While it is tempting to translate evidence from HIC to LMIC, studies need to be replicated in specific contexts to establish their relevance for these settings (Klasen, Reference Klasen and Crombag2013). Multinational research such as this, constructed within a sensitive contextual framework, will allow the developmental community to distinguish how children's experiences in different settings shape specific trajectories of their development (Bornstein et al. Reference Bornstein, Britto, Nonoyama-Tarumi, Ota, Petrovic and Putnick2012).

There is emerging longitudinal evidence from the continent about infant and child development across the lifespan and in the context of an intervention taking place in the first 1000 days. Cooper et al. designed a manualised home-based intervention delivered by community health workers in a peri-urban settlement in South Africa (Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Landman, Molteno, Stein, Mcpherson and Murray2009). The intervention began in the last trimester of pregnancy, and continued for 6 months postpartum. A total of 16 visits were delivered, particularly intensively in the first three postpartum months. The intervention consists of specific measures for encouraging mothers in sensitive, responsive, interactions with their infants. Findings were consistent with studies from HIC where home visiting support in pregnancy and postnatal, delivered by trained community workers, is associated with a range of improvements in maternal and child functioning (Olds et al. Reference Olds, Holmberg, Donelan-Mccall, Luckey, Knudtson and Robinson2014). The fact that the intervention had a positive impact on both maternal sensitivity and infant security of attachment suggests that a long-term benefit can be anticipated. The same team has recently completed (with funding from Grand Challenges Canada Saving Brains programme) a long-term follow-up of the cohort when the children were 13 years old. These results will provide the first longitudinal data from an early intervention aimed at improving infant and child development in Africa.

The World Health Organization has estimated that 57 countries face health worker shortages; 36 of these countries are in sub-Saharan Africa (World Health Organization, 2006). Task shifting is one of the responses to the human resource crisis and is defined as the shifting or delegation of health services tasks from high level cadres of workers to lower level cadres (Zachariah et al. Reference Zachariah, Ford, Philips, Lynch, Massaquoi, Janssens and Harries2009). Scaling up early interventions, using community health workers within existing health systems (Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Landman, Tomlinson, Molteno, Swartz and Murray2002, Klein & Rye, Reference Klein and Rye2004, Walker et al. Reference Walker, Chang, Powell, Simonoff and Grantham-Mcgregor2006), will allow us to identify the best ways of integrating interventions into existing health platforms, while helping to achieve the desired outcomes related to targets set for child survival and well-being (United Nations, 2013). Data such as these would leverage better informed national and global policies for early child development. We also need to demonstrate social and economic impact – something that longitudinal lifespan research is well placed to show (Gertler et al. Reference Gertler, Heckman, Pinto, Zanolini, Vermeersch, Walker, Chang and Grantham-Mcgregor2014). A coherent evidence base for scalable interventions that can be shown to have impact on economic development and human well-being is also essential (Lund et al. Reference Lund, De Silva, Plagerson, Cooper, Chisholm, Das, Knapp and Patel2011). This is the language of most policy makers. Finally, and this is often forgotten, early interventions to improve infant and child development perhaps provide the most cost-effective means by which we can reduce the burden of adult mental illness.

Resilience and differential susceptibility

Current context

Cooper et al. have shown how maternal depression and the endemic high levels of social adversity both have a significant adverse impact on maternal sensitivity (Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Tomlinson, Swartz, Woolgar, Murray and Molteno1999). However, the same team found that in this context almost two-thirds of the infants were securely attached at 18 months (Tomlinson et al. Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005). One of the reasons for the high rates of secure attachment in a context of extreme poverty was the protective contribution of social and cultural organisation that characterised this community. It was the features of the mother–infant relationship, postpartum depression and the mother's experience of partner support, that were the strongest predictors of infant attachment outcome (Tomlinson et al. Reference Tomlinson, Cooper and Murray2005), rather than poverty per se. This in no way omits the role of poverty and adversity in contributing to increased levels of postpartum depression or the lack of partner support (poverty is instrumental in the genesis of both). Rather, these findings suggest that the simple measurement of socio-economic adversity is not, in itself, sufficient for a complex understanding of the factors involved in understanding parenting, poverty and infant and child developmental trajectory. Similarly, caution must be exercised in ascribing a causal role to parenting factors when in fact there are a number of other unmeasured confounders (such as inherited factors); unrecognised factors that we are not able to measure using available instruments; or even confounders that we are not aware of at all (Thapar & Rutter, Reference Thapar and Rutter2009). The capacity of many infants and children to thrive, even in contexts of high adversity (resilience) requires substantial research focus.

Call for action: research on differential susceptibility to context: untangling average, hidden and factitious impact

It is no longer sufficient to view human development from a purely psychosocial or biological perspective. Rapid advances in developmental biology are providing an expanding number of techniques for tracking human development in its environmental context across all stages of the life cycle. Interactions between biology, psychology and environment have implications for individual outcomes and for evaluating the impact of early interventions (Belsky et al. Reference Belsky, Pluess and Widaman2013). A number of recent studies are providing evidence of how infants and children may be differentially susceptible to the environment or context (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van Ijzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn2014). A commonly used metaphor to explain differential susceptibility is that of orchids and dandelions. Some infants and children are like orchids who with optimal early environments and context flourish, but do especially badly when they develop in sub-optimal environments. Conversely, there are infants and children who are like dandelions and who appear to be resilient when in adverse contexts, but do not benefit particularly from positive environments. There is new evidence of how if such interactions are ignored, measured impact frequently consists of an average of large impacts on susceptible children and small impacts on less susceptible children. Average findings conceal hidden impact and falsely portray factitious impact (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van Ijzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn2014).

Figure 1 uses an example from the Fast Track home visiting intervention to explain differential susceptibility. Fast Track was an intervention to prevent high-risk young children from developing externalising psychopathology (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van Ijzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn2014). A purely psychosocial approach that measured externalising psychopathology at age 25 showed an intervention effect on reducing externalising psychopathology (black line). However, introducing individual differences in the glucocorticoid receptor gene (codes for the receptor to which the stress hormone cortisol binds) changes the picture completely. Individuals may carry either the A or non-A form of the gene. The Fast Track researchers had strong biological grounds for targeting this gene (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van Ijzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn2014). They divided the same data used to construct the black line in Figure 1 into two separate data sets according to whether the individuals carried the A or non-A forms of the gene. Plotting this data yielded the grey line (A carriers) and the dotted line (non-A carriers). Seen in this way, the same externalising psychopathology results at age 25 clearly illustrate the differential susceptibility to context (in this case differential susceptibility to intervention) phenomenon. A allele carriers (grey) in the intervention group turn out ‘for better’ (only 18% have psychopathology) whereas control group A carriers turn out ‘for worse’ (75% have psychopathology). For non-A carriers (dotted line), belonging to the intervention or control group makes no difference (56% and 57% have psychopathology, respectively) (Bakermans-Kranenburg & Van Ijzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn2014).

Fig. 1. Differential susceptibility to environment.

Without genotyping, measured impact reflects the average impact on A and non-A carriers combined (black line), while the real impact is underestimated for A carriers (hidden impact) and overestimated for non-A carriers (factitious impact). Differential susceptibility allows the concept of resilience to be unpacked in some important respects. If resilience is a blossoming for the better in the midst of adversity, then the grey orchid line makes it quite clear that resilience is not an inner trait but one that is strongly context dependent with a costly downside when things turn out for worse. Or is resilience a quality of those who do not crash despite adversity – the 40% of dandelions in the Fast Track study who did not develop psychopathology irrespective of whether they were in the intervention or control group? Rather than attempt to answer these questions by reifying resilience as one outcome, differential susceptibility points towards the need to focus on the bigger picture of multiple interacting factors across multiple bio-psychosocial levels.

At the population level, differential susceptibility raises very real impact-evaluation and theory of change questions that are only beginning to come into focus, and will require significant interdisciplinary collaboration across the bio-psychosocial framework including inputs from economics, geography, sociology, public health and law. In order to expand the empirical base going forward, research in Africa should take steps towards integrating biological metrics and differential susceptibility moderators into the conception, design and implementation phases of their work (Shonkoff & Levitt, Reference Shonkoff and Levitt2010). In sum, the longitudinal research priority that we are proposing for African infant mental health and child development research should employ biological metrics, differential susceptibility moderators as well as psychosocial metrics. If this is done, such an approach has the potential to become a potent tool for understanding infant and child development in Africa.

In this paper, we have outlined the current challenges to conducting research into infant mental health and child development in Africa. We have also proposed areas in which African research can be strengthened and in doing so have made a call for a series of actions that we believe will ensure that Africa is able to substantially contribute to the next wave of global infant mental health and developmental research in the next two decades.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 The word ‘infant’ means one who is unable to speak. The age range for what constitutes infancy is fluid, but for the purposes of this paper we will refer to the period of birth to the age of 3 years.

References

Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, Van Ijzendoorn, MH (2015). The hidden efficacy of interventions: gene × environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annual Review of Psychology 66, 381409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barros, AJ, Ronsmans, C, Axelson, H, Loaiza, E, Bertoldi, AD, Franca, GV, Bryce, J, Boerma, JT, Victora, CG (2012). Equity in maternal, newborn, and child health interventions in Countdown to 2015: a retrospective review of survey data from 54 countries. Lancet 379, 12251233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J (1984). The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Development 55, 8396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J, Pluess, M, Widaman, KF (2013). Confirmatory and competitive evaluation of alternative gene-environment interaction hypotheses. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 54, 11351143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyers, JM, Bates, JE, Pettit, GS, Dodge, KA (2003). Neighbourhood structure, parenting processes, and the development of youths’ externalizing behaviors: a multilevel analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology 31, 3553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, MH, Britto, PR, Nonoyama-Tarumi, Y, Ota, Y, Petrovic, O, Putnick, DL (2012). Child development in developing countries: introduction and methods. Child Development 83, 1631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butz, WP, Torrey, BB (2006). Some frontiers in social science. Science 312, 18981900.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, PJ, Landman, M, Tomlinson, M, Molteno, C, Swartz, L, Murray, L (2002). Impact of a mother-infant intervention in an indigent peri-urban South African context: pilot study. British Journal of Psychiatry 180, 7681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, PJ, Tomlinson, M, Swartz, L, Landman, M, Molteno, C, Stein, A, Mcpherson, K, Murray, L (2009). Improving quality of mother-infant relationship and infant attachment in socioeconomically deprived community in South Africa: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 338, b974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, PJ, Tomlinson, M, Swartz, L, Woolgar, M, Murray, L, Molteno, C (1999). Post-partum depression and the mother-infant relationship in a South African peri-urban settlement. British Journal of Psychiatry 175, 554558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
England, MJ, Sim, LJ (2009). Depression in Parents, Parenting, and Children: Opportunities in Improve Identification, Treatment, and Prevention. Committee on Depression, Parenting Practices, and the Healthy Development of Children. National Research Council and Institute of Medicine: Washington.Google Scholar
Garbarino, J, Kostelny, K (1992). Child maltreatment as a community problem. Child Abuse & Neglect 16, 455464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavin, NI, Gaynes, BN, Lohr, KN, Meltzer-Brody, S, Gartlehner, G, Swinson, T (2005). Perinatal depression: a systematic review of prevalence and incidence. Obstetrics & Gynecology 106, 10711083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gehlert, S, Sohmer, D, Sacks, T, Mininger, C, Mcclintock, M, Olopade, O (2008). Targeting health disparities: a model linking upstream determinants to downstream interventions. Health Affairs (Millwood) 27, 339349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gertler, P, Heckman, J, Pinto, R, Zanolini, A, Vermeersch, C, Walker, S, Chang, SM, Grantham-Mcgregor, S (2014). Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science 344, 9981001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghate, D, Hazel, N (2002). Parenting in Poor Environments: Stress, Support and Coping. Jessica Kingsley Publishers: London.Google Scholar
Grantham-Mcgregor, S, Cheung, YB, Cueto, S, Glewwe, P, Richter, L, Strupp, B, International Child Development Steering G (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet 369, 6070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halpern, R (1993). Poverty and infant development. In Handbook of Infant Mental Health (ed. Zeanah, C.H.). pp. 7386, Guiford Press: New York.Google Scholar
Heckman, JJ (2007). The economics, technology, and neuroscience of human capability formation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104, 1325013255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, LG, Siddiq, A, Hertzman, C (2007). Early Child Development: A Powerful Equalizer. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
James, W (1890). The Principles of Psychology. Henry Holt: New York.Google Scholar
Klasen, H, Crombag, A-C (2013). What works where? A systematic review of child and adolescent mental health interventions for low and middle income countries. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 48, 595611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, PS, Rye, H (2004). Interaction-oriented early intervention in Ethiopia: the MISC approach. Infants and Young Children 17, 340354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different temperaments: from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology 33, 228240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawn, JE, Blencowe, H, Oza, S, You, D, Lee, AC, Waiswa, P, Lalli, M, Bhutta, Z, Barros, AJ, Christian, P, Mathers, C, Cousens, SN, Lancet Every Newborn Study G (2014). Every Newborn: progress, priorities, and potential beyond survival. Lancet 384, 189205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, SS, Vos, T, Flaxman, AD, Danaei, G, Shibuya, K, Adair-Rohani, H, Amann, M, Anderson, HR, Andrews, KG, Aryee, M, Atkinson, C, Bacchus, LJ, Bahalim, AN, Balakrishnan, K, Balmes, J, Barker-Collo, S, Baxter, A, Bell, ML, Blore, JD, Blyth, F, Bonner, C, Borges, G, Bourne, R, Boussinesq, M, Brauer, M, Brooks, P, Bruce, NG, Brunekreef, B, Bryan-Hancock, C, Bucello, C, Buchbinder, R, Bull, F, Burnett, RT, Byers, TE, Calabria, B, Carapetis, J, Carnahan, E, Chafe, Z, Charlson, F, Chen, H, Chen, JS, Cheng, AT, Child, JC, Cohen, A, Colson, KE, Cowie, BC, Darby, S, Darling, S, Davis, A, Degenhardt, L, Dentener, F, Des Jarlais, DC, Devries, K, Dherani, M, Ding, EL, Dorsey, ER, Driscoll, T, Edmond, K, Ali, SE, Engell, RE, Erwin, PJ, Fahimi, S, Falder, G, Farzadfar, F, Ferrari, A, Finucane, MM, Flaxman, S, Fowkes, FG, Freedman, G, Freeman, MK, Gakidou, E, Ghosh, S, Giovannucci, E, Gmel, G, Graham, K, Grainger, R, Grant, B, Gunnell, D, Gutierrez, HR, Hall, W, Hoek, HW, Hogan, A, Hosgood, HD III, Hoy, D, Hu, H, Hubbell, BJ, Hutchings, SJ, Ibeanusi, SE, Jacklyn, GL, Jasrasaria, R, Jonas, JB, Kan, H, Kanis, JA, Kassebaum, N, Kawakami, N, Khang, YH, Khatibzadeh, S, Khoo, JP, Kok, C, Laden, F, Lalloo, R, Lan, Q, Lathlean, T, Leasher, JL, Leigh, J, Li, Y, Lin, JK, Lipshultz, SE, London, S, Lozano, R, Lu, Y, Mak, J, Malekzadeh, R, Mallinger, L, Marcenes, W, March, L, Marks, R, Martin, R, McGale, P, McGrath, J, Mehta, S, Mensah, GA, Merriman, TR, Micha, R, Michaud, C, Mishra, V, Mohd Hanafiah, K, Mokdad, AA, Morawska, L, Mozaffarian, D, Murphy, T, Naghavi, M, Neal, B, Nelson, PK, Nolla, JM, Norman, R, Olives, C, Omer, SB, Orchard, J, Osborne, R, Ostro, B, Page, A, Pandey, KD, Parry, CD, Passmore, E, Patra, J, Pearce, N, Pelizzari, PM, Petzold, M, Phillips, MR, Pope, D, Pope, CA 3rd, Powles, J, Rao, M, Razavi, H, Rehfuess, EA, Rehm, JT, Ritz, B, Rivara, FP, Roberts, T, Robinson, C, Rodriguez-Portales, JA, Romieu, I, Room, R, Rosenfeld, LC, Roy, A, Rushton, L, Salomon, JA, Sampson, U, Sanchez-Riera, L, Sanman, E, Sapkota, A, Seedat, S, Shi, P, Shield, K, Shivakoti, R, Singh, GM, Sleet, DA, Smith, E, Smith, KR, Stapelberg, NJ, Steenland, K, Stockl, H, Stovner, LJ, Straif, K, Straney, L, Thurston, GD, Tran, JH, Van Dingenen, R, van Donkelaar, A, Veerman, JL, Vijayakumar, L, Weintraub, R, Weissman, MM, White, RA, Whiteford, H, Wiersma, ST, Wilkinson, JD, Williams, HC, Williams, W, Wilson, N, Woolf, AD, Yip, P, Zielinski, JM, Lopez, AD, Murray, CJ, Ezzati, M, AlMazroa, MA, Memish, ZA (2012). A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 380, 22242260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Link, BG, Phelan, J (1995). Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social behavior Spec No, 80–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, L, Oza, S, Hogan, D, Perin, J, Rudan, I, Lawn, JE, Cousens, S, Mathers, C, Black, RE (2014). Global, regional, and national causes of child mortality in 2000–13, with projections to inform post-2015 priorities: an updated systematic analysis. Lancet.Google Scholar
Lund, C, De Silva, M, Plagerson, S, Cooper, S, Chisholm, D, Das, J, Knapp, M, Patel, V (2011). Poverty and mental disorders: breaking the cycle in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet 378, 15021514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagy, E, Liotti, M, Brown, S, Waiter, G, Bromiley, A, Trevarthen, C, Bardos, G (2010). The neural mechanisms of reciprocal communication. Brain Research 1353, 159167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, TG, Scott, BC (2007). Parenting and Outcomes for Children. Joseph Rowntree Foundation: London.Google Scholar
Olds, DL, Holmberg, JR, Donelan-Mccall, N, Luckey, DW, Knudtson, MD, Robinson, J (2014). Effects of home visits by paraprofessionals and by nurses on children: follow-up of a randomized trial at ages 6 and 9 years. JAMA Pediatrics 168, 114121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Population Reference Bureau (2013). World Population Data Sheet 2013. Population Reference Bureau: Washington.Google Scholar
Rahman, A, Lovel, H, Bunn, J, Iqbal, Z, Harrington, R (2004). Mothers’ mental health and infant growth: a case-control study from Rawalpindi, Pakistan. Child Care, Health and Development 30, 2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rahman, A, Surkan, PJ, Cayetano, CE, Rwagatare, P, Dickson, KE (2013). Grand challenges: integrating maternal mental health into maternal and child health programmes. PLoS Medicine 10, e1001442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, LM, Norris, SA, De Wet, T (2004). Transition from Birth to Ten to Birth to Twenty: the South African cohort reaches 13 years of age. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology 18, 290301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochat, TJ, Tomlinson, M, Barnighausen, T, Newell, ML, Stein, A (2011). The prevalence and clinical presentation of antenatal depression in rural South Africa. Journal of Affective Disorders 135, 362373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, AJ, Seifer, R (1983). Familial risk and child competence. Child Development 54, 12541268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxena, S, Paraje, G, Sharan, P, Karam, G, Sadana, R (2006). The 10/90 divide in mental health research: trends over a 10-year period. British Journal of Psychiatry 188, 8182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, JP, Levitt, P (2010). Neuroscience and the future of early childhood policy: moving from why to what and how. Neuron 67, 689691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shonkoff, JP, Richter, L, Van Der Gaag, J, Bhutta, ZA (2012). An integrated scientific framework for child survival and early childhood development. Pediatrics 129, e460e472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A, Rutter, M (2009). Do prenatal risk factors cause psychiatric disorder? Be wary of causal claims. British Journal of Psychiatry 195, 100101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomlinson, M (2010). Parenting and poverty: a complex interaction. In Parenthood and Mental Health: A Bridge between Infant and Adult Psychiatry (eds Tyano, S., Keren, M., Herman, H. and Cox, J.), pp. 361–372. Wiley Blackwell: London.Google Scholar
Tomlinson, M, Bornstein, MH, Marlow, M, Swartz, L (2014). Imbalances in the knowledge about infant mental health in rich and poor countries: too little progress in bridging the gap. Infant Mental Health Journal 35, 624629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomlinson, M, Cooper, P, Murray, L (2005). The mother-infant relationship and infant attachment in a South African peri-urban settlement. Child Development 76, 10441054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomlinson, M, Swartz, L (2003). Imbalances in the knowledge about infancy: the divide between rich and poor countries. Infant Mental Health Journal 24, 547556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C, Aitken, KJ (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, AC, Tomlinson, M (2012). Mental health spillovers and the Millennium Development Goals: the case of perinatal depression in Khayelitsha, South Africa. Journal of Global Health 2, 010302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations (2013). The Millennium Development Goals Report 2013. United Nations: New York.Google Scholar
Van De Rijt, BA, Van Luit, JE (1999). Milestones in the development of infant numeracy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 40, 6571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Ijzendoorn, MH, Dijkstra, J, Bus, AG (1995). Attachment, intelligence, and language: a meta-analysis†. Social Development 4, 115128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, SP, Chang, SM, Powell, CA, Simonoff, E, Grantham-Mcgregor, SM (2006). Effects of psychosocial stimulation and dietary supplementation in early childhood on psychosocial functioning in late adolescence: follow-up of randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 333, 472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, SP, Wachs, TD, Gardner, JM, Lozoff, B, Wasserman, GA, Pollitt, E, Carter, JA (2007). Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet 369, 145157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, SJ, Monk, CS, Nelson, CA (2001). Mechanisms of postnatal neurobiological development: implications for human development. Developmental Neuropsychology 19, 147171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, MM, Pilowsky, DJ, Wickramaratne, PJ, Talati, A, Wisniewski, SR, Fava, M, Hughes, CW, Garber, J, Malloy, E, King, CA, Cerda, G, Sood, AB, Alpert, JE, Trivedi, MH, Rush, AJ (2006). Remissions in maternal depression and child psychopathology: a STAR*D-child report. JAMA 295, 13891398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winnicott, D (1966). The Family and Individual Development. Basic Books: New York.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2006). The World Health Report 2006: Working together for health. World Health Organization: Geneva.Google Scholar
Young, AR, Beitchman, JH, Johnson, C, Douglas, L, Atkinson, L, Escobar, M, Wilson, B (2002). Young adult academic outcomes in a longitudinal sample of early identified language impaired and control children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 43, 635645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zachariah, R, Ford, N, Philips, M, Lynch, S, Massaquoi, M, Janssens, V, Harries, AD (2009). Task shifting in HIV/AIDS: opportunities, challenges and proposed actions for sub-Saharan Africa. Transaction of Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene 103, 549558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Differential susceptibility to environment.