Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-bslzr Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2025-03-23T15:43:47.524Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Keeping others in our mind or in our heart? Distribution games under cognitive load

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Karen Evelyn Hauge*
Affiliation:
Ragnar Frisch Centre for Economic Research, Gaustadalléen 21, 0349 Oslo, Norway
Kjell Arne Brekke
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, University of Oslo, Postboks 1095 Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway
Lars-Olof Johansson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Box 500, 405 30 Göteborg, Sweden
Olof Johansson-Stenman
Affiliation:
Departement of Economics, School of Business, Economics and Law, University of Gothenburg, Box 640, 405 30 Göteborg, Sweden
Henrik Svedsäter
Affiliation:
Glaxo Smith Kline, Stockley Park West, Blg 10 1-3 Iron Bridge Road, Uxbridge UB11 1BT, UK

Abstract

It has recently been argued that giving is spontaneous while greed is calculated (Rand et al., in Nature 489:427–430, 2012). If greed is calculated we would expect that cognitive load, which is assumed to reduce the influence of cognitive processes, should affect greed. In this paper we study both charitable giving and the behavior of dictators under high and low cognitive load to test if greed is affected by the load. This is tested in three different dictator game experiments. In the dictator games we use both a give frame, where the dictators are given an amount that they may share with a partner, and a take frame, where dictators may take from an amount initially allocated to the partner. The results from all three experiments show that the behavioral effect in terms of allocated money of the induced load is small if at all existent. At the same time, follow-up questions indicate that the subjects’ decisions are more impulsive and less driven by their thoughts under cognitive load.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2015 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-015-9454-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Andreoni, J (1995). Warm glow versus cold prickle: The effect of positive and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. Quartely Journal of Economics, 110(1), 121. 10.2307/2118508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardsley, N (2008). Dictator game giving: altruism or artefact?. Experimental Economics, 11(2), 122133. 10.1007/s10683-007-9172-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamin, DJ, Brown, SA, & Shapiro, JM (2013). Who is ‘behavioral’? Cognitive ability and anomalous preferences. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(6), 12311255. 10.1111/jeea.12055CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosman, R, & van Winden, F (2002). Emotional hazard in a power-to-take experiment. The Economic Journal, 112(476), 147169. 10.1111/1468-0297.0j677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappelletti, D., Güth, W., & Ploner, M. (2011). Being of two minds: ultimatum offers under cognitive constraints. Journal of Economic Psychology, In Press, Accepted Manuscript, doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornelissen, G, Dewitte, S, & Warlop, L (2011). Are social value orientations expressed automatically? decision making in the dictator game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(8), 10801090. 10.1177/0146167211405996CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cubitt, RP, Drouvelis, M, & Gächter, S (2011). Framing and free riding: emotional responses and punishment in social dilemma games. Experimental Economics, 14(2), 254272. 10.1007/s10683-010-9266-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreber, A, Ellingsen, T, Johannesson, M, & Rand, D (2013). Do people care about social context? Framing effects in dictator games. Experimental Economics, 16(3), 349371. 10.1007/s10683-012-9341-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, CC, & Grossman, P (1996). Altruism in anonymous dictator games. Games and Economic Behavior, 16, 181191. 10.1006/game.1996.0081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, C (2011). Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics, 14(4), 583610. 10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, DT, Giesler, BR, & Morris, KA (1995). When comparisons arise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(2), 227236. 10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgement. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814834. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion 2012, London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D, & Beatty, J (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154(3756), 15831585. 10.1126/science.154.3756.1583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization, New York: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Moore, DA, & Loewenstein, G (2004). Self-interest, automaticity, and the psychology of conflict of interest. Social Justice Research, 17(2), 189202. 10.1023/B:SORE.0000027409.88372.b4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piovesan, M, & Wengström, E (2009). Fast or fair? A study of response times. Economics Letters, 105(2), 193196. 10.1016/j.econlet.2009.07.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rand, DG, Greene, JD, & Nowak, MA (2012). Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature, 489(7416), 427430. 10.1038/nature11467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roch, S, Lane, JAS, Samuelson, CD, Allison, ST, & Dent, JL (2000). Cognitive load and the equality heuristic: A two-stage model of resouce overconsumption in small groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83(2), 185212. 10.1006/obhd.2000.2915CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, JF, Fischbacher, U, Thöni, C, & Utikal, V (2012). Affect and fairness: dictator games under cognitive load. Journal of Economic Psychology, 41, 7787. 10.1016/j.joep.2012.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiv, B, & Fedorikhin, A (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278292. 10.1086/209563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiv, B, & Nowlis, Stephen M (2004). The effect of distractions while tasting a food sample: The interplay of informational and affective components in subsequent choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3), 599608. 10.1086/425095CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonnemans, J, Schram, A, & Offerman, T (1998). Public good provision and public bad prevention: The effect of framing. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 34(1), 143161. 10.1016/S0167-2681(97)00042-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swann, WB, Hixon, JG, Stein-Seroussi, A, & Gilbert, DT (1990). The fleeting gleam of praise: Cognitive processes underlying behavioral reaction to self-relevant feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(1), 1726. 10.1037/0022-3514.59.1.17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tinghög, G, Andersson, D, Bonn, C, Böttiger, H, Josephson, C, Lundgren, G et al., (2013). Intuition and cooperation reconsidered. Nature, 498(7452), E1E2. 10.1038/nature12194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trope, Y, & Alfieri, T (1997). Effortfulness and flexibility of dispositional judgment processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 662674. 10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.662CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A, & Kahneman, D (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 11241131. 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Winden, F (2007). Affect and fairness in economics. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 3552. 10.1007/s11211-007-0029-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule-expanded form (Psychology Publications). Iowa: Iowa Research Online: http://ir.uiowa.edu/psychology_pubs/11/.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hauge et al. supplementary material

Hauge et al. supplementary material
Download Hauge et al. supplementary material(File)
File 23.2 KB