Hostname: page-component-7b9c58cd5d-7g5wt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-20T22:59:00.662Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of the level of responsibility on choices under risk: the role of blame

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2025

Gilbert G. Eijkelenboom
Affiliation:
School of Business and Economics, Maastricht University, P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
Ingrid Rohde
Affiliation:
School of Business and Economics, Maastricht University, P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands
Alexander Vostroknutov*
Affiliation:
Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Via delle Regole 101, 38123 Mattarello, TN, Italy

Abstract

We use a within-subjects design to study how responsibility for the payoffs of different number of others influences the choices under risk, and how choosing together with another person changes these decisions. After controlling for the regression to the mean, we find a weak effect of responsibility for one other person on risk taking as compared to choosing just for oneself. We, however, do find that the number of others influenced by the choice matters: when it increases from one to three, risk averse subjects choose riskier options and risk loving subjects choose more cautiously, which pushes the choices towards the modal risk preferences in the population. Mutual responsibility makes choices for others shift even more in the same direction. The observed behavior is in accordance with the blame avoidance hypothesis: decision makers with responsibility try to reduce the amount of blame for their choices, which is minimal when the choices for others are consistent with what they would have chosen for themselves.

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 2018 Economic Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-018-9587-y) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

Andersson, O, Holm, HJ, Tyran, J-R, & Wengström, E (2014). Deciding for others reduces loss aversion. Management Science, 62(1), 2936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, RJ II, Laury, SK, & Williams, AW (2008). Comparing small-group and individual behavior in lottery-choice experiments. Southern Economic Journal, 75(2), 367382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J, & Ritov, I (2004). Omission bias, individual differences, and normality. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94(2), 7485. 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.03.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolton, GE, Ockenfels, A, & Stauf, J (2015). Social responsibility promotes conservative risk behavior. European Economic Review, 74, 109127. 10.1016/j.euroecorev.2014.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Çelen, B, Schotter, A, & Blanco, M (2017). On blame and reciprocity: Theory and experiments. Journal of Economic Theory, 169, 6292. 10.1016/j.jet.2017.01.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, G, & Jackson, MO (2009). The role of responsibility in strategic risk-taking. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 69, 241247. 10.1016/j.jebo.2008.10.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darley, JM, & Shultz, TR (1990). Moral rules: Their content and acquisition. Annual Review of Psychology, 41(1), 525556. 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, JH (1992). Some compelling intuitions about group consensus decisions, theoretical and empirical research, and interpersonal aggregation phenomena: Selected examples, 1950–1990. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 338. 10.1016/0749-5978(92)90044-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eliaz, K, Ray, D, & Razin, R (2006). Choice shifts in groups: A decision-theoretic basis. American Economic Review, 96(4), 13211332. 10.1257/aer.96.4.1321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ertac, S, & Gurdal, MY (2012). Deciding to decide: Gender, leadership and risk-taking in groups. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 83, 2430. 10.1016/j.jebo.2011.06.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics, 10(2), 171178. 10.1007/s10683-006-9159-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, M, & Steinberg, L (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 625635. 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, F (2017). Peer effects on risk behaviour: The importance of group identity. Experimental Economics, 20(1), 100129. 10.1007/s10683-016-9478-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurdal, MY, Miller, JB, & Rustichini, A (2013). Why blame?. Journal of Political Economy, 121(6), 12051247. 10.1086/674409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, GW, Rutström, EE Cox, JC, & Harrison, GW (2008). Risk aversion in laboratory. Risk aversion in experiments, Bingley: Emerald Group Publishing Limited 41196. 10.1016/S0193-2306(08)00003-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, CA, & Laury, SK (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5), 16441655. 10.1257/000282802762024700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D, & Tversky, A (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341 10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.341CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masclet, D, Colombier, N, Denant-Boemont, L, & Lohéac, Y (2009). Group and individual risk preferences: A lottery-choice experiment with self-employed and salaried workers. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 70, 470484. 10.1016/j.jebo.2007.11.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pahlke, J, Strasser, S, & Vieider, FM (2012). Risk-taking for others under accountability. Economics Letters, 114, 102105. 10.1016/j.econlet.2011.09.037CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pahlke, J, Strasser, S, & Vieider, FM (2015). Responsibility effects in decision making under risk. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 51(2), 125146. 10.1007/s11166-015-9223-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruitt, DG (1971). Choice shifts in group discussion: An introductory review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20(3), 339360. 10.1037/h0031922CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, DB, Joseph, J, & Sherwood, R (2009). Risky shift versus cautious shift: Determining differences in risk taking between private and public management decision-making. Journal of Business and Economics Research, 7(1), 6378.Google Scholar
Rohde, IMT, & Rohde, KIM (2011). Risk attitudes in a social context. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 43, 205225. 10.1007/s11166-011-9127-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shupp, RS, & Williams, AW (2008). Risk preference differentials of small groups and individuals. The Economic Journal, 118(525), 258283. 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2007.02112.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoner, J. A. F. (1961). A comparison of individual and group decisions involving risk. Master’s thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Vieider, FM, Villegas-Palacio, C, Martinsson, P, & Mejía, M (2016). Risk taking for oneself and others: A structural model approach. Economic Inquiry, 54(2), 879894. 10.1111/ecin.12290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, MA, Kogan, N, & Bem, DJ (1964). Diffusion of responsibility and level of risk taking in groups. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 68, 263274. 10.1037/h0042190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Eijkelenboom et al. supplementary material

Appendix (for online publication)
Download Eijkelenboom et al. supplementary material(File)
File 296.1 KB