Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T21:47:12.289Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Kin selection as a modulator of human handedness: sex-specific, parental and parent-of-origin effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2024

Bing Dong*
Affiliation:
School of Biology, University of St Andrews, Dyers Brae, St Andrews KY16 9TH, UK
Silvia Paracchini
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, University of St Andrews, North Haugh, St Andrews KY16 9TF, UK
Andy Gardner
Affiliation:
School of Biology, University of St Andrews, Dyers Brae, St Andrews KY16 9TH, UK
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The frequency of left-handedness in humans is ~10% worldwide and slightly higher in males than females. Twin and family studies estimate the heritability of human handedness at around 25%. The low but substantial frequency of left-handedness has been suggested to imply negative frequency-dependent selection, e.g. owing to a ‘surprise’ advantage of left-handers in combat against opponents more used to fighting right-handers. Because such game-theoretic hypotheses involve social interaction, here we perform an analysis of the evolution of handedness based on kin-selection, which is understood to play a major role in the evolution of social behaviour generally. We show that: (1) relatedness modulates the balance of right-handedness vs. left-handedness, according to whether left-handedness is marginally selfish vs. marginally altruistic; (2) sex differences in relatedness to social partners may drive sex differences in handedness; (3) differential relatedness of parents and offspring may generate parent–offspring conflict and sexual conflict leading to the evolution of maternal and paternal genetic effects in relation to handedness; and (4) differential relatedness of maternal-origin vs. paternal-origin genes may generate intragenomic conflict leading to the evolution of parent-of-origin-specific gene effects – such as ‘genomic imprinting’ – and associated maladaptation.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: We investigate how social interaction between kin affects the balance between right-handers and left-handers in humans.

1. Introduction

Most humans show a preference for – or a difference in proficiency of – one hand over the other for a range of tasks (McManus, Reference McManus2019; Papadatou-Pastou et al., Reference Papadatou-Pastou, Ntolka, Schmitz, Martin, Munafò, Ocklenburg and Paracchini2020). The frequency of left-handedness in humans is estimated at 10.6%, fairly stably across regions and populations, and is somewhat higher in males (11.6%) than in females (9.5%) (Papadatou-Pastou et al., Reference Papadatou-Pastou, Ntolka, Schmitz, Martin, Munafò, Ocklenburg and Paracchini2020). Twin studies (Medland et al., Reference Medland, Duffy, Wright, Geffen, Hay, Levy and Boomsma2009) and family studies (Jordan, Reference Jordan1911; Ramaley, Reference Ramaley1913; Lien et al., Reference Lien, Chen, Hsiao and Tsuang2015) show that handedness is heritable, with additive genetic effects appearing to explain around 25% of the variance (Medland et al., Reference Medland, Duffy, Wright, Geffen, Hay, Levy and Boomsma2009). Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have identified 41 loci influencing handedness and explaining around 6% of the heritability (Cuellar-Partida et al., Reference Cuellar-Partida, Tung, Eriksson, Albrecht, Aliev, Andreassen and Medland2021). A recent whole exome sequencing study using the UK Biobank has suggested an association between mutations in the TUBB4B gene and left-handedness and estimated that the heritability of left-handedness owing to rare coding variants to be 0.91% (Schijven et al., Reference Schijven, Sourena, Simon and Clyde2024). Left-handedness has been linked to some psychiatric disorders, such as autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Markou et al., Reference Markou, Ahtam and Papadatou-Pastou2017), schizophrenia (Hirnstein & Hugdahl, Reference Hirnstein and Hugdahl2014) and dyslexia (Abbondanza et al., Reference Abbondanza, Dale, Wang, Hayiou-Thomas, Toseeb, Koomar and Paracchini2023) and there is an overlap among genes underlying these conditions, brain asymmetries and handedness (Papadatou-Pastou et al., Reference Papadatou-Pastou, Ntolka, Schmitz, Martin, Munafò, Ocklenburg and Paracchini2020).

Although many taxa exhibit some form of lateralisation (Rogers, Reference Rogers1980; Ocklenburg & Güntürkün, Reference Ocklenburg and Güntürkün2012), of which handedness is just one form, these typically involve roughly equal numbers of left-sided and right-sided individuals, and so the strong population bias towards right-handers is peculiarly human (Frayer et al., Reference Frayer, Lozano, Bermúdez de Castro, Carbonell, Arsuaga, Radovčić and Bondioli2012; Caspar et al., Reference Caspar, Pallasdies, Mader, Sartorelli and Begall2022). Language processing is also a typically human skill and is highly lateralised, presenting a left hemisphere dominance in most individuals. However, right hemisphere dominance for language can be observed more frequently in left-handers (Knecht et al., Reference Knecht, Dräger, Deppe, Bobe, Lohmann, Flöel and Henningsen2000; Mazoyer et al., Reference Mazoyer, Zago, Jobard, Crivello, Joliot, Perchey and Tzourio-Mazoyer2014). Similarly, atypical lateralisation for other functions is more likely to be observed in left-handers than right-handers (McManus, Reference McManus2022). Understanding how the low but substantial frequency of left-handedness is maintained may therefore serve to illuminate the role of hemispheric specialisation underpinning skills that might have driven human evolution.

The relative stability – with slight variations – of the ~10% incidence of left-handedness in human populations through time (Coren & Porac, Reference Coren and Porac1977; Frayer et al., Reference Frayer, Lozano, Bermúdez de Castro, Carbonell, Arsuaga, Radovčić and Bondioli2012; but see McManus & Bryden, Reference McManus, Bryden, Rapin and Segalowitz1992; McManus, Reference McManus, Sommer and Kahn2009; McManus et al., Reference McManus, Moore, Freegard and Rawles2010) and across regions (Papadatou-Pastou et al., Reference Papadatou-Pastou, Ntolka, Schmitz, Martin, Munafò, Ocklenburg and Paracchini2020) has given rise to the suggestion that left-handedness is maintained by negative frequency-dependent selection, and this has motivated the development of a number of evolutionary game-theoretic hypotheses to explain the phenomenon (Raymond et al., Reference Raymond, Pontier, Dufour and Møller1996; Ghirlanda et al., Reference Ghirlanda, Frasnelli and Vallortigara2009; Abrams & Panaggio, Reference Abrams and Panaggio2012; Schaafsma et al., Reference Schaafsma, Geuze, Riedstra, Schiefenhövel, Bouma and Groothuis2012; Faurie & Raymond, Reference Faurie and Raymond2013). As an illustrative example, the ‘combat hypothesis’ suggests that left-handers suffer a basic disadvantage (Schaafsma et al., Reference Schaafsma, Geuze, Riedstra, Schiefenhövel, Bouma and Groothuis2012; Zickert et al., Reference Zickert, Geuze, van der Feen and Groothuis2018; Papadatou-Pastou et al., Reference Papadatou-Pastou, Ntolka, Schmitz, Martin, Munafò, Ocklenburg and Paracchini2020) – e.g. perhaps owing to disruption of typical brain lateralisation – such that natural selection has resulted in them being in the minority, yet also enjoy a compensating advantage when they are sufficiently rare, owing to the element of surprise in combat and similar competitive interactions (Gibbons, Reference Gibbons1993; Raymond et al., Reference Raymond, Pontier, Dufour and Møller1996; Faurie & Raymond, Reference Faurie and Raymond2013). That is, the rarity advantage explains why left-handedness is present, and the basic disadvantage explains why its frequency is not at 50%. Indirect evidence in support of the combat hypothesis includes higher incidence of left-handers among elite athletes in interactive sports, e.g. tennis, fencing and baseball (Wood & Aggleton, Reference Wood and Aggleton1989; Goldstein & Young, Reference Goldstein and Young1996; Raymond et al., Reference Raymond, Pontier, Dufour and Møller1996; Loffing, Reference Loffing2017), although the link between left-handedness and reproductive success remains obscure (Groothuis et al., Reference Groothuis, McManus, Schaafsma and Geuze2013).

These game-theoretic hypotheses centre upon social interaction, whereby an individual's phenotype has an impact upon the fitness of others (the individual's ‘social partners’). Kin selection – the part of natural selection that arises when individuals socially interact with their genetic relatives – plays a major role in the evolution of social interactions across the tree of life (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1964; Frank, Reference Frank1998; West et al., Reference West, Griffin and Gardner2007a). In addition to influencing the overall incidence of traits within and across populations (Sachs et al., Reference Sachs, Mueller, Wilcox and Bull2004; West et al., Reference West, Griffin and Gardner2007a), kin selection can explain differences in trait levels between different individuals – such as sex differences (West et al., Reference West, Griffin and Gardner2007a; Leedale et al., Reference Leedale, Sharp, Simeoni, Robinson and Hatchwell2018) – and also modulate evolutionary ‘conflicts of interest’ within families and even within individual genomes – resulting in the evolution of parental genetic effects (Wolf et al., Reference Wolf, Cheverud, Moore and Wade1998; Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Komdeur and Burke2004; Kuijper & Johnstone, Reference Kuijper and Johnstone2016) and parent-of-origin effects, e.g. genomic imprinting (Haig, Reference Haig2002; Wilkins & Úbeda, Reference Wilkins, Úbeda, Cheng and Blumenthal2011). Yet theoretical analyses of the evolution of human handedness have so far failed to consider the possible modulating role of kin selection.

Here we undertake a theoretical investigation of how kin selection may shape the biology of human handedness. First, we show that, at evolutionary equilibrium, left-handedness may be classified either as a ‘selfish’ or an ‘altruistic’ trait, depending on its fitness consequences for the individual and for the individual's social partners, and that the direction of the modulating effect of genetic relatedness depends on which of these two situations applies. Second, we explore how demographic processes such as dispersal modulate the population level of left-handedness at evolutionary equilibrium, via their impact on the degree of genetic relatedness between social partners. Third, we investigate the consequences of sex-biased dispersal, and associated sex differences in an individual's relatedness to social partners, for the evolution of sex differences in left-handedness. Fourth, we determine the consequences of extending genetic control of handedness to the individual's parents, resulting in parent–offspring conflict and sexual conflict and the evolution of parental genetic effects in relation to human handedness. Fifth, we descend to the level of individual genes and investigate the scope for intragenomic conflict between maternal-origin vs. paternal-origin genes and the resulting evolution of parent-of-origin effects – including genomic imprinting – in relation to human handedness.

We conduct our theoretical investigation by means of a qualitative exploration of the logic of kin selection as it applies to left-handedness, and we complement this conceptual approach with an explicitly mathematical illustrative analysis presented in the Supplementary Material. For the purpose of illustration and concreteness, in each case we derive predictions for ‘within-group combat’ and ‘between-group combat’ game-theoretic scenarios, but our analysis applies to any scenario in which an individual's handedness has an impact upon their own reproductive success and that of their genetic relatives. We allow for handedness to be a highly polygenic trait and, although for ease of conceptualisation we will refer to handedness in a binary way, our analysis readily accommodates a spectrum of handedness. Indeed, in contrast to previous studies which have focused on the consequences of these different genetic architectures (McManus & Bryden, Reference McManus, Bryden, Rapin and Segalowitz1992; McManus et al., Reference McManus, Davison and Armour2013; McManus, Reference McManus2022), our focus is on how natural selection shapes the handedness phenotype. More generally, rather than giving post hoc explanations for existing observations, our major aim is to provide a priori, testable, comparative predictions to motivate new empirical research avenues and to develop a theoretical framework within which the results of future analyses can be conceptualised.

2. Results

(a) Kin selection and human handedness

Natural selection adapts individuals as if for the purpose of passing on their alleles to future generations (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1964; Grafen, Reference Grafen2006; West & Gardner, Reference West and Gardner2013). There are two basic routes through which individuals can accomplish this: first, by promoting their own reproductive success (direct fitness); and second, by promoting the reproductive success of their genetic relatives, who tend to share alleles in common (indirect fitness) (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1964). The part of natural selection that is driven by the impact of individuals on their genetic relatives defines ‘kin selection’ (Frank, Reference Frank1998). According to Hamilton's (Reference Hamilton1964) rule, a behaviour that incurs a fitness cost c for the actor can nevertheless be ‘favoured’ by natural selection if it provides a sufficiently large fitness benefit b to a sufficiently closely related recipient, where the kin-selection coefficient of relatedness r describes the statistical association between their genetical traits (i.e. r = 1/2 for full siblings, r = 1/4 for half-siblings, r = 1/8 for first cousins, and so on, in a simple outbred population setting; Frank Reference Frank1998). Specifically, the behaviour is favoured if −c + rb > 0. More generally, we can define four types of social behaviour, according to the sign of the fitness effects: traits incurring a cost for the actor and yielding a benefit for the recipient (c > 0 and b > 0) are ‘altruistic’; traits yielding a benefit for the actor and incurring a cost for the recipient (c < 0 and b < 0) are ‘selfish’; traits yielding a benefit for both parties (c < 0 and b > 0) are ‘mutually beneficial’; and traits incurring a cost for both parties (c > 0 and b < 0) are ‘spiteful’ (Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1964; West et al., Reference West, Griffin and Gardner2007b).

At evolutionary equilibrium, where natural selection favours neither an increase nor a decrease in the trait (−c + rb  = 0), then so long as relatedness is positive (r > 0) the trait must either be marginally altruistic (c > 0 and b > 0) or marginally selfish (c < 0 and b < 0) (Hitchcock et al., Reference Hitchcock, Paracchini and Gardner2019). Accordingly, if natural selection acts in a negative frequency-dependent way in relation to human handedness – as suggested by the game-theoretic models (Ghirlanda & Vallortigara, Reference Ghirlanda and Vallortigara2004; Ghirlanda et al., Reference Ghirlanda, Frasnelli and Vallortigara2009; Abrams & Panaggio, Reference Abrams and Panaggio2012) – such that it favours an increase in the incidence of left-handedness when this has dropped below a threshold level and favours a decrease in left-handedness when it has exceeded the threshold, then evolutionary equilibrium is attained when the incidence of left-handedness is at the threshold, and at this point left-handedness is either marginally altruistic or marginally selfish. If left-handedness is marginally altruistic then a higher degree of genetic relatedness between actor and recipient is expected to be associated with a higher incidence of left-handedness at the evolutionary equilibrium, whereas if left-handedness is marginally selfish then a higher degree of genetic relatedness is expected to be associated with a lower incidence of left-handedness.

Taking the combat hypothesis as a purely illustrative example, if we imagine that combat occurs mainly within human groups – i.e. between individuals who are members of the same community and are somewhat genetically related to each other – then the indirect-fitness consequences of enjoying a surprise advantage in combat owing to left-handedness are expected to be negative (because the opponent, who loses out, is a genetic relative). Hence, at equilibrium, this indirect-fitness cost is expected to be exactly balanced by a direct-fitness benefit (owing to improved success in combat outweighing the basic disadvantage of left-handedness). In this scenario, left-handedness is a marginally selfish trait, and kin selection disfavours left-handedness, such that a higher level of relatedness is expected to be associated with a lower incidence of left-handedness (Figure 1 and S3a). Alternatively, if combat mainly occurs between non-relatives in an intergroup-warfare context in which success in combat is associated with a positive indirect-fitness effect owing to the benefits that accrue to the individual's genetically related groupmates, then at equilibrium this is expected to be exactly balanced by a direct-fitness cost (owing to the basic disadvantage of left-handedness failing to outweigh the improved success in combat). In this alternative scenario, left-handedness is a marginally altruistic trait, and kin selection favours left-handedness, such that a higher level of relatedness is expected to be associated with a higher incidence of left-handedness (Figure 1 and S3a).

Figure 1. Comparative predictions as to how dispersal affects relatedness between social partners and hence the evolutionarily favoured incidence of left-handedness, depending on the type of social interactions: within-group (selfish) or between-group (altruistic) combat. Lower dispersal leads to higher relatedness, hence low dispersal favours a lower incidence of left-handedness when it is selfish and a higher incidence of left-handedness when it is altruistic. Sex-biased dispersal creates asymmetry in relatedness and hence favours a sex difference in left-handedness.

Genetic relatedness will usually depend on the ecology and demography of the population, and so the above kin-selection logic also yields predictions as to how population processes relate to the evolutionarily favoured incidence of left-handedness. As a concrete example, we consider the rate of dispersal. If individuals have a higher tendency to disperse away from their place of origin and pursue reproductive opportunities within other groups, then this is expected to result in lower relatedness among groupmates. Accordingly, if left-handedness is marginally selfish – as, for example, in the within-group combat scenario – then as the rate of dispersal increases, the degree of relatedness decreases, and hence the evolutionarily favoured level of left-handedness is expected to increase (Figure 1 and S3a). And, in contrast, if left-handedness is marginally altruistic – as, for example, in the between-group combat scenario – then as the rate of dispersal increases, the degree of relatedness decreases, and hence the level of left-handedness is expected to decrease (Figure 1 and S3a). These predictions relate to contemporary and/or historical between-population comparisons and also, potentially, to the dynamics of handedness within a single population across evolutionary timescales in responses to demographic change (see Discussion).

(b) Sex differences in human handedness

Above, we have shown that the average genetic relatedness between social partners – and the population processes that modulate this – is expected to influence the evolutionarily favoured incidence of left-handedness at a population level. Similarly, inter-individual differences in relatedness to one's social partners – and the population processes responsible for such variation – are expected to drive differences in levels of left-handedness among different subdivisions of the population. In particular, sex-specific demographic processes – such as sex-biased dispersal – may result in a sex difference in the relatedness of social partners, which may therefore favour a sex difference in the incidence of left-handedness. For example, all else being equal, female-biased dispersal is expected to result in relatedness between social partners being lower for woman than for men. Hence, all else being equal, a higher level of left-handedness would be favoured among women than among men if left-handedness is marginally selfish (such as in the within-group combat scenario) and a higher level of left-handedness would be favoured among men than among women if left-handedness is marginally altruistic (such as in the between-group combat scenario). The opposite pattern is expected under male-biased dispersal (Figure 1 and S3b).

In addition to differences in relatedness, the sexes might also differ with respect to the fitness consequences – that is, the benefits and costs – associated with left-handedness. Such fitness differences would also be expected to modulate sex differences in the incidence of left-handedness. For example, if the frequency-dependent advantage of left-handedness when rare applies more strongly to men than to women – as would be expected in the combat scenarios if men engage in combat more frequently than do women and/or if men have more to gain from winning in combat in terms of enhanced reproductive success (Micheletti et al., Reference Micheletti, Ruxton and Gardner2018) – then, all else being equal, the incidence of left-handedness is expected to be higher among men than among women. Note that we need not expect each sex's observed level of left-handedness to correspond exactly to its adaptive optimum; to the extent that the same genes underpin left-handedness in both sexes, sexual antagonism might keep both sexes from attaining their respective optima (Grafen, Reference Grafen2006). More generally, these sex-difference results concern adaptive evolution, and are based upon considerations of female vs. male fitness optima. Accordingly, they neglect non-adaptive sex differences arising, for example, from a greater vulnerability of males to developmental perturbation away from a default phenotype, which has been reported in disorders including ASD (Antaki et al., Reference Antaki, Guevara, Maihofer, Klein, Gujral, Grove and Sebat2022). Such non-adaptive mechanisms could offer alternative explanations for the higher incidence of left-handedness among males (see Discussion).

(c) Parental genetic effects in human handedness

Above, we have shown how the evolutionarily favoured level of left-handedness may be modulated by the evolutionary value that individuals place upon the reproductive success of social partners relative to their own reproductive success. This assumes that an individual's own genotype controls the handedness phenotype. If, instead, the handedness phenotype were controlled by the parental genotype – i.e. a ‘parental genetic effect’ (Trivers, Reference Trivers and Campbell1972, Reference Trivers1974; Wilson, Reference Wilson1980) – then we might expect the evolutionarily favoured incidence of left-handedness to reflect the relatedness valuations made by the individual's parents. More generally, if an individual's predisposition to left-handedness is modulated in part by the individual's own genotype and also in part by the genotypes of the individual's parents then we might expect an evolutionary ‘conflict of interests’ – and associated evolutionary arms race – between parent and offspring (Trivers, Reference Trivers1974), and between the parents themselves (Trivers, Reference Trivers and Campbell1972), as each party is favoured to move the handedness phenotype closer to their own fitness optimum.

If an individual's handedness phenotype represents a trade-off between the individual's own reproductive success and the reproductive success of the individual's groupmates, then in general terms we expect the individual's parents to favour a balance that is relatively biased towards the groupmates’ reproductive interests and the individual to favour a balance that is relatively biased towards their own reproductive interests, so long as there is relatedness among groupmates (see Supplementary Material §§S1.7 and S2.5 for details). This owes to individuals being genetically identical to themselves and only somewhat genetically related to their offspring. Accordingly, if left-handedness is a marginally selfish trait (as in the illustrative within-group combat scenario) then we expect parents to favour a lower predisposition for left-handedness in their offspring than their offspring would themselves favour. And if left-handedness is a marginally altruistic trait (as in the illustrative between-group combat scenario) then we expect parents to favour a higher predisposition for left-handedness in their offspring than their offspring would themselves favour (Figure 1 and S3c).

Moreover, although both parents are equally related to their offspring they may be differentially related to their offspring's social partners, so that mothers and fathers may favour different dispositions for left-handedness among their offspring. For example, under female-biased dispersal, mothers are expected to be less related to their offspring's social partners than are fathers, and hence more inclined to their offspring having a disposition for left-handedness if this is a marginally selfish trait (as in the illustrative within-group combat scenario) and less inclined to their offspring having a disposition for left-handedness if this is a marginally altruistic trait (as in the illustrative between-group combat scenario). The opposite set of outcomes is expected under male-biased dispersal (Figure S5). Accordingly, considerations of patterns of relatedness and concomitant kin selection yield predictions as to parental genetic effects – including maternal genetic effects and paternal genetic effects – working at cross purposes with the individual's own genome, as well as with each other, in relation to the individual's handedness phenotype.

(d) Parent-of-origin effects in human handedness

Above, we have shown that sex-specific demography – such as sex-biased dispersal – may generate differences in the relatedness valuations made by mothers and fathers regarding the reproductive success of their offspring vs. their offspring's social partners, resulting in the evolution of parental genetic effects in relation to handedness. Similarly, this relatedness asymmetry can also extend into the offspring's own genome and ignite an evolutionary conflict of interests between the individual's own maternal-origin vs. paternal-origin genes. Such intragenomic conflict in relation to other social traits has been suggested to drive the evolution of parent-of-origin specific genetic effects, including genomic imprinting (Haig, Reference Haig2002; Gardner & Úbeda, Reference Gardner and Úbeda2017) – and induce vulnerability to a number of associated developmental disorders, e.g. Silver–Russell syndrome and Beckwith–Wiedemann syndrome (Crespi, Reference Crespi2011; Wilkins & Úbeda, Reference Wilkins, Úbeda, Cheng and Blumenthal2011).

For example, if left-handedness is marginally selfish (such as in the within-group combat scenario) then under female-biased dispersal the relatedness between social partners through maternal-origin genes – all else being equal – will be lower than the relatedness through paternal-origin genes, and hence maternal-origin genes are expected to favour a higher level of left-handedness than are paternal-origin genes (Figures 2 and S4). Under male-biased dispersal relatedness will be higher through maternal-origin genes than through paternal-origin genes, and hence maternal-origin genes are expected to favour a lower level of left-handedness than are paternal-origin genes (Figures 2 and S4). Conversely, when left-handedness is marginally altruistic (such as in the between-group combat scenario) then under female-biased dispersal maternal-origin genes are expected to favour a lower level of left-handedness than are paternal-origin genes, whereas under male-biased dispersal maternal-origin genes are expected to favour a higher level of left-handedness than are paternal-origin genes (Figure 2 and S4).

Figure 2. Sex differences, parental genetic effects and parent-of-origin effects at loci underpinning left-handedness. Sex-biased expression of genes can lead to a sex difference in incidence of left-handedness. Expression of parents’ genes can lead to parental genetic effects in relation to left-handedness. Parent-of-origin-specific gene expression can lead to parent-of-origin effects in relation to left-handedness.

According to the kinship theory of genomic imprinting (Haig, Reference Haig2002), this form of intragenomic conflict will typically lead to one of the copies of the gene being silenced. Specifically, according to the ‘loudest voice prevails’ principle (Haig, Reference Haig2002), the two copies of the gene at the affected locus are favoured to adjust their level of expression in opposite directions, such that the one favouring a higher level of left-handedness will act to increase the level of left-handedness while the one favouring a lower level of left-handedness will act to decrease the level of left-handedness, until the gene being favoured to decrease its expression falls silent. At a locus for which an increase in gene expression results in an increase in the level of left-handedness – a ‘left-handedness promoter’ locus – it is the gene that favours a higher level of left-handedness that is expected to remain expressed while the gene that favours a lower level of left-handedness is silenced. And at a locus for which an increase in gene expression results in a decrease in the level of left-handedness – a ‘left-handedness inhibitor’ locus – it is the gene that favours a lower level of left-handedness that is expected to remain expressed while the gene that favours a higher level of left-handedness is silenced. Accordingly, the function of the gene product determines the direction of imprint.

For example, if left-handedness is marginally selfish (e.g. within-group combat), then under female-biased dispersal we expect left-handedness promoters to be maternally expressed and paternally silenced and left-handedness inhibitors to be maternally silenced and paternally expressed, and under male-biased dispersal left-handedness promoters are expected to be maternally silenced and paternally expressed and left-handedness inhibitors to be maternally expressed and paternally silenced. However if left-handedness is marginally altruistic (e.g. between-group combat), then under female-biased dispersal we expect left-handedness promoters to be maternally silenced and paternally expressed and left-handedness inhibitors to be maternally expressed and paternally silenced; and under male-biased dispersal left-handedness promoters are expected to be maternally expressed and paternally silenced and left-handedness inhibitors to be maternally silenced and paternally expressed (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Predictions as to how dispersal pattern and gene function modulate the pattern of genomic imprinting. See Supplementary Material Figure S7 for additional predictions concerning the phenotypic consequences of gene deletions, gene duplications, epimutations and uniparental disomies.

3. Discussion

Although game theoretic attempts to explain the evolutionary maintenance of a substantial minority of left-handed individuals in human population fundamentally hinge upon social interaction, and although kin selection is a fundamental driver of social evolution, the possible role for kin selection in modulating the evolution of human handedness has previously been neglected. We have shown how patterns of genetic relatedness – and the demographic processes underpinning these – are expected to shape patterns of human handedness. Specifically, our kin-selection analyses show that: (1) relatedness between social partners – modulated by population processes such as dispersal – is expected to influence the population level of left-handedness in a direction that depends upon whether left-handedness is marginally selfish (as in our illustrative within-group combat scenario) vs. marginally altruistic (as in our illustrative between-group combat scenario); (2) sex-specific demography – such as sex-biased dispersal – can result in sex differences in relatedness to one's social partners, which may go some way to explaining sex differences in incidence of left-handedness; (3) differences in relatedness valuations made by different family members can ignite conflicts of interest between parents and offspring and between an individual's mother and father over their handedness phenotype, driving the evolution of parental genetic effects; and (4) such relatedness differences may even ignite evolutionary conflicts of interest within the individual's own genome, with maternal-origin and paternal-origin genes favouring different handedness phenotypes, which is expected to drive the evolution of parent-of-origin effects – such as ‘genomic imprinting’ – in relation to handedness. By pitching our analysis in a qualitative way we achieve a considerable degree of generality, with our comparative predictions applying across a diversity of genetic architectures. Although factors such as the number of loci underpinning the left-handedness phenotype and the size of human groups will clearly make a quantitative impact on evolutionary outcomes, the basic logic of kin selection applies as generally as the concept of natural selection itself (Gardner et al., Reference Gardner, West and Wild2011).

Our analyses have shown that the degree of genetic relatedness between social partners is expected to modulate the evolutionary equilibrium frequency of left-handedness in the population, with higher relatedness being associated with a lower level of left-handedness when left-handedness tends to benefit the individual at the expense of social partners (selfishness) and a higher level of left-handedness when left-handedness tends to benefit social partners at the expense of the individual (altruism). The degree of relatedness is itself expected to depend on ecological and demographic parameters such as rate of dispersal, with higher dispersal of individuals tending to reduce the extent of genetic relatedness between social partners. At a comparative level, variation in ecological and demographic parameters between different human populations could potentially explain between-population differences in incidence of left-handedness. Variation in ecological and demographic parameters within a single human population over time might also explain temporal differences in the incidence of left-handers, but only insofar as the variation in ecology and demography occurs over a relatively long timescale and the evolutionary fine-tuning of handedness occurs over a relatively short timescale – i.e. so that there is time for adaptation to current conditions to occur before those conditions change. Our analysis offers little quantitative guidance as to the relevant timescales, but the population bias towards right-handedness does appear to have already been in place when hominin lineages diverged from the great apes around 7 million years ago (Uomini & Ruck, Reference Uomini, Ruck, Forrester, Hopkins, Hudry and Lindell2018; Papadatou-Pastou et al., Reference Papadatou-Pastou, Ntolka, Schmitz, Martin, Munafò, Ocklenburg and Paracchini2020).

Our analysis also shows that sex-specific selection can give rise to sex differences in handedness. We have shown how sex-specific demographies – such as sex-biased dispersal – may lead to sex differences in relatedness between social partners and hence sex-differences in the level of left-handedness favoured by females vs. males. Whether humans have been characterised by sex-biased dispersal in our evolutionary past, and in which direction, remains a controversial topic: the traditional view is that human dispersal has been female-biased (Ember, Reference Ember1975), but evidence has also been marshalled in support of dispersal having been unbiased or mixed (Marlowe, Reference Marlowe2004). Our use of sex-biased dispersal is merely as an illustration, and the results extend more generally to any ecological and demographic factors that result in sex-differences in relatedness to one's social partners – such as patterns of inbreeding (Wilkins & Haig, Reference Wilkins and Haig2003). In addition to relatedness, our analysis has emphasised that sex difference in left-handedness might also reflect sex differences in the costs and/or benefits of left-handedness. For example, men are generally understood to engage in – and to benefit from winning – combat more than do women (Micheletti et al., Reference Micheletti, Ruxton and Gardner2018), which could explain a higher incidence in left-handedness on account of a surprise advantage in combat settings. The higher incidence of left-handedness in males could also arise for non-adaptive reasons. One possibility is sexually differential liability thresholds (Khramtsova et al., Reference Khramtsova, Davis and Stranger2019; Merikangas & Almasy, Reference Merikangas and Almasy2020; Antaki et al., Reference Antaki, Guevara, Maihofer, Klein, Gujral, Grove and Sebat2022), whereby the number of risk alleles required for an individual to exhibit a minority phenotype is greater for females than males, i.e. the ‘female buffering’ effect. Another is sex-linked inheritance (McManus & Bryden, Reference McManus, Bryden, Rapin and Segalowitz1992; Jones & Martin, Reference Jones and Martin2010; but see McManus, Reference McManus2010), although X-linked left-handedness genes have not been found in the recent sex-stratified GWAS analysis (Cuellar-Partida et al., Reference Cuellar-Partida, Tung, Eriksson, Albrecht, Aliev, Andreassen and Medland2021).

Our analysis shows the potential for parental genetic effects to occur in relation to left-handedness, such that alleles carried by a parent exert an influence on their offspring's handedness phenotype, irrespective of whether the offspring carries the same alleles. These parental genetic effects are expected to arise evolutionarily as a consequence of parents having different interests regarding their offspring's handedness phenotype, and our analysis yields predictions as to patterns of such genetic effects depending on the sex of parent and offspring (see Supplementary Material §§S1.7 and S2.5). Note that we have taken a ‘battleground’ approach (Godfray, Reference Godfray1995) that enables characterisation of the existence and direction of this parent–offspring conflict. A further ‘resolution’ model (Godfray, Reference Godfray1995) would be required to explore more fully the resulting arms race and to determine its evolutionary endpoint – this represents an avenue for future theoretical analysis. Schmitz et al.'s (Reference Schmitz, Zheng, Lui, McBride, Ho and Paracchini2022) genomic analyses suggest the existence of parental effects in relation to hand preference, and stronger maternal effects than paternal effects in another multidimensional laterality trait – footedness, when controlling for possible non-paternity cases. However, despite the well-known difference in association between offspring handedness and maternal vs. paternal handedness (Jordan, Reference Jordan1911; Ramaley, Reference Ramaley1913), the possibility of parental genetic effects has been neither confirmed nor excluded. Parental genetic effects have been suggested to arise in neurodevelopmental disorders associated with handedness, such as maternal genetic effects in relation to loci associated with ASD – potential loci include SHANK3 on chromosome 22 and WBSCR17 on chromosome 7q11 – but these findings have not been replicated (Connolly et al., Reference Connolly, Anney, Gallagher and Heron2017). The predictions of our analysis therefore offer a new perspective for understanding the role of parental genetic effects in neurodevelopmental disorders.

Finally, our analysis also shows maternal-origin vs. paternal-origin genes within an individual's own genome may come into conflict in relation to their carrier's handedness phenotype, and how this conflict may lead to the evolution of parent-of-origin-specific gene expression. Genomic imprinting is associated with a variety of debilitating disorders, with parent-of-origin-specific clinical effects and non-standard patterns of inheritance that are often predictable in light of the kinship theory (Wilkins & Úbeda, Reference Wilkins, Úbeda, Cheng and Blumenthal2011). Our results concerning patterns of imprinting allow us to make predictions as to the effects of a range of different mutational and epimutational perturbations of imprinted loci affecting handedness (Figure S7). For example, a gene deletion at an imprinted locus is expected to have no impact on the phenotype if the gene was to be silenced anyway, but it is expected to have a potentially major impact upon the phenotype if it was to be expressed such that no functional gene product at all will derive from the affected locus (Figure S7). Such effects might often be lethal insofar as they involve disruption to early stages of brain development when left–right asymmetry is usually established. These predictions could potentially enhance our understanding of various neurodevelopmental disorders associated with handedness. A range of neurodevelopmental conditions are associated with an elevated level of left (or non-right) handedness, e.g. dyslexia or developmental language disorders (Abbondanza et al., Reference Abbondanza, Dale, Wang, Hayiou-Thomas, Toseeb, Koomar and Paracchini2023; Packheiser et al., Reference Packheiser, Papadatou-Pastou, Koufaki, Paracchini, Stein, Schmitz and Ocklenburg2023), schizophrenia (Hirnstein & Hugdahl, Reference Hirnstein and Hugdahl2014) and ASD (Markou et al., Reference Markou, Ahtam and Papadatou-Pastou2017). Several loci that are associated with ASD have been suggested to have parent-of-origin effects – with maternally over-expressed components including a region between LOC391642 and LOC645641 on chromosome 4 and the LRRC16A gene on chromosome 6, and paternally over-transmitted genes including the STPG2 gene on chromosome 4 and the TBC1D4 gene on chromosome 13 – but these findings are not replicated (Connolly et al., Reference Connolly, Anney, Gallagher and Heron2017). Considering novel parent-of-origin effects on complex traits have recently been reported with larger samples and new methods such as the probabilistic approach (Hofmeister et al., Reference Hofmeister, Rubinacci, Ribeiro, Buil, Kutalik and Delaneau2022), we suggest that parent-of-origin effects might be more widespread than anticipated.

In relation to parent-of-origin effects, we have focused on the ‘loudest voice prevails’ model of the evolution of genomic imprinting (Haig, Reference Haig2002), which applies here to loci whereby a greater level of gene expression either increases (‘left-handedness promoter’) or decreases (‘left-handedness inhibitor’) the likelihood of the individual exhibiting left-handedness. For loci at which an intermediate level of gene expression yields a right-handed phenotype and deviations in gene expression (in either direction) are liable to yield a left-handed phenotype – in line with the developmental instability hypothesis of handedness (Yeo & Gangestad, Reference Yeo and Gangestad1993) – we might instead expect the gene that favours a greater incidence of left-handedness to exhibit more stochastic expression, i.e. the ‘chaotic voice prevails’ logic of Úbeda et al. (Reference Úbeda, Ohtsuki and Gardner2014). More generally, the kinship theory of genomic imprinting, as it currently stands, predicts genomic imprinting of all loci that experience parent-of-origin conflict, irrespective of the intensity of the conflict. Yet empirical studies suggest that genomic imprinting is quite rare – around 1% of genes in the human genome (Luedi et al., Reference Luedi, Dietrich, Weidman, Bosko, Jirtle and Hartemink2007). Clearly, there are additional requirements for a locus to evolve imprinting, and our hope is that through confronting these quite speculative theoretical predictions with empirical data, the theory can be further refined.

The strong population bias in favour of one sidedness type while the other remains a substantial minority appears to be an exclusively human phenomenon. However, lateralisation itself has a taxonomically widespread occurrence. The historical view that lateralisation is unique in humans was disputed in 1970s during a renaissance of lateralisation studies (Güntürkün et al., Reference Güntürkün, Ströckens and Ocklenburg2020), and since then lateralisation has been reported across the animal kingdom (Rogers, Reference Rogers1980; Ocklenburg & Güntürkün, Reference Ocklenburg and Güntürkün2012). Some species show lateralisation only at individual level, such as paw preference in rodents (Manns et al., Reference Manns, Basbasse, Freund and Ocklenburg2021), and in cats and dogs (Ocklenburg et al., Reference Ocklenburg, Isparta, Peterburs and Papadatou-Pastou2019), turning preferences in insects (Girling et al., Reference Girling, Hassall and Turner2007) and in fishes (Vallortigara & Rogers, Reference Vallortigara and Rogers2005), and eye preference in octopuses (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Kuba and Meisel2004). While lateralisation at population level seems to be relatively rarer (Vallortigara & Rogers, Reference Vallortigara and Rogers2005; Meguerditchian et al., Reference Meguerditchian, Vauclair and Hopkins2013), supporting evidence has steadily accumulated from studies of indoor/captive individuals and from the field (Forrester et al., Reference Forrester, Quaresmini, Leavens, Mareschal and Thomas2013), including hand preference in non-human primates (Caspar et al., Reference Caspar, Pallasdies, Mader, Sartorelli and Begall2022), foot (Rogers, Reference Rogers1980) and eye preferences (Brown & Magat, Reference Brown and Magat2011) in Australian parrots, left-leg preference for prey touching in spitting spiders (Ades & Ramires, Reference Ades and Ramires2002), right-leg preference in kicking undesirable males by female mosquitoes (Benelli et al., Reference Benelli, Romano, Messing and Canale2015), turning bias in ants (Hunt et al., Reference Hunt, O'Shea-Wheller, Albery, Bridger, Gumn and Franks2014) and a higher frequency of being attacked on the right in trilobites (Babcock, Reference Babcock1993).

Ghirlanda et al. (Reference Ghirlanda, Frasnelli and Vallortigara2009) argued that population-level brain lateralisation can occur in two steps: first, individuals should benefit from increased cognitive efficiency by being lateralised in either direction (Güntürkün et al., Reference Güntürkün, Diekamp, Manns, Nottelmann, Prior, Schwarz and Skiba2000; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Zucca and Vallortigara2004; Vallortigara & Rogers, Reference Vallortigara and Rogers2005); second, a population-level bias in preference to one direction should bring additional benefits, e.g. the majority of individuals moving in the same direction creates a dilution effect which reduces the chances of being eaten by predators (Ghirlanda & Vallortigara, Reference Ghirlanda and Vallortigara2004; Vallortigara & Rogers, Reference Vallortigara and Rogers2005), while the minority may also enjoy a surprise advantage if predators learn which direction the majority of their prey prefer (Ghirlanda & Vallortigara, Reference Ghirlanda and Vallortigara2004). Although the additional benefits were first discussed in relation to prey–predator interactions, similar benefits might also emerge from intraspecific interactions. Ghirlanda et al. (Reference Ghirlanda, Frasnelli and Vallortigara2009) and Abrams & Panaggio (Reference Abrams and Panaggio2012) have suggested that the population balance of right-handers vs. left-handers reflects the relative prevalence of cooperative vs. competitive interactions, with cooperative interactions promoting the fitness of the majority handedness type and competitive interactions promoting the fitness of the minority handedness type. All of these game theoretical models focus on social interactions, which are very likely to be mediated by genetic relatedness as shown in general cases of social evolution, yet our investigation is the first time that kin selection has been considered in human handedness. The predicted effect of relatedness on the evolution of handedness crucially depends on whether left-handedness is marginally altruistic or selfish. Although current data are not sufficient for answering that question, our analyses provide a framework within which future data can be motivated and conceptualised.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2024.24

Acknowledgements

We thank Chedhawat Chokechaipaisarn, Mauricio González-Forero, Thomas Hitchcock, Carolin Kosiol, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Petri Rautiala, Graeme Ruxton, Kerstin Stucky and Kalyani Twyman, for helpful discussion. We also want to thank the editors and reviewers who genuinely made this paper a better contribution.

Author contributions

BD, SP and AG designed the study. BD, SP and AG wrote the manuscript. BD led the mathematical analysis.

Financial support

This work was supported by European Research Council (grant no. 771387) and Natural Environment Research Council (grant no. NE/K009524/1). BD was supported by a China Scholarship Council PhD studentship (202104910116).

Publishing ethics

The manuscript is our own original work, and does not duplicate any other previously published work. The manuscript has been submitted only to the journal – it is not under consideration, accepted for publication or in press elsewhere. All listed authors know of and agree to the manuscript being submitted to the journal. The manuscript contains nothing that is abusive, defamatory, fraudulent, illegal, libellous or obscene.

Conflict of interest

None.

Research transparency and reproducibility

All the equations and derivations can be found in our uploaded supplementary material.

Data availability statement

All the equations and derivations can be found in our uploaded supplementary material.

References

Abbondanza, F., Dale, P. S., Wang, C. A., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Toseeb, U., Koomar, T. S., …, Paracchini, S. (2023). Language and reading impairments are associated with increased prevalence of non-right-handedness. Child Development, 94(4), 970984. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13914CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abrams, D. M., & Panaggio, M. J. (2012). A model balancing cooperation and competition can explain our right-handed world and the dominance of left-handed athletes. Journal of The Royal Society Interface, 9(75), 27182722. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2012.0211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ades, C., & Ramires, E. N. (2002). Asymmetry of leg use during prey handling in the spider Scytodes globula (Scytodidae). Journal of Insect Behavior, 15(4), 563570. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016337418472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antaki, D., Guevara, J., Maihofer, A. X., Klein, M., Gujral, M., Grove, J., …, Sebat, J. (2022). A phenotypic spectrum of autism is attributable to the combined effects of rare variants, polygenic risk and sex. Nature Genetics, 54(9), 12841292. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01064-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babcock, L. E. (1993). Trilobite malformations and the fossil record of behavioral asymmetry. Journal of Paleontology, 67(2), 217229. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022336000032145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benelli, G., Romano, D., Messing, R. H., & Canale, A. (2015). First report of behavioural lateralisation in mosquitoes: Right-biased kicking behaviour against males in females of the Asian tiger mosquito, Aedes albopictus. Parasitology Research, 114(4), 16131617. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00436-015-4351-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C., & Magat, M. (2011). Cerebral lateralization determines hand preferences in Australian parrots. Biology Letters, 7(4), 496498. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2010.1121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. A., Kuba, M. J., & Meisel, D. V. (2004). Lateralized eye use in Octopus vulgaris shows antisymmetrical distribution. Animal Behaviour, 68(5), 11071114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.11.027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspar, K. R., Pallasdies, F., Mader, L., Sartorelli, H., & Begall, S. (2022). The evolution and biological correlates of hand preferences in anthropoid primates. eLife, 11, e77875. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.77875CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connolly, S., Anney, R., Gallagher, L., & Heron, E. A. (2017). A genome-wide investigation into parent-of-origin effects in autism spectrum disorder identifies previously associated genes including SHANK3. European Journal of Human Genetics, 25(2), 234239. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejhg.2016.153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coren, S., & Porac, C. (1977). Fifty centuries of right-handedness: The historical record. Science, 198(4317), 631632. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.335510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crespi, B. J. (2011). The evolutionary biology of child health. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1711), 14411449. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuellar-Partida, G., Tung, J. Y., Eriksson, N., Albrecht, E., Aliev, F., Andreassen, O. A., …, Medland, S. E. (2021). Genome-wide association study identifies 48 common genetic variants associated with handedness. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(1), 5970. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00956-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ember, C. R. (1975). Residential variation among hunter–gatherers. Behavior Science Research, 10(3), 199227. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939717501000302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faurie, C., & Raymond, M. (2013). The fighting hypothesis as an evolutionary explanation for the handedness polymorphism in humans: Where are we? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1288, 110113. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12159CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forrester, G. S., Quaresmini, C., Leavens, D. A., Mareschal, D., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2013). Human handedness: An inherited evolutionary trait. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 200206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, S. A. (1998). Foundations of social evolution. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frayer, D. W., Lozano, M., Bermúdez de Castro, J. M., Carbonell, E., Arsuaga, J. L., Radovčić, J., …, Bondioli, L. (2012). More than 500,000 years of right-handedness in Europe. Laterality, 17(1), 5169. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.529451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, A., & Úbeda, F. (2017). The meaning of intragenomic conflict. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1(12), 18071815. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-017-0354-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, A., West, S. A., & Wild, G. (2011). The genetical theory of kin selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24(5), 10201043. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02236.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghirlanda, S., & Vallortigara, G. (2004). The evolution of brain lateralization: A game-theoretical analysis of population structure. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(1541), 853857. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2669CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghirlanda, S., Frasnelli, E., & Vallortigara, G. (2009). Intraspecific competition and coordination in the evolution of lateralization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1519), 861866. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0227CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbons, A. (1993). Evolutionists take the long view on sex and violence. Science, 261(5124), 987988. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.8351524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girling, R., Hassall, M., & Turner, J. (2007). Do turning biases by the 7-spot ladybird, Coccinella septempunctata, increase their foraging efficiency? Behaviour, 144(2), 143163. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853907779947337Google Scholar
Godfray, H. C. J. (1995). Evolutionary theory of parent–offspring conflict. Nature, 376(6536), 133138. https://doi.org/10.1038/376133a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, S. R., & Young, C. A. (1996). ‘Evolutionary’ stable strategy of handedness in major league baseball. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110(2), 164169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.110.2.164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grafen, A. (2006). Optimization of inclusive fitness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 238(3), 541563. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.06.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groothuis, T. G. G., McManus, I. C., Schaafsma, S. M., & Geuze, R. H. (2013). The fighting hypothesis in combat: How well does the fighting hypothesis explain human left-handed minorities? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1288(1), 100109. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Güntürkün, O., Diekamp, B., Manns, M., Nottelmann, F., Prior, H., Schwarz, A., & Skiba, M. (2000). Asymmetry pays: visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Current Biology, 10(17), 10791081. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00671-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Güntürkün, O., Ströckens, F., & Ocklenburg, S. (2020). Brain lateralization: A comparative perspective. Physiological Reviews, 100(3), 10191063. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00006.2019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haig, D. (2002). Genomic imprinting and kinship. Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I & II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 152. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirnstein, M., & Hugdahl, K. (2014). Excess of non-right-handedness in schizophrenia: Meta-analysis of gender effects and potential biases in handedness assessment. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 205(4), 260267. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.137349CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitchcock, T. J., Paracchini, S., & Gardner, A. (2019). Genomic imprinting as a window into human language evolution. Bioessays, 41(6), 1800212. https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.201800212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmeister, R. J., Rubinacci, S., Ribeiro, D. M., Buil, A., Kutalik, Z., & Delaneau, O. (2022). Parent-of-origin inference for biobanks. Nature Communications, 13(1), 6668. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-34383-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, E. R., O'Shea-Wheller, T., Albery, G. F., Bridger, T. H., Gumn, M., & Franks, N. R. (2014). Ants show a leftward turning bias when exploring unknown nest sites. Biology Letters, 10(12), 20140945. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0945CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, G. V., & Martin, M. (2010). Language dominance, handedness and sex: Recessive X-linkage theory and test. Cortex, 46(6), 781786. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, H. E. (1911). The inheritance of left-handedness. The American Breeders Magazine, 2(1), 1929, 113–124.Google Scholar
Khramtsova, E. A., Davis, L. K., & Stranger, B. E. (2019). The role of sex in the genomics of human complex traits. Nature Reviews Genetics, 20(3), 173190. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41576-018-0083-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knecht, S., Dräger, B., Deppe, M., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Flöel, A., …, Henningsen, H. (2000). Handedness and hemispheric language dominance in healthy humans. Brain, 123(12), 25122518. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.12.2512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuijper, B., & Johnstone, R. A. (2016). Parental effects and the evolution of phenotypic memory. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 29(2), 265276. https://doi.org/10.1111/jeb.12778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leedale, A. E., Sharp, S. P., Simeoni, M., Robinson, E. J. H., & Hatchwell, B. J. (2018). Fine-scale genetic structure and helping decisions in a cooperatively breeding bird. Molecular Ecology, 27(7), 17141726. https://doi.org/10.1111/mec.14553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lien, Y.-J., Chen, W. J., Hsiao, P.-C., & Tsuang, H.-C. (2015). Estimation of heritability for varied indexes of handedness. Laterality, 20(4), 469482. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.1000920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loffing, F. (2017). Left-handedness and time pressure in elite interactive ball games. Biology Letters, 13(11), 20170446. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2017.0446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luedi, P. P., Dietrich, F. S., Weidman, J. R., Bosko, J. M., Jirtle, R. L., & Hartemink, A. J. (2007). Computational and experimental identification of novel human imprinted genes. Genome Research, 17(12), 17231730. https://doi.org/10.1101/gr.6584707CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manns, M., Basbasse, Y. E., Freund, N., & Ocklenburg, S. (2021). Paw preferences in mice and rats: Meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 127, 593606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markou, P., Ahtam, B., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2017). Elevated levels of atypical handedness in autism: Meta-analyses. Neuropsychology Review, 27(3), 258283. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-017-9354-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Marital residence among foragers. Current Anthropology, 45(2), 277284. https://doi.org/10.1086/382256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Jobard, G., Crivello, F., Joliot, M., Perchey, G., …, Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2014). Gaussian mixture modeling of hemispheric lateralization for language in a large sample of healthy individuals balanced for handedness. PLoS One, 9(6), e101165. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, I. C. (2009). The history and geography of human handedness. In Sommer, I. E. C. & Kahn, R. S. (Eds.), Language lateralization and psychosis (pp. 3758). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, I. C. (2010). Precisely wrong? The problems with the Jones and Martin genetic model of sex differences in handedness and language lateralisation. Cortex, 46(5), 700702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, I. C. (2019). Half a century of handedness research: Myths, truths; fictions, facts; Backwards, but mostly forwards. Brain and Neuroscience Advances, 3, 2398212818820513. https://doi.org/10.1177/2398212818820513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McManus, I. C. (2022). Cerebral polymorphisms for lateralisation: Modelling the genetic and phenotypic architectures of multiple functional modules. Symmetry, 14(4), 814. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym14040814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, I. C., & Bryden, M. P. (1992). The genetics of handedness, cerebral dominance, and lateralization. In Rapin, I. & Segalowitz, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology, Vol. 6 (pp. 115144). Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
McManus, I. C., Moore, J., Freegard, M., & Rawles, R. (2010). Science in the making: Right hand, left hand. III: Estimating historical rates of left-handedness. Laterality, 15(12), 186–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500802565313Google ScholarPubMed
McManus, I. C., Davison, A., & Armour, J. A. L. (2013). Multilocus genetic models of handedness closely resemble single-locus models in explaining family data and are compatible with genome-wide association studies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1288(1), 4858. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medland, S. E., Duffy, D. L., Wright, M. J., Geffen, G. M., Hay, D. A., Levy, F., …, Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Genetic influences on handedness: Data from 25,732 Australian and Dutch twin families. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 330337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meguerditchian, A., Vauclair, J., & Hopkins, W. D. (2013). On the origins of human handedness and language: A comparative review of hand preferences for bimanual coordinated actions and gestural communication in nonhuman primates. Developmental Psychobiology, 55(6), 637650. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikangas, A. K., & Almasy, L. (2020). Using the tools of genetic epidemiology to understand sex differences in neuropsychiatric disorders. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 19(6), e12660. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12660CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Micheletti, A. J. C., Ruxton, G. D., & Gardner, A. (2018). Why war is a man's game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1884), 20180975. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0975CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ocklenburg, S., & Güntürkün, O. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetries: The comparative view. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ocklenburg, S., Isparta, S., Peterburs, J., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2019). Paw preferences in cats and dogs: Meta-analysis. Laterality, 24(6), 647677. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019.1578228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packheiser, J., Papadatou-Pastou, M., Koufaki, A., Paracchini, S., Stein, C. C., Schmitz, J., … Ocklenburg, S. (2023). Elevated levels of mixed-hand preference in dyslexia: Meta-analyses of 68 studies Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 154, 105420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105420CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papadatou-Pastou, M., Ntolka, E., Schmitz, J., Martin, M., Munafò, M. R., Ocklenburg, S., & Paracchini, S. (2020). Human handedness: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(6), 481524. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramaley, F. (1913). Inheritance of left-handedness. The American Naturalist 47(564), 730738. https://doi.org/10.1086/279383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raymond, M., Pontier, D., Dufour, A.-B., & Møller, A. P. (1996). Frequency-dependent maintenance of left handedness in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 263(1377), 16271633. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1996.0238Google ScholarPubMed
Richardson, D. S., Komdeur, J., & Burke, T. (2004). Inbreeding in the seychelles warbler: Environment-dependent maternal effects. Evolution, 58(9), 20372048. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb00488.xGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, L. J. (1980). Lateralisation in the avian brain. Bird Behavior, 2(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.3727/015613880791573835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, L. J., Zucca, P., & Vallortigara, G. (2004). Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, S420S422. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2004.0200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachs, J. L., Mueller, U. G., Wilcox, T. P., & Bull, J. J. (2004). The evolution of cooperation. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 79(2), 135160. https://doi.org/10.1086/383541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaafsma, S. M., Geuze, R. H., Riedstra, B., Schiefenhövel, W., Bouma, A., & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2012). Handedness in a nonindustrial society challenges the fighting hypothesis as an evolutionary explanation for left-handedness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(2), 9499. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schijven, D., Sourena, S.-N., Simon, E. F., & Clyde, F. (2024). Exome-wide analysis implicates rare protein-altering variants in human handedness. Nature Communications, 15(1), 2632. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-46277-wCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitz, J., Zheng, M., Lui, K. F. H., McBride, C., Ho, C. S. H., & Paracchini, S. (2022). Quantitative multidimensional phenotypes improve genetic analysis of laterality traits. Translational Psychiatry, 12(1), 68. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-01834-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual selection and the Descent of Man (pp. 136179). AldineGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14(1), 249264. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/14.1.249CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Úbeda, F., Ohtsuki, H., & Gardner, A. (2014). Ecology drives intragenomic conflict over menopause. Ecology Letters, 17(2), 165174. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/ele.12208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uomini, N. T., & Ruck, L. (2018). Manual laterality and cognition through evolution: An archeological perspective. In Forrester, G. S., Hopkins, W. D., Hudry, K., & Lindell, A. (Eds.), Cerebral lateralization and cognition: Evolutionary and developmental investigations of behavioral biases (pp. 295323). Elsevier Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallortigara, G., & Rogers, L. (2005). Survival with an asymmetrical brain: Advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 28(4), 575589. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
West, S. A., & Gardner, A. (2013). Adaptation and inclusive fitness. Current Biology, 23(13), R577R584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007a). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17(16), R661R672. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, S. A., Griffin, A. S., & Gardner, A. (2007b). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 20(2), 415432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, J. F., & Haig, D. (2003). Inbreeding, maternal care and genomic imprinting. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 221(4), 559564. https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2003.3206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, J. F., & Úbeda, F. (2011). Diseases associated with genomic imprinting. In Cheng, X. & Blumenthal, R. M. (Eds.), Progress in molecular biology and translational science, Vol. 101 (pp. 401445). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1980). Sociobiology: The abridged edition. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wolf, J. B., Brodie, III, E. D., Cheverud, J. M., Moore, A. J., & Wade, M. J. (1998). Evolutionary consequences of indirect genetic effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13(2), 6469. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(97)01233-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, C. J., & Aggleton, J. P. (1989). Handedness in ‘fast ball’ sports: Do left-handers have an innate advantage? British Journal of Psychology, 80(2), 227240. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02316.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeo, R. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1993). Developmental origins of variation in human hand preference. Genetica, 89(1), 281296. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02424521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zickert, N., Geuze, R. H., van der Feen, F. E., & Groothuis, T. G. G. (2018). Fitness costs and benefits associated with hand preference in humans: A large internet study in a Dutch sample. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(2), 235248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Comparative predictions as to how dispersal affects relatedness between social partners and hence the evolutionarily favoured incidence of left-handedness, depending on the type of social interactions: within-group (selfish) or between-group (altruistic) combat. Lower dispersal leads to higher relatedness, hence low dispersal favours a lower incidence of left-handedness when it is selfish and a higher incidence of left-handedness when it is altruistic. Sex-biased dispersal creates asymmetry in relatedness and hence favours a sex difference in left-handedness.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Sex differences, parental genetic effects and parent-of-origin effects at loci underpinning left-handedness. Sex-biased expression of genes can lead to a sex difference in incidence of left-handedness. Expression of parents’ genes can lead to parental genetic effects in relation to left-handedness. Parent-of-origin-specific gene expression can lead to parent-of-origin effects in relation to left-handedness.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Predictions as to how dispersal pattern and gene function modulate the pattern of genomic imprinting. See Supplementary Material Figure S7 for additional predictions concerning the phenotypic consequences of gene deletions, gene duplications, epimutations and uniparental disomies.

Supplementary material: File

Dong et al. supplementary material

Dong et al. supplementary material
Download Dong et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.7 MB