Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T04:20:05.017Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural variation in running techniques among non-industrial societies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2022

Ian J. Wallace*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA
Thomas S. Kraft
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Vivek V. Venkataraman
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
Helen E. Davis
Affiliation:
Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
Nicholas B. Holowka
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY, USA
Alexandra R. Harris
Affiliation:
Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Daniel E. Lieberman
Affiliation:
Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
Michael Gurven
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Research among non-industrial societies suggests that body kinematics adopted during running vary between groups according to the cultural importance of running. Among groups in which running is common and an important part of cultural identity, runners tend to adopt what exercise scientists and coaches consider to be good technique for avoiding injury and maximising performance. In contrast, among groups in which running is not particularly culturally important, people tend to adopt suboptimal technique. This paper begins by describing key elements of good running technique, including landing with a forefoot or midfoot strike pattern and leg oriented roughly vertically. Next, we review evidence from non-industrial societies that cultural attitudes about running associate with variation in running techniques. Then, we present new data from Tsimane forager–horticulturalists in Bolivia. Our findings suggest that running is neither a common activity among the Tsimane nor is it considered an important part of cultural identity. We also demonstrate that when Tsimane do run, they tend to use suboptimal technique, specifically landing with a rearfoot strike pattern and leg protracted ahead of the knee (called overstriding). Finally, we discuss processes by which culture might influence variation in running techniques among non-industrial societies, including self-optimisation and social learning.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: New evidence from the Tsimane underscores that running techniques vary between societies according to the cultural importance of running

Introduction

Culture influences how people move their bodies (Mauss, Reference Mauss1935), which is an idea of shared interest to researchers working in all fields of anthropology. Ethnographers, social anthropologists and linguistic anthropologists have studied how cultural norms and values affect patterns of body movement involved in dance, play, communication, rituals, food production, tool making and numerous other activities (e.g. Abel, Reference Abel1984; Bateson, Reference Bateson1977; Bril, Reference Bril, Wade and Whiting1986; Farnell, Reference Farnell1995; Gandon et al., Reference Gandon, Bootsma, Endler and Grosman2013; Hall, Reference Hall1996; Kaeppler, Reference Kaeppler and Spencer1985; Kendon, Reference Kendon1988; Mahias, Reference Mahias and Lemonnier1993; Pelosse, Reference Pelosse1981; Williams, Reference Williams1975). Archaeological research often proceeds from the premise that material culture traditions are the result of manufacturing methods associated with body techniques that were socially learned and passed down through generations (e.g. Inizan et al., Reference Inizan, Reduron-Ballinger, Roche and Tixier1999; Lemonnier, Reference Lemonnier1986; Pétrequin, Reference Pétrequin and Lemonnier1993; Roux, Reference Roux2019; Tostevin, Reference Tostevin2012). Biological anthropologists have emphasised how cultural, environmental and physiological factors interact in shaping variation in human body movement patterns (e.g. Holowka et al., Reference Holowka, Kraft, Wallace, Gurven and Venkataramanin press; Kraft et al., Reference Kraft, Venkataraman and Dominy2014; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2014; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010, Reference Lieberman, Castillo, Otarola-Castillo, Sang, Sigei, Ojiambo and Pitsiladis2015a; Venkataraman et al., Reference Venkataraman, Kraft and Dominy2012, Reference Venkataraman, Yegian, Wallace, Holowka, Tacey, Gurven and Kraft2018; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Koch, Holowka and Lieberman2018a; Watanabe, Reference Watanabe1971; Willems et al., Reference Willems, Stassijns, Cornelis and D'Août2017). Even primatologists have studied the ways that social learning shapes the gestures employed by our species’ closest living relatives – chimpanzees and bonobos – for grooming, communication and tool use (e.g. Boesh, 1991; Halina et al., Reference Halina, Rossano and Tomasello2013; Lonsdorf et al., Reference Lonsdorf, Eberly and Pusey2004; Wrangham et al., Reference Wrangham, Koops, Machanda, Worthington, Bernard, Brazeau and Muller2016). Thus, achieving a better understanding of the ways that body movement patterns are shaped by culture can be considered a major goal of anthropology.

Notwithstanding interest among anthropologists, the influence of culture on the body techniques employed during many common types of physical activity has yet to be thoroughly explored. Here, we consider one such activity: running. More specifically, we consider running at submaximal speed (i.e. aerobic intensity), as opposed to sprinting. Over the past decade, evidence has accumulated from research among non-industrial societies that body movement patterns adopted during running tend to vary between groups according to the perceived cultural importance of running. Among groups that value running and consider it an important part of their cultural identity, people tend to adopt elements of what many exercise scientists and coaches consider to be good running technique for avoiding injury and maximising performance (Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2014; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010, Reference Lieberman, Castillo, Otarola-Castillo, Sang, Sigei, Ojiambo and Pitsiladis2015a). In contrast, among groups in which running is not considered important to cultural identity, people tend to adopt suboptimal techniques when they run (Hatala et al., Reference Hatala, Dingwall, Wunderlich and Richmond2013; Pontzer et al., Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014).

This paper is structured as follows: first, we briefly describe key differences between good and suboptimal running techniques. Second, we review recent evidence from non-industrial societies that cultural attitudes about running are associated with variation in running techniques. Third, we present new data on running frequency and technique collected among the Tsimane, a non-industrial forager–horticulturalist group living in the Bolivian Amazon. Our observations suggest that running is not a common activity among the Tsimane, and when Tsimane people do run, they tend to employ suboptimal running technique. Fourth and finally, we speculate about potential ways in which culture is responsible for shaping variation in running techniques among non-industrial societies.

Good vs. suboptimal running techniques

Exercise scientists and coaches generally consider good running technique in terms of body kinematics that minimise susceptibility to injury and maximise performance (Anderson, Reference Anderson2018; Daniels, Reference Daniels2014; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2021). Few running-related injuries are caused by a single, traumatic event but instead by the cumulative effects of forces that act repeatedly on the body, often called repetitive stress injuries. Such injuries include plantar fasciitis, skeletal stress fractures, patellofemoral pain syndrome and iliotibial band syndrome. The forces that have shown the most consistent relationship with running-related repetitive stress injuries are impact transients (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Bowser and Mullineaux2016; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Tenforde, Outerleys, Reilly and Davis2020), the abrupt collision forces of approximately 1.5–3 times body weight that occur within the first 50 milliseconds after the foot strikes the ground. Thus, from the perspective of injury prevention, attenuation of impact transients is regarded as an important feature of good running technique (Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2012, Reference Lieberman2021; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010).

Running performance is determined largely by three physiological factors: running economy, maximal oxygen uptake and blood lactate accumulation (Bassett & Howley, Reference Bassett and Howley2000; Conley & Krahenbuhl, Reference Conley and Krahenbuhl1980; Farrell et al., Reference Farrell, Wilmore, Coyle, Billing and DL1979; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Howley, Bassett, Thompson and Fitzhugh2010). Of these factors, the one most affected by running technique is running economy (Barnes & Kilding, Reference Barnes and Kilding2015; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Pyne, Telford and Hawley2004), defined as distance travelled per unit of energy expended. Because of our evolutionary history, all humans share key anatomical traits that minimise energy expenditure during running, such as long lower limbs, long Achilles tendons, foot arches and short toes (Bramble & Lieberman, Reference Bramble and Lieberman2004; Holowka & Lieberman, Reference Holowka and Lieberman2018; Rolian et al., 2008; Venkadesan et al., Reference Venkadesan, Yawar, Eng, Dias, Singh, Tommasini and Mandre2020). Nevertheless, running economy can vary substantially between individuals and groups (Barnes & Kilding, Reference Barnes and Kilding2015), particularly between people who run habitually vs. infrequently (Bransford & Howley, Reference Bransford and Howley1977; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Bransford, Costill, Daniels, Howley and Krahenbuhl1995). Several elements of running technique potentially contribute to this variation, including stride length and rate, leg and thigh kinematics, orientation of the foot at ground strike, ground contact time, vertical oscillation of the centre of mass, torso lean and arm swing (Cavanagh et al., Reference Cavanagh, Pollock and Landa1977; Folland et al., Reference Folland, Allen, Black, Handsaker and Forrester2017; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Warrener, Wang and Castillo2015b; Moore, Reference Moore2016; Preece et al., Reference Preece, Bramah and Mason2019; Tartaruga et al., Reference Tartaruga, Brisswalter, Peyre-Tartaruga, Avila, Alberton, Coertjens and Kruel2012; Trowell et al., Reference Trowell, Phillips, Saunders and Bonacci2019; Williams & Cavanagh, Reference Williams and Cavanagh1987; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cavanagh and Ziff1987).

Importantly, neither injury risk nor running performance are dictated by any single element of running technique but rather by the combined effects of multiple elements (Folland et al., Reference Folland, Allen, Black, Handsaker and Forrester2017; Williams & Cavanagh, Reference Williams and Cavanagh1987), as well as other factors such as anthropometry, physical fitness and training, environmental conditions and footwear (Barnes & Kilding, Reference Barnes and Kilding2015; Cheung & Ngai, Reference Cheung and Ngai2016; Perl et al., Reference Perl, Daoud and Lieberman2012; Raichlen et al., Reference Raichlen, Armstrong and Lieberman2011; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Pyne, Telford and Hawley2004; van der Worp et al., Reference van der Worp, ten Haaf, van Cingel, de Wijer, Nijhuis-van der Sanden and Staal2015). Nevertheless, certain aspects of running technique are more consequential and have received more research attention than others. Recent anthropological research among non-industrial societies has focused on two particular elements of running technique: foot orientation at ground strike and the position of the foot relative to the ipsilateral knee and hip at ground strike (Hatala et al., Reference Hatala, Dingwall, Wunderlich and Richmond2013; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2014; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010, Reference Lieberman, Castillo, Otarola-Castillo, Sang, Sigei, Ojiambo and Pitsiladis2015a; Pontzer et al., Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014).

Orientation of the foot at ground strike is commonly categorised into three types: a forefoot strike, in which the ball of the foot (metatarsal heads) makes contact with the ground first; a midfoot strike, in which the heel and ball of the foot simultaneously contact the ground; and a rearfoot strike, in which the heel first contacts the ground (Figure 1). Evidence indicates that vulnerability to repetitive stress injuries may be greater among runners who rearfoot strike than those who forefoot or midfoot strike (Daoud et al., Reference Daoud, Geissler, Wang, Saretsky, Daoud and Lieberman2012; Diebal et al., Reference Diebal, Gregory, Alitz and Gerber2012; Roper et al., Reference Roper, Harding, Doerfler, Dexter, Kravitz, Dufek and Mermier2016). One likely cause of this variation in injury risk is the association between different foot strike types and impact transient forces. Experiments have shown that rearfoot striking produces especially large and rapid impact transients, whereas forefoot and midfoot striking typically produce small or no measurable impact transients. The primary reasons for this are that, compared with rearfoot strikes, forefoot and midfoot strikes involve greater ankle compliance and lower effective mass of the decelerating lower limb at ground contact (Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010). With regard to performance, whereas most recreational runners tend to rearfoot strike, more top finishers in middle- and long-distance races tend to forefoot or midfoot strike (Hanley et al., Reference Hanley, Bissas, Merlino and Gruber2019; Hasegawa et al., Reference Hasegawa, Yamauchi and Kraemer2007; Hayes & Caplan, Reference Hayes and Caplan2012; Larson et al., Reference Larson, Higgins, Kaminski, Decker, Preble, Lyons and Normile2011; Preece et al., Reference Preece, Bramah and Mason2019). It is conceivable that this is because forefoot and midfoot striking provide some advantage in terms of running economy, perhaps by promoting greater elastic energy storage in the Achilles tendons and foot arches, although most experiments designed to test this hypothesis have found running economy to be similar between forefoot, midfoot and rearfoot strikers (Cunningham et al., Reference Cunningham, Schilling, Anders and Carrier2010; Gruber et al., Reference Gruber, Umberger, Braun and Hamill2013; Perl et al., Reference Perl, Daoud and Lieberman2012). Nevertheless, given the potential benefits for injury prevention, and at least no apparent cost for running economy, exercise scientists and coaches often consider forefoot or midfoot striking to be a feature of good running technique (Anderson, Reference Anderson2018; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2021).

Figure 1. Variation in foot strike patterns during running: forefoot strike (left), midfoot strike (middle), and rearfoot strike (right). Drawing by Samantha Shields.

The position of a runner's foot at ground strike relative to their ipsilateral knee and hip is determined by the degree of flexion in their knee and hip. A commonly accepted characteristic of good running technique is landing on the ground with enough knee and hip flexion to position the foot below the knee and orient the leg (tibia) roughly vertically (Anderson, Reference Anderson2018; Daniels, Reference Daniels2014; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2021). Overstriding, defined as contacting the ground with a foot placed far ahead of the knee and hip, is widely considered suboptimal form and occurs when a runner lands with their knee extended and leg oriented at a protracted angle (Figure 2). Studies have shown that, compared with landing with a flexed knee, overstriding produces larger and possibly more rapid impact transient forces (Gerritsen et al., Reference Gerritsen, van den Bogert and Nigg1995; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Warrener, Wang and Castillo2015b), primarily because landing with an extended knee increases the stiffness and effective mass of the lower limb (Derrick, Reference Derrick2004). Thus, overstriding is commonly believed to increase risk of injury (Anderson, Reference Anderson2018; Daniels, Reference Daniels2014; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2021). In terms of performance, while overstriding is common among untrained and infrequent runners, more skilled and faster distance runners are often observed to avoid overstriding (Folland et al., Reference Folland, Allen, Black, Handsaker and Forrester2017; Preece et al., Reference Preece, Bramah and Mason2019; Trowell et al., Reference Trowell, Phillips, Saunders and Bonacci2019), possibly because it negatively affects running economy. In particular, overstriding has been shown to generate especially large braking forces, the posteriorly directed forces in the horizontal plane that slow the body down during running (Heiderscheit et al., Reference Heiderscheit, Chumanov, Michalski, Wille and Ryan2011; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Warrener, Wang and Castillo2015b; Napier et al., Reference Napier, MacLean, Maurer, Taunton and Hunt2019; Wille et al. Reference Wille, Lenhart, Wang, Thelen and Heiderscheit2014). To counter these larger braking forces and maintain a steady running speed, overstriding is expected to require leg and thigh muscles to generate larger forces to propel the body forward, thus costing more energy (Chang & Kram, Reference Chang and Kram1999).

Figure 2. Different orientations of the leg (tibia) at foot strike during running: oriented roughly vertically (left) and at a protracted angle, referred to as overstriding (right). Drawing by Samantha Shields.

In sum, while recognising that there is no single correct way to run, in terms of the two features of running technique that have recently received research attention from anthropologists, for the purposes of this paper, we define good running technique as involving striking the ground with the forefoot or midfoot and with the leg oriented roughly vertically.

Running techniques among non-industrial societies

The study of running techniques is a relatively new scientific field that began attracting a large number of researchers only in the 1970s, as a result of the rapid growth of participation in recreational running that occurred in the United States in the late 1960s and early 1970s (Cavanagh, Reference Cavanagh and Cavanagh1990). With this running boom, issues such as injury prevention and performance suddenly became relevant to many more people. Among anthropologists, general interest in running started to grow at roughly the same time, particularly after the publication of a seminal paper by Carrier (Reference Carrier1984) which proposed that long-distance running ability represents a key evolutionary adaptation that differentiates humans from other primates. Bramble and Lieberman (Reference Bramble and Lieberman2004) later developed this idea more fully, including by demonstrating that fossil evidence for enhanced running ability in the human lineage traces back over a million years. Eventually, interest in running among anthropologists grew to include the topic of running techniques, specifically the body movement patterns that ancient runners might have adopted, millennia prior to the invention of modern running shoes and scientific research on injury risk and performance. This led to a series of studies of the running techniques used by people in contemporary non-industrial societies, with a focus on groups that are habitually barefoot or minimally shod, as people were throughout the vast majority of human evolutionary history.

The first group to be studied were the Kalenjin, an ethnolinguistic group living mainly in the highlands of western Kenya (Lieberman et al., 2010, Reference Lieberman, Castillo, Otarola-Castillo, Sang, Sigei, Ojiambo and Pitsiladis2015a). Traditionally, Kalenjin people subsist on some combination of small-scale farming and pastoralism, and many people are habitually barefoot. The Kalenjin famously include many of the world's top competitive middle- and long-distance runners (Pitsiladis et al. Reference Pitsiladis, Onywera, Geogiades, O'Connell and Boit2004; Onywera et al. Reference Onywera, Scott, Boit and Pitsiladis2006). Running has long been part of Kalenjin culture, owing to the need to run to carry out cattle raids, which is an important custom among some pastoralist groups in East Africa that typically requires running long distances, sometimes well beyond the distance of a marathon (Bale & Sang, Reference Bale and Sang1996; Christensen & Damkjaer, Reference Christensen, Damkjaer, Sands and Sands2010). Western-style track and field sports were first introduced to Kenya by British colonialists during the early twentieth century, and by the mid-1940s, Kalenjin runners were recognised as among the best in the country. By the 1950s, Kalenjin runners were competing internationally, and in the 1960s, Kalenjin runner Kipchoge Keino broke multiple world records, won a gold medal at the 1968 Olympics, and inspired generations of Kalenjins to follow in his footsteps. Since then, numerous other Kalenjin runners have achieved international fame, and running excellence has become a major part of Kalenjin cultural identity. Today, young Kalenjins often grow up running long distances on a regular basis, usually to and from school, many with the hope of one day becoming successful competitive runners (Ojiambo et al., Reference Ojiambo, Easton, Casajús, Konstabel, Reilly and Pitsiladis2012; Onywera et al., Reference Onywera, Scott, Boit and Pitsiladis2006).

Lieberman and colleagues (2010, Reference Lieberman, Castillo, Otarola-Castillo, Sang, Sigei, Ojiambo and Pitsiladis2015a) studied running techniques among the Kalenjin, focusing on foot orientation at ground strike among rural, habitually barefoot adolescents and adults, and among elite competitive runners who mostly grew up in rural communities and were once habitually barefoot. During barefoot running, both rural individuals and elite runners were observed to mostly land with a forefoot or midfoot strike pattern. Among elite runners wearing cushioned running shoes, forefoot and midfoot striking were still found to be the most common landing patterns, although rearfoot striking was more common than during barefoot running. Based on these findings, Lieberman and colleagues (Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010, Reference Lieberman2015a) hypothesised that more frequent use of forefoot and midfoot striking used to be greater among runners in the past, before the introduction of modern running shoes.

Additional support for this hypothesis came from a study by Lieberman (Reference Lieberman2014) of running techniques among the Tarahumara, a Native American group of subsistence farmers living in the Sierra Madre Occidental of northwestern Mexico. For many generations of Tarahumara (who call themselves Rarámuri), running has played an integral role in their cultural identity (Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Mahaffey, Cubesare Quimare, Holowka, Wallace and Baggish2020), as it has for other Native American groups in North America (Collier, Reference Collier2007; Gilbert, Reference Gilbert2018; Martin, Reference Martin2020; Nabokov, Reference Nabokov1981). In the past, Tarahumara ran to hunt animals by chasing them over long distances (Levi, Reference Levi2020; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Mahaffey, Cubesare Quimare, Holowka, Wallace and Baggish2020; Lumholtz, Reference Lumholtz1902; Schwatka, Reference Schwatka1893), a strategy referred to as persistence hunting (Carrier, Reference Carrier1984), and which has been documented among multiple other non-industrial societies (Estioko-Griffen, Reference Estioko-Griffen1986; Liebenberg, Reference Liebenberg2006; Lowie, Reference Lowie1924; McCarthy, Reference McCarthy1957; Nabokov, Reference Nabokov1981). Today, persistence hunting is rare among the Tarahumara, but many people continue to participate in an ancient custom of long-distance footraces known as rarajípare and ariwete (Bennett & Zingg, Reference Bennett and Zingg1935; Irigoyen-Rascón & Palma-Batista, Reference Irigoyen-Rascón and Palma-Batista1994; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Mahaffey, Cubesare Quimare, Holowka, Wallace and Baggish2020; Pennington, Reference Pennington1963). In rarajípare, men run over rugged terrain while kicking and chasing a small wooden ball, often for distances exceeding that of a marathon. In ariwete, women run while using a hooked stick to flick a hoop made of stiff plant fibers, usually for distances longer than a half-marathon. These races provide important opportunities to bring together and strengthen ties among communities, and they also serve an important function in Tarahumara spirituality (Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Mahaffey, Cubesare Quimare, Holowka, Wallace and Baggish2020). In recent decades, in addition to engaging in traditional races, many Tarahumara have begun taking part in Western-style long-distance running competitions held in Mexico, and a few top runners have participated and excelled in international competitions (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Espino, Infante-Ramírez, Cervantes-Borunda, Hernández-Torres, Rivera-Cisneros and Kjaergaard2017; McDougall, Reference McDougall2009). For example, among the first Tarahumara runners to gain international recognition were Victoriano Churro and Juan Herrera who won the Leadville Trail 100 ultramarathon in 1993 and 1994, respectively, while running in traditional Tarahumara attire including minimal sandals consisting only of a piece of stiff material fastened to the bottom of the foot by a leather strap (McDougall, Reference McDougall2009; Severance, Reference Severance1993). Before roughly a half-century ago, the Tarahumara made the soles of their traditional sandals from animal hides or plant fibers (Bennett & Zingg, Reference Bennett and Zingg1935; Levi, Reference Levi1998). Today, however, their sandals’ soles are typically made from the tread surfaces of car tyres (Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Koch, Holowka and Lieberman2018a), a design innovation that has occurred independently among other non-industrial societies that use minimal footwear (Musiba et al., Reference Musiba, Tuttle, Hallgrímsson and Webb1997; Somjee, Reference Somjee1993).

Lieberman's (Reference Lieberman2014) study of running techniques among the Tarahumara focused on foot strike patterns and lower limb kinematics in a sample of Tarahumara farmers who habitually wore traditional sandals, all of whom were experienced rarajípare runners and a few of whom had competed in national and international ultramarathons. It was observed that, while running in their minimal sandals, the Tarahumara rarely landed with a rearfoot strike pattern. Also, overstriding was rare. Instead, Tarahumara runners tended to use either a forefoot or midfoot strike pattern and land with their legs oriented roughly vertically (Figure 3). Therefore, consistent with Kalenjin runners, Tarahumara runners were found to adopt key features of what many exercise scientists and coaches consider to be good running technique.

Figure 3. Tarahumara runner in Mexico landing with a midfoot strike pattern and leg that is oriented roughly vertically. Photo by David Ramos and used here with permission.

However, studies of running techniques among two other non-industrial societies – the Daasanach and Hadza – showed different patterns than those reported for the Kalenjin and Tarahumara (Hatala et al., Reference Hatala, Dingwall, Wunderlich and Richmond2013; Pontzer et al., Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014). The Daasanach, who live primarily in southern Ethiopia and northern Kenya near the Omo River and Lake Turkana, are traditionally pastoralists, although many people today also subsist on small-scale farming and fishing. The Hadza live in northern Tanzania near Lake Eyasi and are traditionally hunter–gatherers. Among both the Daasanach and Hadza, nearly all people are habitually barefoot or wear minimal sandals made from car tyre treads (Musiba et al., Reference Musiba, Tuttle, Hallgrímsson and Webb1997), similar to Tarahumara sandals. Hatala and colleagues (Reference Hatala, Dingwall, Wunderlich and Richmond2013) examined foot strike patterns during barefoot running among a sample of Daasanach adults in northern Kenya and found that rearfoot striking was very common, midfoot striking was much less common and forefoot striking was very rare. Pontzer and colleagues (Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014) examined foot strike patterns among a sample of Hadza juveniles and adults running barefoot and in minimal sandals and found that, both with and without footwear, rearfoot and midfoot striking were roughly equally common and forefoot striking was never observed. Overstriding was not quantified in either study, but figures in both the paper by Hatala et al. (Reference Hatala, Dingwall, Wunderlich and Richmond2013) and Pontzer et al. (Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014) illustrating typical Daasanach and Hadza rearfoot strikes, respectively, clearly depict people who are overstriding. Considered together with the results from the Kalenjin and Tarahumara, these findings in just four populations demonstrate that running techniques are variable among people in contemporary non-industrial societies, and hence also probably were among people living in the ancient past. Yet, if simply being barefoot or minimally shod does not dictate running techniques among non-industrial societies, what factors are important?

One possible explanation for why running techniques among the Daasanach and Hadza were found to differ from those of the Kalenjin and Tarahumara is that unlike among the Kalenjin and Tarahumara, running is not a particularly common activity among the Daasanach or Hadza, nor is there any indication that it is an important part of cultural identity. In other words, among non-industrial societies, running techniques potentially vary between groups according to the cultural importance of running. Elements of what is considered good running technique may be more likely to be developed among groups in which people run on a regular basis and consider running to be an important part of their culture, whereas suboptimal running technique may be more common among groups in which running is not considered culturally important and there are few opportunities to develop elements of good running technique. If so, variation in running techniques among non-industrial societies may represent an elegant and underappreciated example of how culture influences how people use their bodies to move.

To further explore this hypothesis, we studied running techniques among the Tsimane, a forager–horticulturalist group living in the Bolivian Amazon. We also objectively measured the frequency of running among the Tsimane, and as we show below, running was not found to be a common activity. Therefore, we hypothesised that, similar to the Daasanach and Hadza, Tsimane would commonly adopt elements of suboptimal running technique, specifically rearfoot striking and overstriding.

Methods

The Tsimane

The Tsimane are an indigenous group living in the tropical lowland of the Bolivian Amazon (Beni Department). They subsist on a combination of slash-and-burn horticulture (primarily plantains, rice, manioc and maize); fishing in rivers, streams and lagoons; hunting a variety of neotropical mammals; and gathering of fruits, nuts, honey and other foods. The Tsimane inhabit over 90 villages of approximately 50–500 people located along rivers and interfluvial terra firma. Since the early 2000s, numerous aspects of Tsimane behaviour, culture, and biology have been studied as part of the ongoing longitudinal research programme known as the Tsimane Health and Life History Project (Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Stieglitz, Trumble, Blackwell, Beheim, Davis and Kaplan2017). This research has shown, among other things, that the Tsimane tend to be very physically fit compared with many people in post-industrial societies, including by having excellent aerobic and cardiovascular fitness into older age (Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Thompson, Trumble, Wann, Allam, Beheim and Thomas2017; Pisor et al., Reference Pisor, Gurven, Blackwell, Kaplan and Yetish2013). Tsimane people also engage in high levels of physical activity across their lifecourse, primarily for purposes of subsistence (Davis & Cashdan, Reference Davis and Cashdan2019; Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Jaeggi, Kaplan and Cummings2013; Kraft et al., Reference Kraft, Venkataraman, Wallace, Crittenden, Holowka, Stieglitz and Pontzer2021; Stieglitz et al., Reference Stieglitz, Trumble, Kaplan and Gurven2017). For example, Tsimane women and men walk, on average, 7.6 and 9.9 km per day, respectively (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Gurven and Cashdan2022).

Nevertheless, throughout roughly two decades of research among the Tsimane, there has been no indication that running is an especially common activity or that running is an important part of Tsimane cultural identity. Hunting practices, for example, have been studied extensively among the Tsimane (Gurven et al., Reference Gurven, Kaplan and Gutierrez2006; Hooper et al., Reference Hooper, Demps, Gurven, Gerkey and Kaplan2015; Schniter et al., Reference Schniter, Gurven, Kaplan, Wilcox and Hooper2015; Trumble et al., Reference Trumble, Smith, O'Connor, Kaplan and Gurven2014), yet there has never been an observation or report of persistence hunting taking place, either recently or in the past. Tsimane hunters often sprint for brief periods to chase or outmanoeuvre prey animals, and to follow hunting dogs when in pursuit, but they seemingly never utilise endurance running to exhaust their prey. In addition, there has never been an observation or report of customary footraces taking place among the Tsimane. In recent years, football (soccer) has become a popular recreational activity among some Tsimane, which involves running intermittently at submaximal speeds, but the Tsimane seemingly do not engage in sports that require sustained running at aerobic intensity over long distances. Anecdotes aside, however, running has never been formally investigated among the Tsimane, hence the motivation for this study.

Another topic for which data are currently lacking is the footwear habits of the Tsimane. Our impression from fieldwork has been that the majority of Tsimane are habitually barefoot, especially during childhood and adolescence, but this has never been quantified. Among people who wear shoes, traditionally the most common types of footwear are minimal sandals made from car tyre treads (called abarca by the Tsimane). However, these have been increasingly replaced by commercially available flip-flop sandals (chinelas). Football cleats are now often worn by men, but only during recreational football matches. Given anthropological interest in relationships between footwear and running techniques among non-industrial societies, for this study, in addition to assessing running frequency and techniques, we also evaluated patterns of footwear use among the Tsimane.

Footwear frequency

To assess footwear use patterns among the Tsimane, a sample of 2,099 females and 1,992 males were surveyed in 83 villages. These data were collected primarily for the purposes of a separate study focused on the influence of footwear on risk of hookworm infection through the feet. Participants ranged in age from young children to elderly adults. Participants were asked whether they never wear shoes, rarely wear shoes, sometimes wear shoes, usually wear shoes or always wear shoes. For young children, responses were often obtained from a parent.

Running frequency

To assess the frequency of running among the Tsimane, 43 women and 30 men were recruited from four villages. Participant movement was recorded using GPS data loggers (QStarz BT-Q1000XT, Qstarz Int. Co., Taipei, Taiwan) worn around the neck on a lanyard. Movement was recorded every 10 seconds – which is considered by the manufacturer to be optimal for recording travel on foot while conserving battery power – over three consecutive days per participant. Power limitations in remote areas and technical difficulties owing to inclement weather resulted in some missing data (13.7% of participants had only two days with recorded tracks, rather than three). After GPS data were collected, GPS tracks were cleaned of spurious points by the DIGIT Lab at the University of Utah. To determine time spent running, first, we calculated travel speeds for all GPS point intervals by dividing distance moved by interval time. Second, because a few participants occasionally travelled by motorbike or taxi while wearing GPS units, we developed a custom algorithm to filter out intervals that involved motorised travel. In brief, we identified any intervals with travel speeds greater than 6.5 m/s and removed these points along with 100 surrounding consecutive points (corresponding to approximately 15 minutes) on either side within a given person-day. This procedure was effective at eliminating cases where acceleration or slow movement on roads produced plausible running speeds but manual investigation of GPS tracks indicated clear motorised transit. Third, we filtered out any remaining points where people travelled greater than 5 m/s, which we considered to be an upper-bound of realistic possible running speeds. Fourth, we converted travel speeds to dimensionless speeds (Froude number) as:

$$\displaystyle{{\rm Dimensionless \;speed = (travel \;speed)^2/(hip\; height \times \it g) }}.$$

where travel speed is in m/s (over epochs with median duration of 10 seconds), hip height (in metres) is calculated from standing and sitting heights and g is the gravitational constant (9.81 m/s2). Fifth, following Pontzer and colleagues (Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014), we calculated the percentage of movement bouts that indicated running by removing all intervals with travel speeds <0.5 m/s and dividing the number of bouts with dimensionless speeds >0.5 (the threshold between walking and running; Alexander & Jayes, Reference Alexander and Jayes1983) by the total number of bouts.

Running kinematics

To assess running techniques among the Tsimane, a group of 17 women and 15 men were recruited from the village of Jämsi Bayedye. Average (SD) stature was 152.4 (4.2) and 163.8 (8.1) cm among female and male participants, respectively. Average body weight was 59.1 (7.4) and 63.9 (8.9) kg among females and males, respectively. No participants reported any injury that would affect running ability, and none exhibited any visible problems with their gait. Among participants, 88% reported having been habitually barefoot for much of their lives and 34% reported having been habitually barefoot for their entire lives. All participants reported that they rarely run except for brief periods, for example, to escape danger, sprint after animals during hunting and while playing football.

Participants were recorded with a video camera (Casio EX-ZR100, Casio Computer Co., Tokyo, Japan) while running over the two types of terrain that Tsimane people most often encounter: an open, flat ground surface of hard-packed dirt located in the village, and a relatively flat dirt trail through forest surrounding the village. In both settings, evenly spaced flags were used to mark the running path. Participants were instructed to run at a speed that they would be comfortable running at for several minutes. They were told to maintain this speed and not decelerate until they had passed a flag approximately 3 m beyond the camera. The camera was positioned perpendicular to the running routes at a height of approximately 75 cm and distance between 3 and 5 m. The capture rate of the camera was 240 frames per second. Two trials were recorded for each participant in each setting. During all trials, participants ran barefoot.

Prior to recording running trials, reflective tape markers were affixed to the following anatomical locations on the right sides of participants’ bodies: greater trochanter (hip), centre of the knee (between the lateral femoral epicondyle and lateral tibial plateau), lateral malleolus and the lateral surface of the fifth metatarsal head. All anatomical locations were determined by manual palpation. The greater trochanter marker was affixed to the participant's overlying clothes, but all other markers were affixed directly on the participant's skin. Lower extremity joint angles were measured at the moment of foot strike, defined as the first video frame in which the right foot made contact with the ground. Foot strike angle was measured as the angle between the ground plane and a line connecting the heel surface and distal fifth metatarsal. Positive angles correspond to rearfoot striking, negative angles correspond to forefoot striking, and an angle of zero corresponds to midfoot striking. Ankle angle was measured as the angle between the lines from the knee centre to the lateral malleolus and from the lateral malleolus to the distal fifth metatarsal. Overstride was defined as how far the ankle landed anterior to the knee and was measured as the angle between the leg (from the lateral malleolus to the knee centre) and a vertical line between the knee centre and ground plane. An angle of zero or less indicates no overstride, and higher angles indicate more overstride. An angular measurement of overstride was used rather than a linear measurement because there is less error in measuring the angle of the leg than the projected distance between the knee centre and foot (Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2014). Stride length was measured as the distance between two consecutive foot strikes, and dimensionless stride length was calculated by dividing stride length by lower extremity length (greater trochanter height) to standardise for body size variation among participants. Running speed was measured based on the horizontal translation of the tape marker placed on the greater trochanter, from which dimensionless speed was calculated to standardise for body size variation. All camera data were analysed using ImageJ software (v. 1.50i, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA). Trials were discarded when body markers were obscured in the camera recordings. The final sample was 78 trials (n = 52 in the open setting).

To assess sources of variation in lower extremity kinematics during running, we used general linear mixed models (GLMMs). In a main set of analyses, joint angles and dimensionless stride length were included as dependent variables, dimensionless speed, setting (open vs. forest) and sex were included as fixed effects, and participant identity was included as a random effect. A separate GLMM was used to assess the association between foot strike angle and overstride angle, which included foot strike angle as the dependent variable, overstride angle, dimensionless speed, setting and sex as fixed effects, and participant identity as a random effect. Another GLMM was used to assess the association between dimensionless speed and setting, which included dimensionless speed as the dependent variable, setting and sex as fixed effects, and participant identity as a random effect. All statistical analyses of kinematic data were conducted using JMP Pro software (v. 16.0, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA), with statistical significance set at p < 0.05.

Results

Among the Tsimane surveyed about their footwear habits, 65% of all people reported never or rarely wearing shoes (Figure 4). Always being barefoot or rarely wearing shoes was especially common among children and adolescents, but even among adults, a minimum of approximately 50% of people at all ages reported never or rarely wearing shoes. Among all age groups, less than 15% of people reported always wearing shoes.

Figure 4. Footwear habits among the Tsimane across different age categories.

Among the Tsimane adults who wore GPS units to measure their movement patterns, running was very rare among both women and men (Figure 5). Across all timepoints during which participants were recorded to be actively moving, only 2.4% of bouts occurred at dimensionless speeds that indicated running (Froude number >0.5).

Figure 5. Kernel density plot of dimensionless speed (Froude number) by sex among the Tsimane who wore GPS units to measure their physical activity patterns. The dashed line indicates the threshold above which dimensionless speed represents running (dimensionless speed >0.5). The plot only includes recorded bouts in which GPS recordings indicated that participants were actively moving (travel speeds >0.5 m/s).

In trials to assess running techniques among a sample of Tsimane adults, rearfoot striking and overstriding were both very common (Figures 6 and 7). No participant was ever observed to forefoot strike, midfoot striking was observed only once and rearfoot striking was observed in 99% of trials. In only two trials did participants land with their legs oriented vertically, whereas overstriding occurred in 98% of trials. Foot strike and overstride angles were positivity associated (p = 0.0043), such that foot strike angles were greater when participants overstrode more (Figure 8). Foot strike and overstride angles both increased at higher dimensionless speeds (p < 0.0001 and p = 0.0021, respectively), as did dimensionless stride lengths (p < 0.0001). Ankle angles, however, were not significantly affected by dimensionless speed (p = 0.43). In the forest, dimensionless speeds were slower than in the open setting (p = 0.011). After controlling for this variation in dimensionless speed, overstride angles and dimensionless stride lengths were lower in the forest than the open setting (p = 0.018 and p = 0.038, respectively), while foot strike and ankle angles were not significantly affected by setting (p = 0.25 and p = 0.49, respectively). The only kinematic variable for which sex was a significant predictor was overstride angle, which tended to be greater in females than males (p = 0.038).

Figure 6. Tsimane study participant in Bolivia running with a rearfoot strike pattern and overstriding.

Figure 7. Running kinematic variables analysed among the Tsimane relative to dimensionless speed (Froude number): dimensionless stride length (a), overstride angle (b), ankle angle (c), and foot strike angle (d). Black circles indicate open setting trials and green triangles indicate forest trail trials.

Figure 8. Association between foot strike and overstride angles among the Tsimane during running. Black circles indicate open setting trials and green triangles indicate forest trail trials.

Discussion

Our findings from research among the Tsimane add to evidence from other non-industrial societies that body movement patterns adopted during running tend to vary between groups according to the cultural importance of running. Similar to the Daasanach and Hadza, Tsimane people rarely run, and there are no indications that running is considered an important part of their cultural identity. Also similar to the Daasanach and Hadza, when Tsimane people do run, they tend to adopt elements of what many exercise scientists and coaches consider to be suboptimal running technique, specifically rearfoot striking and overstriding. In these ways, the Tsimane are different from the Kalenjin and Tarahumara, for whom running is a common activity and a major part of cultural identity, and who tend to use elements of what is considered good running technique for preventing injury and maximising performance, specifically forefoot or midfoot striking and landing with a roughly vertically oriented leg. Although the processes by which culture influences variation in running techniques among non-industrial societies are not currently known, we propose two possibilities that warrant future investigation.

First, among non-industrial societies in which running is a routine activity, people may be more likely to adopt elements of good running technique because they have had more opportunities to self-optimise their movement patterns through trial and error. This kind of fine-tuning of a person's running technique could be entirely subconscious and not require any social learning. Ample evidence of self-optimisation has been provided by experiments with runners in laboratory settings. For example, a common experimental design has been to measure a person's preferred stride length and frequency as they run on a treadmill, and then systematically vary (increase and decease) their stride length and frequency while simultaneously measuring energy expenditure in order to determine their energetically optimal stride length–frequency combination. In such experiments, frequent runners are consistently observed to naturally choose stride length–frequency combinations that closely match their experimentally determined most economical stride lengths and frequencies (Cavanagh & Williams, Reference Cavanagh and Williams1982; Hunter & Smith, Reference Hunter and Smith2007; Connick & Li, Reference Connick and Li2014), whereas inexperienced runners tend to choose less economical stride length–frequency combinations (de Ruiter et al., Reference de Ruiter, Verdijk, Werker, Zuidema and de Haan2014; van Oeveren et al., Reference van Oeveren, de Ruiter, Beek and van Dieën2017). In all likelihood, many other elements of running technique are, at least in part, shaped by self-optimisation, including lower extremity kinematics (Moore, Reference Moore2016; Moore et al., Reference Moore, Jones and Dixon2012, Reference Moore, Ashford, Cross, Hope, Jones and McCarthy-Ryan2019; Williams & Cavanagh, Reference Williams and Cavanagh1987). Moreover, owing to the heightened somatosensory feedback associated with being barefoot or minimally shod, self-optimisation might be expected to have an especially strong influence on running technique development among many non-industrial societies (Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2012).

Second, in addition to the effects of self-optimisation, among non-industrial societies in which running is common and an important part of cultural identity, people may tend to develop elements of good running technique as a result of social learning. Most likely, this would occur primarily through observational and imitative learning rather than explicit instruction. Numerous intervention studies with runners in post-industrial societies have shown that many elements of running technique can be altered through training and coaching, including lower extremity kinematics (Bailey & Messier, Reference Bailey and Messier1991; Clansey et al., Reference Clansey, Hanlon, Wallace, Nevill and Lake2014; Craighead et al., Reference Craighead, Lehecka and King2014; Crowell & Davis, Reference Crowell and Davis2011; Dallam et al., Reference Dallam, Wilber, Jadelis, Fletcher and Romanov2005; Diebal et al., Reference Diebal, Gregory, Alitz and Gerber2012; Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Bartlett, Romanov and Fotouhi2008; Messier & Cirillo, Reference Messier and Cirillo1989; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Martin, Craib, Caruso, Clifton and Hopewell1994). Among non-industrial societies, however, we are not aware of any documented cases of deliberate teaching of running technique. Competitive Kalenjin runners often receive substantial coaching, but not until they have already achieved elite status and their running technique has largely been established. Also, the coaching is often from non-Kalenjins (Wilber & Pitsiladis, Reference Wilber and Pitsiladis2012). Nevertheless, people in every society develop important skills by watching and imitating others who are good at them (Henrich, Reference Henrich2016; Henrich & Broesch, Reference Henrich and Broesch2011), and there is no reason that this would not be the case for running. Among non-industrial societies, customary public displays of running such as the traditional rarajípare and ariwete footraces of the Tarahumara would provide especially useful opportunities for observational learning. Indeed, Tarahumara runners often say when asked how they learn to run that they follow the techniques of champion footracers (Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2021). Persistence hunting, cattle raiding and any other activity in which people might run in a group could also provide valuable chances to watch and learn the techniques of skilled runners.

Footwear is another dimension of culture that may have some influence on running techniques among non-industrial societies, but not necessarily in a straightforward way. Studies of habitual runners in post-industrial societies have often found that people who run barefoot typically forefoot or midfoot strike and take shorter strides compared with shod runners, leading to less overstride (DeWit et al., Reference DeWit, De Clercq and Aerts2000; Divert et al., Reference Divert, Mornieux, Freychat, Baly, Mayer and Belli2008; Hamill et al., Reference Hamill, Russell, Gruber and Miller2011; Kerrigan et al., Reference Kerrigan, Franz, Keenan, Dicharry, Della Croce and Wilder2009; Larsen, 2014; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Venkadesan, Werbel, Daoud, D'Andrea, Davis and Pitsiladis2010; Squadrone & Gallozi, Reference Squadrone and Gallozi2009). However, previous data from the Daasanach and Hadza (Hatala et al., Reference Hatala, Dingwall, Wunderlich and Richmond2013; Pontzer et al., Reference Pontzer, Suchman, Raichlen, Wood, Mabulla and Marlowe2014), and new data presented here from the Tsimane, indicate that plenty of habitually barefoot people in some non-industrial societies run with a rearfoot strike pattern and overstride. Thus, cultural footwear norms alone do not determine good running technique. Nevertheless, it remains very possible that were Daasanach, Hadza and Tsimane people to run more frequently, doing so barefoot as opposed to shod would more strongly encourage them to adopt forefoot or midfoot striking and avoid overstriding. In other words, there are likely to be interaction effects on running techniques between footwear patterns and frequency of running.

Without a doubt, cultural factors are not the only determinants of running techniques in non-industrial societies. Among other potential influences, variation in local environmental conditions probably plays a major role. Experiments in laboratory settings have shown that runners vary their body kinematics when running on different surfaces, including flat vs. sloped surfaces (Gottschall & Kram, Reference Gottschall and Kram2005; Lussiana et al., Reference Lussiana, Fabre, Hébert-Losier and Mourot2013; Padulo et al., Reference Padulo, Annino, Migliaccio, D'Ottavio and Tihanyi2012; Swanson & Caldwell, Reference Swanson and Caldwell2000), even vs. uneven surfaces (Dhawale & Venkadesan, Reference Dhawale and Venkadesan2021; Voloshina & Ferris, Reference Voloshina and Ferris2015) and stiff vs. compliant surfaces (Ferris et al., Reference Ferris, Louie and Farley1998; Pinnington et al., Reference Pinnington, Lloyd, Besier and Dawson2005; Tung et al., Reference Tung, Franz and Kram2014). Natural terrains clearly also vary in terms of slope, evenness and stiffness, making it probable that these and other surface characteristics contribute to shaping running techniques among non-industrial societies. In this study, we found running kinematics among the Tsimane to differ when participants ran on a forest trail vs. in an open setting. In both settings, the running surfaces were relatively flat and consisted of hard-packed dirt. Yet, on the forest trail, participants ran slower with shorter stride lengths and less overstride. Our suspicion is that these differences in running kinematics between forest and open settings are prompted primarily by safety considerations rather than differences in surface characteristics, as previous laboratory studies found little effect of surface irregularity or compliance on stride length (Ferris et al., Reference Ferris, Louie and Farley1998; Voloshina & Ferris, Reference Voloshina and Ferris2015). Forests are cluttered with potential obstacles like fallen branches and roots that may occur on trails, which could lead people to constrain stride length and proceed more slowly and cautiously during running. This, in addition to the fact that animals in forests can easily hide and cannot be seen over long distances, could preclude persistence hunting from being a viable option in certain forest environments (but see Estioko-Griffen, Reference Estioko-Griffen1986). Ultimately, more research is needed to better understand the ways in which environmental variation influences running techniques among non-industrial societies.

Throughout this paper, it has been implied that suboptimal running technique is a kind of default strategy that people are expected to adopt unless they learn to do otherwise. Although this is a hypothesis that remains to be tested, we consider it a reasonable assumption. Specifically, we suspect that among people who rarely run, it feels most natural to use kinematic patterns that are similar to those employed during the much more common activity of walking. During walking, nearly all people in every society, regardless of footwear, usually make initial contact with the ground with their rearfoot and have a roughly fully extended knee (Addison & Lieberman, Reference Addison and Lieberman2015; Alexander, Reference Alexander1992; Holowka et al., Reference Holowka, Wynands, Drechsel, Yegian, Tobolsky, Okutoyi and Lieberman2019; Muybridge, Reference Muybridge1887; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Koch, Holowka and Lieberman2018a; Willems et al., Reference Willems, Stassijns, Cornelis and D'Août2017), including the Tsimane (Holowka et al., Reference Holowka, Kraft, Wallace, Gurven and Venkataramanin press). In part, this is likely because during walking, landing on the rearfoot provides a clear benefit in terms of energy savings (Cunningham et al., Reference Cunningham, Schilling, Anders and Carrier2010). If running is an unfamiliar activity, it would make sense that a person would resort to an established motor programme for a familiar activity such as walking, and as a result be prone to rearfoot striking and overstriding. Moreover, when walking at faster speeds, people almost always take longer strides (Bertram & Ruina, Reference Bertram and Ruina2001), so there may be a natural tendency to transition into running by taking even longer strides, which would also increase the likelihood of rearfoot striking and overstriding. A natural tendency to rearfoot strike may partly explain why even some elite runners have been observed to habitually use a rearfoot striking pattern (Hanley et al., Reference Hanley, Bissas, Merlino and Gruber2019; Hasegawa et al., Reference Hasegawa, Yamauchi and Kraemer2007; Hayes & Caplan, Reference Hayes and Caplan2012). Among these runners, learned adjustments to other aspects of their technique could compensate for any negative consequences of rearfoot striking for injury risk and performance (Williams & Cavanagh, Reference Williams and Cavanagh1987).

When considering variation in running techniques, it is important to keep in mind that all humans possess numerous anatomical and physiological adaptations that make our species better at running long distances compared with other primates. These adaptations almost certainly originated long ago in the human evolutionary lineage, very likely among early members of Homo during the Early Pleistocene (Bramble & Lieberman, Reference Bramble and Lieberman2004). Examples of such adaptations can be found across diverse systems in our bodies including the musculoskeletal system (Bramble & Lieberman, Reference Bramble and Lieberman2004; Eng et al., Reference Eng, Arnold, Biewener and Lieberman2015; Holowka & Lieberman, Reference Holowka and Lieberman2018; Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Raichlen, Pontzer, Bramble and Cutright-Smith2006; Rolian et al., 2008; Venkadesan et al., Reference Venkadesan, Yawar, Eng, Dias, Singh, Tommasini and Mandre2020), cardiopulmonary system (Bramble & Carrier, Reference Bramble and Carrier1983; Callison et al., Reference Callison, Holowka and Lieberman2019; Shave et al., Reference Shave, Lieberman, Drane, Brown, Batterham, Worthington and Baggish2019), thermoregulatory system (Carrier, Reference Carrier1984; Kamberov et al., Reference Kamberov, Guhan, DeMarchis, Jiang, Wright, Morgan and Lieberman2018; Lieberman, Reference Lieberman2015) and nervous system (Raichlen et al., Reference Raichlen, Foster, Gerdeman, Seillier and Giuffrida2012; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Hainline and Lieberman2018b). Thus, from a broad evolutionary perspective, regardless of variation in running techniques, humans are in general ‘good’ runners, or at least we have an evolved natural capacity to run well. Nonetheless, a key takeaway from this study is that even with this species-wide natural capacity, running is still a skill that requires learning and practice for it to be fully mastered. This was presumably just as true among our ancient ancestors as it is for people today. Unfortunately, the degree to which, and ways in which, running techniques varied among ancient cultures are impossible to know. Even so, we suspect that by at least the Middle Pleistocene (probably earlier), as our ancestors were occupying increasingly diverse environments, the efficacy of persistence hunting would have varied among groups in different habitats. This alone would probably have resulted in running being a more common and important activity among some groups than others, which we would expect to give rise to variation in running techniques. Social learning might have been an important process by which ancient runners developed their techniques, similar to how social learning has been hypothesised to have been critical to the development of Paleolithic stone tool manufacturing techniques (e.g. Pargeter et al., Reference Pargeter, Khreisheh and Stout2019; Stout et al., Reference Stout, Rogers, Jaeggi and Semaw2019; Tostevin, Reference Tostevin2012).

In conclusion, much remains to be learned about how culture influences the body techniques employed during running among non-industrial societies, as well as the techniques used in many other types of physical activity. Admittedly, our research with the Tsimane was not fully comprehensive. The kinematic variables analysed were limited to those examined in recent anthropological studies of other non-industrial societies, but more variables could have been considered. Also, kinetic variables were not considered at all. Although good evidence was provided that running is an uncommon activity among the Tsimane, our conclusion that running is not a major part of Tsimane cultural identity was based on informal impressions rather than systematic interviews. Furthermore, we did not investigate the roles that self-optimisation, social learning or other processes might play in the development of Tsimane running techniques. Hopefully, future studies will take a more thorough and multidisciplinary approach to analysing running techniques among non-industrial societies. Nevertheless, we hope that the ideas and evidence presented here will help motivate such future studies. Beyond being of interest from an anthropological perspective, achieving a better understanding of how culture influences running techniques among non-industrial societies would provide exercise scientists, coaches and the general public with a more holistic understanding of the factors influencing running performance and injury risk, as well as a more nuanced view of how humans evolved to run.

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to all the Tsimane who participated in this study. For assistance with fieldwork, we thank Juan Dino Cari Ista, Agustina Bani Cuata, Alberto Vie Tayo, Basilio Vie Tay, Jonathan Stieglitz and other members of the Tsimane Health and Life History Project. For assistance with GPS data analysis, we thank Elizabeth Cashdan, the Spatial Cognition and Navigation Project and the DIGIT Lab at the University of Utah. We also thank Alison Murray and two reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Author contributions

IJW, TSK, VVV and NBH conceived and designed the study. TSK, VVV, HED and MG conducted fieldwork and collected data among the Tsimane. IJW, TSK, HED, NBH and ARH processed data. IJW and TSK performed statistical analyses. IJW wrote the manuscript, with contributions from TSK and NBH. DEL revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Financial support

The study was supported by grants from the American School of Prehistoric Research (Harvard University) and National Institute on Aging (R01AG024119, RF1AG054442). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflicts of interest

All authors declare no conflicts of interests.

Ethics

All participants in this study provided informed consent, and all procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of California Santa Barbara.

Data availability

Data from the Tsimane who participated in the running kinematics trials are available in the Supplementary Material. Data on Tsimane footwear frequency and GPS data used to determine running frequency among the Tsimane are available on reasonable request from MG and HED, respectfully.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.12

References

Abel, F. (1984). Ejen ‘pétrir’: Produire de la nourriture–et du sens? Techniques et Culture, 3, 6580.Google Scholar
Addison, B. J., & Lieberman, D. E. (2015). Tradeoffs between impact loading rate, vertical impulse and effective mass for walkers and heel strike runners wearing footwear of varying stiffness. Journal of Biomechanics, 48, 13181324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, R. M. (1992). The human machine: How the body works. Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Alexander, R. M., & Jayes, A. S. (1983). A dynamic similarity hypothesis for the gaits of quadrupedal mammals. Journal of Zoology, 201, 135152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, O. (2018). Running form: How to run faster and prevent injury. Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Bailey, S. P., & Messier, S. P. (1991). Variations in stride length and running economy in male novice runners subsequent to a seven-week training program. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 12, 299304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bale, J, & Sang, J. (1996). Kenyan running: Movement culture, geography and global change. Routledge.Google Scholar
Barnes, K. R., & Kilding, A. E. (2015). Running economy: Measurement, norms, and determining factors. Sports Medicine Open, 1, 8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassett, D. R. Jr, & Howley, E. T. (2000). Limiting factors for maximum oxygen uptake and determinants of endurance performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 32, 7084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, G. (1977). Les usages sociaux du corps à Bali. Actes de la Recherche en Sciences Sociales, 14, 333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, W. C., & Zingg, R. M. (1935). The Tarahumara: An Indian tribe of northern Mexico. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bertram, J. E., & Ruina, A. (2001). Multiple walking speed-frequency relations are predicted by constrained optimization. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 209, 445453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bramble, D. M., & Carrier, D. R. (1983). Running and breathing in mammals. Science, 21, 251256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bramble, D. M., & Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature, 432, 345352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bransford, D. R., & Howley, E. T. (1977). Oxygen cost of running in trained and untrained men and women. Medicine and Science in Sports, 9, 4144Google ScholarPubMed
Bril, B. (1986). The acquisition of an everyday technical motor skill: The pounding of cereals in Mali (Africa). In Wade, M. G. & Whiting, H. T. A. (Eds.), Themes in motor development (pp. 315326). Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callison, W. É., Holowka, N. B., & Lieberman, D. E. (2019). Thoracic adaptations for ventilation during locomotion in humans and other mammals. Journal of Experimental Biology, 222, jeb189357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrier, D. R. (1984). The energetic paradox of human running and hominid evolution. Current Anthropology, 25, 483495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavanagh, P. R. (1990). The mechanics of distance running: A historical perspective. In Cavanagh, P. R. (Ed.), Biomechanics of distance running (pp. 134). Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Cavanagh, P. R., Pollock, M. L., & Landa, J. (1977). A biomechanical comparison of elite and good distance runners. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 301, 328345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, P. R., & Williams, K. R. (1982). The effect of stride length variation on oxygen uptake during distance running. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 14, 3035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, Y. H., & Kram, R. (1999). Metabolic cost of generating horizontal forces during human running. Journal of Applied Physiology, 86, 16571662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, R. T., & Ngai, S. P. (2016). Effects of footwear on running economy in distance runners: A meta-analytical review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 19, 260266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, D. L., & Damkjaer, S. (2010). Traditional and modern running culture among the Kalenjin of Kenya: A historical and anthropological perspective. In Sands, R. R. & Sands, L. R. (Eds.). The anthropology of sport and human movement: A biocultural perspective (pp. 187212). Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Christensen, D. L., Espino, D., Infante-Ramírez, R., Cervantes-Borunda, M. S., Hernández-Torres, R. P., Rivera-Cisneros, A. E., … Kjaergaard, J. (2017). Transient cardiac dysfunction but elevated cardiac and kidney biomarkers 24 h following an ultra-distance running event in Mexican Tarahumara. Extreme Physiology & Medicine, 6, 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clansey, A. C., Hanlon, M., Wallace, E. S., Nevill, A., & Lake, M. J. (2014). Influence of tibial shock feedback training on impact loading and running economy. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 46, 973981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collier, B. S. (2007). ‘To bring honor to my village’: Steve Gachupin and the community ceremony of Jemez running and the Pike's Peak Marathon. Journal of the West, 46, 6271.Google Scholar
Conley, D. L., & Krahenbuhl, G. S. (1980). Running economy and distance running performance of highly trained athletes. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 12, 357360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connick, M. J., & Li, F. X. (2014). Changes in timing of muscle contractions and running economy with altered stride pattern during running. Gait & Posture, 39, 634637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craighead, D. H., Lehecka, N., & King, D. L. (2014). A novel running mechanic's class changes kinematics but not running economy. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 28, 31373145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowell, H. P., & Davis, I. S. (2011). Gait retraining to reduce lower extremity loading in runners. Clinical Biomechanics, 26, 7883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, C. B., Schilling, N., Anders, C., & Carrier, D. R. (2010). The influence of foot posture on the cost of transport in humans. Journal of Experimental Biology, 213, 790797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallam, G. M., Wilber, R. L., Jadelis, K., Fletcher, G., & Romanov, N. (2005). Effect of a global alteration of running technique on kinematics and economy. Journal of Sports Sciences, 23, 757764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, J. (2014). Daniels’ running formula, 3rd ed. Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Daoud, A. I., Geissler, G. J., Wang, F., Saretsky, J., Daoud, Y. A., & Lieberman, D. E. (2012). Foot strike and injury rates in endurance runners: A retrospective study. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 44, 13251334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, H., Gurven, M., & Cashdan, E. (2022). Navigational experience and the preservation of spatial abilities into old age among tropical horticulturalists. Topics in Cognitive Science, doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2019). Spatial cognition, navigation, and mobility among children in a forager–horticulturalist population, the Tsimané of Bolivia. Cognitive Development, 52, 100800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, I. S., Bowser, B. J., & Mullineaux, D. R. (2016). Greater vertical impact loading in female runners with medically diagnosed injuries: A prospective investigation. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 50, 887892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derrick, T. R. (2004). The effects of knee contact angle on impact forces and accelerations. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 36, 832837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Ruiter, C. J., Verdijk, P. W., Werker, W., Zuidema, M. J., & de Haan, A. (2014). Stride frequency in relation to oxygen consumption in experienced and novice runners. European Journal of Sport Science, 14, 251258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeWit, B., De Clercq, D., & Aerts, P. (2000). Biomechanical analysis of the stance phase during barefoot and shod running. Journal of Biomechanics, 33, 269278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhawale, N., & Venkadesan, M. (2021). How human runners regulate footsteps on uneven terrain. bioRxiv, doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.22.432238Google Scholar
Diebal, A. R., Gregory, R., Alitz, C., & Gerber, J. P. (2012). Forefoot running improves pain and disability associated with chronic exertional compartment syndrome. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 40, 10601067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Divert, C., Mornieux, G., Freychat, P., Baly, L., Mayer, F., & Belli, A. (2008). Barefoot-shod running differences: Shoe or mass effect? International Journal of Sports Medicine, 29, 512518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eng, C. M., Arnold, A. S., Biewener, A. A., & Lieberman, D. E. (2015). The human iliotibial band is specialized for elastic energy storage compared with the chimp fascia lata. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 23822393.Google ScholarPubMed
Estioko-Griffen, A. (1986). Daughters of the forest: Agta women of the Philippines hunt large game animals and still raise their children. Natural History, 95, 3642.Google Scholar
Farnell, B. (1995). Human action signs in cultural context: The visible and the invisible in movement and dance. Scarecrow Press.Google Scholar
Farrell, P. A., Wilmore, J. H., Coyle, E. F., Billing, J. E., & DL, Costill. (1979). Plasma lactate accumulation and distance running performance. Medicine and Science in Sports, 11, 338344.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferris, D. P., Louie, M., & Farley, C. T. (1998). Running in the real world: Adjusting leg stiffness for different surfaces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 265, 989994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, G., Bartlett, R., Romanov, N., & Fotouhi, A. (2008). Pose method technique improves running performance without economy changes. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 3, 365380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folland, J. P., Allen, S. J., Black, M. I., Handsaker, J. C., & Forrester, S. E. (2017). Running technique is an important component of running economy and performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 49, 14121423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gandon, E., Bootsma, R. J., Endler, J. A., & Grosman, L. (2013). How can ten fingers shape a pot? Evidence for equivalent function in culturally distinct motor skills. PLoS One, 8, e81614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerritsen, K. G., van den Bogert, A. J., & Nigg, B. M. (1995). Direct dynamics simulation of the impact phase in heel-toe running. Journal of Biomechanics, 28, 661668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, M. S. (2018). Hopi runners: Crossing the terrain between Indian and American. University Press of Kansas.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottschall, J. S., & Kram, R. (2005). Ground reaction forces during downhill and uphill running. Journal of Biomechanics, 38, 445452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, A. H., Umberger, B. R., Braun, B., & Hamill, J. (2013). Economy and rate of carbohydrate oxidation during running with rearfoot and forefoot strike patterns. Journal of Applied Physiology, 115, 194201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., Jaeggi, A. V., Kaplan, H., & Cummings, D. (2013). Physical activity and modernization among Bolivian Amerindians. PLoS One, 8, e55679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gutierrez, M. (2006). How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of delayed growth. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 454470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M., Stieglitz, J., Trumble, B., Blackwell, A. D., Beheim, B., Davis, H., … Kaplan, H. (2017). The Tsimane Health and Life History Project: Integrating anthropology and biomedicine. Evolutionary Anthropology, 26, 5473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halina, M., Rossano, F., & Tomasello, M. (2013). The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal Cognition, 16, 653666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, F. (1996). Posture in Irish dancing. Visual Anthropology, 8, 251266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamill, J., Russell, E. M., Gruber, A. H., & Miller, R. (2011). Impact characteristics in shod and barefoot running. Footwear Science, 3, 3340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanley, B., Bissas, A., Merlino, S., & Gruber, A. H. (2019). Most marathon runners at the 2017 IAAF World Championships were rearfoot strikers, and most did not change footstrike pattern. Journal of Biomechanics, 92, 5460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasegawa, H., Yamauchi, T., & Kraemer, W. J. (2007). Foot strike patterns of runners at the 15-km point during an elite-level half marathon. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 21, 888893.Google ScholarPubMed
Hatala, K. G., Dingwall, H. L., Wunderlich, R. E., & Richmond, B. G. (2013). Variation in foot strike patterns during running among habitually barefoot populations. PLoS One, 8, e52548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, P., & Caplan, N. (2012). Foot strike patterns and ground contact times during high-calibre middle-distance races. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30, 12751283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heiderscheit, B. C., Chumanov, E. S., Michalski, M. P., Wille, C. M., & Ryan, M. B. (2011). Effects of step rate manipulation on joint mechanics during running. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 43, 296302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. (2016). The story of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 11391148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holowka, N. B., Kraft, T. S., Wallace, I. J., Gurven, M., & Venkataraman, V. V. (in press). Forest terrains influence walking kinematics among indigenous Tsimane of the Bolivian Amazon. Evolutionary Human Sciences.Google Scholar
Holowka, N. B., & Lieberman, D. E. (2018). Rethinking the evolution of the human foot: Insights from experimental research. Journal of Experimental Biology, 221, jeb174425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holowka, N. B., Wynands, B., Drechsel, T. J., Yegian, A. K., Tobolsky, V. A., Okutoyi, P., … Lieberman, D. E. (2019). Foot callus thickness does not trade off protection for tactile sensitivity during walking. Nature, 571, 261264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooper, P. L., Demps, K., Gurven, M., Gerkey, D., & Kaplan, H. S. (2015). Skills, division of labour and economies of scale among Amazonian hunters and South Indian honey collectors. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 370, 20150008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, I., & Smith, G. A. (2007). Preferred and optimal stride frequency, stiffness and economy: Changes with fatigue during a 1-h high-intensity run. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 100, 653661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inizan, M.-L., Reduron-Ballinger, M., Roche, H., & Tixier, J. (1999). Technology and terminology of knapped stone. Cercle de Recherches et d’Études Préhistoriques.Google Scholar
Irigoyen-Rascón, F., & Palma-Batista, J. M. (1994). Rarajípare: The kick ball race of the Tarahumara Indians. Centra Librero de la Prensa, Sucursal Universidad.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. D., Tenforde, A. S., Outerleys, J., Reilly, J., & Davis, I. S. (2020). Impact-related ground reaction forces are more strongly associated with some running injuries than others. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 48, 30723080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaeppler, A. (1985). Structured movement systems in Tonga. In Spencer, P. (Ed.), Society and the dance: The social anthropology of performance and process (pp. 92118). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kamberov, Y. G., Guhan, S. M., DeMarchis, A., Jiang, J., Wright, S. S., Morgan, B. A., … Lieberman, D. E. (2018). Comparative evidence for the independent evolution of hair and sweat gland traits in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 125, 99105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, H., Thompson, R. C., Trumble, B. C., Wann, L. S., Allam, A. H., Beheim, B., … Thomas, G. S. (2017). Coronary atherosclerosis in indigenous South American Tsimane: A cross-sectional cohort study. Lancet, 389, 17301739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendon, A. (1988). Sign languages of aboriginal Australia: Cultural, semiotic, and communicative perspectives. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kerrigan, D. C., Franz, J. R., Keenan, G. S., Dicharry, J., Della Croce, U., & Wilder, R. P. (2009). The effect of running shoes on lower extremity joint torques. Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 1, 10581063.Google ScholarPubMed
Kraft, T. S., Venkataraman, V. V., & Dominy, N. J. (2014). A natural history of human tree climbing. Journal of Human Evolution, 71, 105118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraft, T. S., Venkataraman, V. V., Wallace, I. J., Crittenden, A. N., Holowka, N. B., Stieglitz, J., … Pontzer, H. (2021). The energetics of uniquely human subsistence strategies. Science, 374, eabf0130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, P. (2014). Comparison of foot strike patterns of barefoot and minimally shod runners in a recreational road race. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 3, 137142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, P., Higgins, E., Kaminski, J., Decker, T., Preble, J., Lyons, D., … Normile, A. (2011). Foot strike patterns of recreational and sub-elite runners in a long-distance road race. Journal of Sports Sciences, 29, 16651673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemonnier, P. (1986). The study of material culture today: Towards an anthropology of technology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5, 147186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi, J. M. (1998). The bow and the blanket: Religion, identity, and resistance in Rarámuri material culture. Journal of Anthropological Research, 54, 299–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi, J. M. (2020). Comment on ‘Running in Tarahumara (Rarámuri) culture: Persistence hunting, footracing, dancing, work, and the fallacy of the athletic savage’. Current Anthropology, 61, 369370.Google Scholar
Liebenberg, L. (2006). Persistence hunting by modern hunter–gatherers. Current Anthropology, 47, 10171026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D. E. (2012). What we can learn about running from barefoot running: An evolutionary medical perspective. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 40, 6372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. E. (2014). Strike type variation among Tarahumara Indians in minimal sandals versus conventional running shoes. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 3, 8694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D. E. (2015). Human locomotion and heat loss: An evolutionary perspective. Comprehensive Physiology, 5, 99117.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D. E. (2021). Exercised: Why something we never evolved to do is healthy and rewarding. Pantheon.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D. E., Castillo, E. R., Otarola-Castillo, E., Sang, M. K., Sigei, T. K., Ojiambo, R., … Pitsiladis, Y. (2015a). Variation in foot strike patterns among habitually barefoot and shod runners in Kenya. PLoS One, 10, e0131354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D. E., Mahaffey, M., Cubesare Quimare, S., Holowka, N. B., Wallace, I. J., & Baggish, A. L. (2020). Running in Tarahumara (Rarámuri) culture: Persistence hunting, footracing, dancing, work, and the fallacy of the athletic savage. Current Anthropology, 61, 356379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, D. E., Raichlen, D. A., Pontzer, H., Bramble, D. M., & Cutright-Smith, E. (2006). The human gluteus maximus and its role in running. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 21432155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. E., Venkadesan, M., Werbel, W. A., Daoud, A. I., D'Andrea, S., Davis, I. S., … Pitsiladis, Y. (2010). Foot strike patterns and collision forces in habitually barefoot versus shod runners. Nature, 463, 531535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. E., Warrener, A. G., Wang, J., & Castillo, E. R. (2015b). Effects of stride frequency and foot position at landing on braking force, hip torque, impact peak force and the metabolic cost of running in humans. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 34063414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Eberly, L. E., & Pusey, A. E. (2004). Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees. Nature, 428, 715716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowie, R. H. (1924). Notes on Shoshonean ethnography. Anthropological Papers of the American Museum of Natural History, 20, 183324.Google Scholar
Lumholtz, C. (1902). Unknown Mexico. Scribner's.Google Scholar
Lussiana, T., Fabre, N., Hébert-Losier, K., & Mourot, L. (2013). Effect of slope and footwear on running economy and kinematics. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 23, e246253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahias, M.-C. (1993). Pottery techniques in India: Technical variants and social choice. In Lemonnier, P. (Ed.), Technological choices: Transformations in material cultures since the Neolithic (pp. 157180). Routledge.Google Scholar
Martin, D. Q. (2020). Comment on ‘Running in Tarahumara (Rarámuri) culture: Persistence hunting, footracing, dancing, work, and the fallacy of the athletic savage’. Current Anthropology, 61, 371372.Google Scholar
Mauss, M. (1935). Les techniques du corps. Journal de Psychologie, 32, 271293.Google Scholar
McCarthy, F. D. (1957). Australian Aborigines: Their life and culture. Colorgravure Publications.Google Scholar
McDougall, C. (2009). Born to run: A hidden tribe, superathletes, and the greatest race the world has never seen. Knopf.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, J. E., Howley, E. T., Bassett, D. R. Jr, Thompson, D. L., & Fitzhugh, E. C. (2010). Test of the classic model for predicting endurance running performance. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 42, 991997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messier, S. P., & Cirillo, K. J. (1989). Effects of a verbal and visual feedback system on running technique, perceived exertion and running economy in female novice runners. Journal of Sports Sciences, 7, 113126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, I. S. (2016). Is there an economical running technique? A review of modifiable biomechanical factors affecting running economy. Sports Medicine, 46, 793807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, I. S., Ashford, K. J., Cross, C., Hope, J., Jones, H. S. R., & McCarthy-Ryan, M. (2019). Humans optimize ground contact time and leg stiffness to minimize the metabolic cost of running. Frontiers in Sports and Active Living, 1, 53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, I. S., Jones, A. M., & Dixon, S. J. (2012). Mechanisms for improved running economy in beginner runners. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 44, 17561763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, D., Martin, P., Craib, M., Caruso, C., Clifton, R., & Hopewell, R. (1994). Effect of step length optimization on the aerobic demand of running. Journal of Applied Physiology, 77, 245-51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, D. W., Bransford, D. R., Costill, D. L., Daniels, J. T., Howley, E. T., & Krahenbuhl, G. S. (1995). Variation in the aerobic demand of running among trained and untrained subjects. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 27, 404409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musiba, C. M., Tuttle, R. H., Hallgrímsson, B., & Webb, D. M. (1997). Swift and sure-footed on the savanna: A study of Hadzabe gaits and feet in northern Tanzania. American Journal of Human Biology, 9, 303–321.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muybridge, E. (1887). Animal locomotion. University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Nabokov, P. (1981). Indian running: Native American history and tradition. Ancient City Press.Google Scholar
Napier, C., MacLean, C. L., Maurer, J., Taunton, J. E., & Hunt, M. A. (2019). Kinematic correlates of kinetic outcomes associated with running-related injury. Journal of Applied Biomechanics, 35, 123130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ojiambo, R. M., Easton, C., Casajús, J. A., Konstabel, K., Reilly, J. J., & Pitsiladis, Y. (2012). Effect of urbanization on objectively measured physical activity levels, sedentary time, and indices of adiposity in Kenyan adolescents. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 9, 115123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onywera, V. O., Scott, R. A., Boit, M. K., & Pitsiladis, Y. P. (2006). Demographic characteristics of elite Kenyan endurance runners. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 415–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padulo, J., Annino, G., Migliaccio, G. M., D'Ottavio, S., & Tihanyi, J. (2012). Kinematics of running at different slopes and speeds. Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research, 26, 13311339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pargeter, J., Khreisheh, N., & Stout, D. (2019). Understanding stone tool-making skill acquisition: Experimental methods and evolutionary implications. Journal of Human Evolution, 133, 146166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelosse, J.-L. (1981). Analyse gestuelle de trois procédés de tricotage pratiqués en Europe. Geste et Image, 2, 1343.Google Scholar
Pennington, C. W. (1963). The Tarahumar of Mexico: Their environment and material culture. University of Utah Press.Google Scholar
Perl, D. P., Daoud, A. I., & Lieberman, D. E. (2012). Effects of footwear and strike type on running economy. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 44, 13351343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pétrequin, P. (1993). North wind, south wind: Neolithic technical choices in the Jura Mountains, 3700-2400 BC. In Lemonnier, P. (Ed.), Technological choices: Transformations in material cultures since the Neolithic (pp. 3676). Routledge.Google Scholar
Pinnington, H. C., Lloyd, D. G., Besier, T. F., & Dawson, B. (2005). Kinematic and electromyography analysis of submaximal differences running on a firm surface compared with soft, dry sand. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 94, 242253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pisor, A. C., Gurven, M., Blackwell, A. D., Kaplan, H., & Yetish, G. (2013). Patterns of senescence in human cardiovascular fitness: VO2 max in subsistence and industrialized populations. American Journal of Human Biology, 25, 756769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitsiladis, Y. P., Onywera, V. O., Geogiades, E., O'Connell, W., & Boit, M. K. (2004). The dominance of Kenyans in distance running. Equine and Comparative Exercise Physiology, 1, 285291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pontzer, H., Suchman, K., Raichlen, D. A., Wood, B. M., Mabulla, A. Z. P., & Marlowe, F. W. (2014). Foot strike patterns and hind limb joint angles during running in Hadza hunter–gatherers. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 3, 95101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preece, S. J., Bramah, C., & Mason, D. (2019). The biomechanical characteristics of high-performance endurance running. European Journal of Sports Science, 19, 784792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichlen, D. A., Armstrong, H., & Lieberman, D. E. (2011). Calcaneus length determines running economy: Implications for endurance running performance in modern humans and Neandertals. Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 299308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raichlen, D. A., Foster, A. D., Gerdeman, G. L., Seillier, A., & Giuffrida, A. (2012). Wired to run: Exercise-induced endocannabinoid signaling in humans and cursorial mammals with implications for the ‘runner's high’. Journal of Experimental Biology, 215, 13311336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolian, C., Lieberman, D. E., Hamill, J., Scott, J. W., & Werbel, W. (2009). Walking, running and the evolution of short toes in humans. Journal of Experimental Biology, 212, 713721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roper, J. L., Harding, E. M., Doerfler, D., Dexter, J. G., Kravitz, L., Dufek, J. S., & Mermier, C. M. (2016). The effects of gait retraining in runners with patellofemoral pain: A randomized trial. Clinical Biomechanics, 35, 1422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roux, V. (2019). Ceramics and society: A technological approach to archaeological assemblages. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, P. U., Pyne, D. B., Telford, R. D., & Hawley, J. A. (2004). Factors affecting running economy in trained distance runners. Sports Medicine, 34, 465–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schniter, E., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H. S., Wilcox, N. T., & Hooper, P. L. (2015). Skill ontogeny among Tsimane forager–horticulturalists. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 158, 318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwatka, F. (1893). In the land of the cliff and cave dwellers. Cassell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Severance, P. (1993). The legend of the Tarahumara. Runner's World, 28(2), 7480.Google Scholar
Shave, R. E., Lieberman, D. E., Drane, A. L., Brown, M. G., Batterham, A. M., Worthington, S., … Baggish, A. L. (2019). Selection of endurance capabilities and the trade-off between pressure and volume in the evolution of the human heart. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 116, 1990519910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somjee, S. (1993). Material culture of Kenya. East African Educational Publishers.Google Scholar
Squadrone, R., & Gallozi, C. (2009). Biomechanical and physiological comparison of barefoot and two shod conditions in experienced barefoot runners. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 49, 613.Google ScholarPubMed
Stieglitz, J., Trumble, B. C., Kaplan, H., & Gurven, M. (2017). Horticultural activity predicts later localized limb status in a contemporary pre-industrial population. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 163, 425436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D., Rogers, M. J., Jaeggi, A. V., & Semaw, S. (2019). Archaeology and the origins of human cumulative culture: A case study from the earliest Oldowan at Gona, Ethiopia. Current Anthropology, 60, 309340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, S. C., & Caldwell, G. E. (2000). An integrated biomechanical analysis of high speed incline and level treadmill running. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 32, 11461155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tartaruga, M. P., Brisswalter, J., Peyre-Tartaruga, L. A., Avila, A. O., Alberton, C. L., Coertjens, M., … Kruel, L. F. (2012). The relationship between running economy and biomechanical variables in distance runners. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83, 367375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tostevin, G. B. (2012). Seeing lithics: A middle-range theory for testing for cultural transmission in the Pleistocene. American School of Prehistoric Research Publications.Google Scholar
Trowell, D., Phillips, E., Saunders, P., & Bonacci, J. (2019). The relationship between performance and biomechanics in middle-distance runners. Sports Biomechanics, 31, 111.Google Scholar
Trumble, B. C., Smith, E. A., O'Connor, K. A., Kaplan, H. S., & Gurven, M. D. (2014). Successful hunting increases testosterone and cortisol in a subsistence population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20132876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tung, K. D., Franz, J. R., & Kram, R. (2014). A test of the metabolic cost of cushioning hypothesis during unshod and shod running. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 46, 324329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Worp, M. P., ten Haaf, D. S., van Cingel, R., de Wijer, A., Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W., & Staal, J. B. (2015). Injuries in runners: A systematic review on risk factors and sex differences. PLoS One, 10, e0114937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Oeveren, B. T., de Ruiter, C. J., Beek, P. J., & van Dieën, J. H. (2017). Optimal stride frequencies in running at different speeds. PLoS One, 12, e0184273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venkadesan, M., Yawar, A., Eng, C. M., Dias, M. A., Singh, D. K., Tommasini, S. M., … Mandre, S. (2020). Stiffness of the human foot and evolution of the transverse arch. Nature, 579, 97100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venkataraman, V. V., Kraft, T. S., & Dominy, N. J. (2012). Tree climbing and human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110, 12371242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venkataraman, V. V., Yegian, A. K., Wallace, I. J., Holowka, N. B., Tacey, I., Gurven, M., & Kraft, T. S. (2018). Locomotor constraints favour the evolution of the human pygmy phenotype in tropical rainforests. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 285, 20181492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voloshina, A. S., & Ferris, D. P. (2015). Biomechanics and energetics of running on uneven terrain. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218, 711719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, I. J., Hainline, C., & Lieberman, D. E. (2018b). Sports and the human brain: An evolutionary perspective. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 158, 310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, I. J., Koch, E., Holowka, N. B., & Lieberman, D. E. (2018a). Heel impact forces during barefoot versus minimally shod walking among Tarahumara subsistence farmers and urban Americans. Royal Society Open Science, 5, 180044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, H. (1971). Running, creeping, and climbing: A new ecological and evolutionary perspective on human locomotion. Mankind, 8, 113.Google Scholar
Wilber, R. L., & Pitsiladis, Y. P. (2012). Kenyan and Ethiopian distance runners: What makes them so good? International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance, 7, 92102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wille, C. M., Lenhart, R. L., Wang, S., Thelen, D. G., & Heiderscheit, B. C. (2014). Ability of sagittal kinematic variables to estimate ground reaction forces and joint kinetics in running. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy, 44, 825830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willems, C., Stassijns, G., Cornelis, W., & D'Août, K. (2017). Biomechanical implications of walking with indigenous footwear. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 162, 782793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. (1975). The role of movement in selected symbolic systems. PhD dissertation, Oxford University.Google Scholar
Williams, K. R., & Cavanagh, P. R. (1987). Relationship between distance running mechanics, running economy, and performance. Journal of Applied Physiology, 63, 12361245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, K. R., Cavanagh, P. R., & Ziff, J. L. (1987). Biomechanical studies of elite female distance runners. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 8, 107118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wrangham, R. W., Koops, K., Machanda, Z. P., Worthington, S., Bernard, A. B., Brazeau, N. F., … Muller, M. N. (2016). Distribution of a chimpanzee social custom is explained by matrilineal relationship rather than conformity. Current Biology, 26, 30333037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Variation in foot strike patterns during running: forefoot strike (left), midfoot strike (middle), and rearfoot strike (right). Drawing by Samantha Shields.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Different orientations of the leg (tibia) at foot strike during running: oriented roughly vertically (left) and at a protracted angle, referred to as overstriding (right). Drawing by Samantha Shields.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Tarahumara runner in Mexico landing with a midfoot strike pattern and leg that is oriented roughly vertically. Photo by David Ramos and used here with permission.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Footwear habits among the Tsimane across different age categories.

Figure 4

Figure 5. Kernel density plot of dimensionless speed (Froude number) by sex among the Tsimane who wore GPS units to measure their physical activity patterns. The dashed line indicates the threshold above which dimensionless speed represents running (dimensionless speed >0.5). The plot only includes recorded bouts in which GPS recordings indicated that participants were actively moving (travel speeds >0.5 m/s).

Figure 5

Figure 6. Tsimane study participant in Bolivia running with a rearfoot strike pattern and overstriding.

Figure 6

Figure 7. Running kinematic variables analysed among the Tsimane relative to dimensionless speed (Froude number): dimensionless stride length (a), overstride angle (b), ankle angle (c), and foot strike angle (d). Black circles indicate open setting trials and green triangles indicate forest trail trials.

Figure 7

Figure 8. Association between foot strike and overstride angles among the Tsimane during running. Black circles indicate open setting trials and green triangles indicate forest trail trials.

Supplementary material: File

Wallace et al. supplementary material

Wallace et al. supplementary material

Download Wallace et al. supplementary material(File)
File 19.5 KB