Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T22:26:52.814Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Role of Individual Income in Modulating the Impact of Health Determinants: Exploring the Interplay between Socioeconomic Factors and Health Outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 April 2024

Ignacio Amate-Fortes
Affiliation:
University of Almeria, Faculty of Economics and Business, La Cañada de San Urbano, Ctra. Sacramento s/n, 04120 Almería, Spain. Email: [email protected]
Almudena Guarnido-Rueda
Affiliation:
University of Almeria, Faculty of Economics and Business, La Cañada de San Urbano, Ctra. Sacramento s/n, 04120 Almería, Spain. Email: [email protected]
Diego Martínez-Navarro
Affiliation:
University of Almeria, Faculty of Economics and Business, La Cañada de San Urbano, Ctra. Sacramento s/n, 04120 Almería, Spain. Email: [email protected]
Francisco J. Oliver-Márquez
Affiliation:
University of Almeria, Faculty of Economics and Business, La Cañada de San Urbano, Ctra. Sacramento s/n, 04120 Almería, Spain. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article analyses the self-perceived health of the population surveyed by the European Health Interview Survey (EHIS), with 316,277 observations. The main novelties of this research are: first, we use the econometric technique of quantile regression, which will allow us to distinguish the respondent’s income level. Second, the personal, social, lifestyle and macroeconomic context dimensions of the respondents are considered simultaneously as determinants of self-perceived health. In this way, we will be able to see what the determinants of health are, and whether they vary with income. Finally, it is evident that there are indeed different responses to the same stimulus depending on the level of income, especially in the elasticity of the response, seeing how the higher the income, the more or less the same stimulus influences a person. In addition, it is established that age, nationality and employment status are the most influential variables in self-perceived health.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Academia Europaea Ltd

Introduction

In some cases, self-perceived health is more important than the actual health of an individual, as evidenced by the Placebo Effect, which can improve the health status of the subject if they perceive that they are being treated, and also the relevance of the attitude of the cancer patient in their healing process, as attested by Vidhya et al. (Reference Vidhya, Gupta, Lekshmi, Bhardwaj, Kusum, Kalyani and Gupta2022). For this reason, we believe it is important to understand the determinants of the self-perceived health of the population, as well as to consider whether it varies substantially according to the individual’s income level. In this way, we hope to be able to point out where the focus of attention should be placed with a view to implementing policies that increase the self-perceived health status of the population so that it actually ends up being healthier, and thus improve the country’s health system by being able to focus on those people who suffer from ailments that need to be attended to by a professional. And to find out whether these policies would reach the whole population equally or whether those with high income levels would not be influenced by such measures.

The starting questions of this research are the following: (1) what determines the self-perceived health of the European population? As well as (2) does income influence how the different variables affect the individual’s self-perceived health status? And, in addition to the determinants established in previous research, the question is posed from a macroeconomic perspective: (3) could we include macro magnitudes as relevant variables of self-perceived health?

In order to answer these questions, we will introduce the issue in context by referring to previous studies. The empirical section will then explain the data we have collected for the research and the analytical techniques employed. Next, the results and discussion will be presented. Finally, the article will conclude with the limitations presented and some brief conclusions.

Background

Since the 1980s, the interest of researchers in studying health inequality has increased. But perhaps the work that best reflects the importance of socioeconomic determinants of health is the one conducted by Stringhini et al. (Reference Stringhini, Carmeli, Jokela, Avendaño, Muennig, Guida and Tumino2017). According to these authors, poverty and socioeconomic inequality shorten life more than hypertension, obesity, and excessive alcohol consumption, and they criticize the fact that the World Health Organization (WHO) does not include in its agenda those determinants of health that are as important or more important than others that do form part of its objectives and recommendations.

One of the important issues when conducting this type of study is the choice of the health measure to be analysed (Clarke et al. Reference Clarke, Gerdtham, Johannesson, Bingefors and Smith2002; Ziebarth Reference Ziebarth2010; Amate-Fortes et al. Reference Amate-Fortes, Guarnido-Rueda and Molina-Morales2020). In this sense, we have decided to use self-perceived health as a measure of health. The reliability of these self-assessments has been found to be as good or better than measures such as functional capacity, chronic diseases, and psychological well-being (Shields and Shooshtari, Reference Shields and Shooshtari2001).

Based on the model developed by Dahlgren and Whitehead (Reference Dahlgren and Whitehead1991), which shows the determinants of health in concentric layers, from structural determinants (external layer) to individual lifestyles (internal layer), with at the centre the characteristics of individuals that cannot be modified, such as sex, age or constitutional factors, many authors have tried to explain differences in health using socioeconomic factors. Complementarily, Link and Phelan (Reference Link and Phelan1995) and Phelan et al. (Reference Phelan, Link and Tehranifar2010) show that social conditions are a fundamental cause of health inequalities. Thus, Nolan and Layte (Reference Nolan and Layte2014) point out that there is extensive empirical evidence linking socioeconomic status and health outcomes in both children and adults. For adults, the observed socioeconomic status gradient in health status has been found to be robust to the definition of socioeconomic status (income, wealth, education, social class, etc.) and health (mortality, morbidity) (Palloni et al. Reference Palloni, Milesi, White and Turner2009; Stowasser et al., Reference Stowasser, Heiss, McFadden, Winter and Willis2011). Data for adults are also consistent within and between countries, at all ages and at all points in the distribution of socioeconomic status (Case and Paxson Reference Case and Paxson2010). Therefore, the economic literature has demonstrated the relationship between socioeconomic status and health, and it is often argued that inequalities in physical health are due to direct or indirect material disadvantages (e.g., chronic environmental stress, health resources, etc.). However, as Garrison and Rodgers (Reference Garrison and Rodgers2019) argue, such explanations do not describe finely stratified health differences across the full range of the socioeconomic status gradient. Still, socioeconomic status gradients across the lifespan are crucial to understanding health inequalities in older adults (Harber-Aschan et al. Reference Harber-Aschan, Calderón-Larrañaga, Darin-Mattson, Hu, Fratiglioni and Dekhtyar2020).

Shields and Shooshtari (Reference Shields and Shooshtari2001) laid the definitive foundations for what are considered to be the fundamental determinants of self-perceived health, these being age, gender, physical determinants such as mobility, pain or chronic conditions as well as socioeconomic factors such as educational level, income and marital status. In later years, these determinants were refined, as is evident in work such as that of Cornelisse-Vermaat et al. (Reference Cornelisse-Vermaat, Antonides, Van Ophem and Van Den Brink2006) where body mass index, being unemployed and smoking, among others, are already included. In this context, we take these variables traditionally understood as determinants of health and apply them to the different quantiles of the sample in order to contrast whether they differ in their effects on different individuals. In general terms, as might be expected, there are relationships contrasted by these and subsequent authors that indicate that as BMI increases, age, and this whole group of variables associated with negative or limiting factors have negative impacts on self-perceived health, while factors such as being married, being employed and having high levels of education are associated with better results on self-perceived health. The topicality of these variables is evidenced in studies such as Tadiri et al. (Reference Tadiri, Gisinger, Kautzky-Willer, Kublickiene, Herrero, Norris and Pilote2021) and Su et al. (Reference Su, D’Arcy, Li and Meng2022).

Likewise, authors such as Fleury-Bahi et al. (Reference Fleury-Bahi, Préau, Annabi-Attia, Marcouyeux and Wittenberg2015) and Goldberg et al. (Reference Goldberg, Wheeler, Burnett, Mayo, Valois, Brophy and Giannetti2015) point out how self-perceived health is influenced by conditioning factors such as exposure to air pollution. In addition, the work of Huang (Reference Huang2020) points out that not only does the situation itself, such as air pollution in this case, have an influence, but also the coverage and treatment given to it in the media. Based on this evidence, this article incorporates macro-magnitudes such as CO2 emissions as possible determinants of self-perceived health status, in accordance with previous work, as well as the HDI, which reflects the country’s level of development, and the Gini index, which indicates the country’s level of inequality. With these variables, we aim to contrast whether the country’s environment influences the individual’s self-perceived health through emotions such as empathy in line with studies such as Kirman and Teschl (Reference Kirman and Teschl2010) and Riess (Reference Riess2017).

Another important issue, perhaps less addressed by the economic literature, is the methodology of the empirical analysis. In this article we use quantile regression. This technique is a valuable tool for analysing data that may not fit the assumptions of mean regression and allow the conditional distribution of the outcome variable to be examined across different quantiles, providing a more complete understanding of the relationship between variables.

Considering that most of the works applying this methodology in health study the determinants of health costs (Hu et al. Reference Hu, Li, Ji and Sanderson2020; Li et al. Reference Li, Hu, Ji, Mckendrick, Moreno, Kelley and Aldridge2022) and the factors affecting child malnutrition (Sharaf et al. Reference Sharaf, Mansour and Rashad2019; Aheto Reference Aheto2020; Rahman and Hossain Reference Rahman and Hossain2020), our work aims to further deepen the analysis of the socioeconomic determinants of differences in health, measured through self-perceived health, and using the methodology of quantile regression, which will allow us to study how the effects on health of the analysed socioeconomic determinants vary according to the different income deciles.

Empirical Analysis

Details of where the data were obtained from, and the analysis technique used will be given below to clarify any doubts about the data.

Data

The data used in this article are drawn from The European Health Interview Survey (EHIS). This compact database surveys the population aged 15 years and older in European countries. This article uses EHIS 2, the second wave of the survey, which was conducted during 2013 and 2015 and includes the EU member states as well as Switzerland and Turkey. EHIS 3 was planned to be published in 2021, with surveys starting to be conducted in 2019, however, this was not possible due to the global pandemic caused by COVID-19. Moreover, it is conceivable that the surveys obtained for this latest wave would be contaminated by the health situation, which undoubtedly affects self-perceived health status. For both reasons, because of both unavailability and the intention to study health in a normal context, we consider it appropriate to work with EHIS 2, a version that includes more countries, more variables and is more up to date than EHIS 1, which was conducted between 2003 and 2006.

Macro-magnitudes such as carbon dioxide (CO2) emissions, the human development index (HDI) and the Gini index have also been included in the analysis. These variables have been extracted, respectively, from the World Bank, UNDP and the Standardized World Income Inequality Database (SWIID) reports published by Solt (Reference Solt2020). It is noteworthy that the data used in this cross-sectional study refer to the mean value of the macro-magnitude for the period 2013–2015, which is the year to which the survey refers. In doing so, we aim to capture the macroeconomic environment in which the individuals find themselves. The absence of GDP per capita is due to the fact that it is included in the HDI, as well as being found indirectly in the survey itself, since one question indicates the respondent’s income in relation to the median income of the country and this variable is what classifies the sample into the different quantiles.

Based on the EHIS 2, several variables have been developed that will form the basis of this analysis. Tables 1, 2, 3 and 4 show the descriptive statistics for each of the quartiles into which the sample has been divided. Furthermore, examining these data, we can see that the mean of the Education variable (the individual’s level of education) is higher as we move from one quantile to the next, i.e., the higher the income, the more educated the individuals. This is intended as a sign that the data show expected results; it does not claim that they have more income because they have a higher level of education, nor that the reason they have a higher level of education is because they have a higher income. In addition, the constructed variables will be specified below.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the first quartile (Q1).

Note: All data provided are unweighted averages.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the second quartile (Q2).

Note: All data provided are unweighted averages.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the third quartile (Q3).

Note: All data provided are unweighted averages.

Table 4. Descriptive statistics for the maximum values.

Note: All data provided are unweighted averages.

In order to take into account some particular aspects of the individual, the following physical health variables have been designed:

  • Age range: These variables take the value 1 if the respondent in the household has an age in the range given next to the variable in brackets and 0 otherwise. the age ranges given by EHIS are:

    • Age25-34 (25–34 years old)

    • Age35-44 (35–44 years old)

    • Age45-54 (45–54 years old)

    • Age55-64 (55–64 years old)

    • Age65-74 (65–74 years old)

    • Age75-84 (75–84 years old)

    • Age85 (85 years old and older)

The variable Age15-24 (15–24 years old) is omitted from the analysis to use the youngest segment of the population as a reference when interpreting the results.

  • Male which will indicate the gender of the respondent, with 1 indicating that the subject is male and 0 otherwise.

  • BMI will inform us of the ratio between the respondent’s weight and height, which will give us an approximation of his physical health with respect to his body mass.

In order to situate each individual in the social context, we will take into account the variables:

  • EuropeanBorn which will take the value 1 if the respondent was born in an EU country, and 0 otherwise.

  • URBANzone which will take the value 1 if they live in an urban area, and 0 otherwise.

  • Spouse which will take the value 1 if they live with their partner, and 0 otherwise.

  • Employee which will take value 1 if the respondent is employed, and 0 otherwise.

  • PeopleHousehold which will indicate how many people live in the household.

  • Education which will indicate the level of education and can take the value 0 (kindergarten) up to 8 (doctorate). The intermediate values correspond to having primary education (2), having lower secondary education (3), having upper secondary education (4), having a higher than secondary but not tertiary education (4), having short-cycle tertiary education (5), having a degree or equivalent (6) and finally the remaining value corresponds to having a master’s degree (7).

On the other hand, to find out about the respondent’s lifestyle habits, we have:

  • Ncigarette which counts the number of cigarettes smoked on a regular basis.

  • CardioDays which counts the number of days in a typical week on which physical sports, fitness or recreational activities that cause at least a small increase in breathing or heart rate for at least 10 minutes continuously are performed.

  • FreqAlcohol counts the amount of alcoholic beverages consumed in the past 12 months.

  • UnitsFrutVeg counts the units of fruit and vegetables consumed regularly.

  • FluVaccine indicates how long it has been since the last flu vaccination, the values being 1 if vaccinated less than 12 months ago; 2 if more than 12 months ago; and 3 if never vaccinated for flu.

Finally, to know the self-perceived health status of the respondent, we have the variable SPHS (Self-Perceived Health Status), which takes the following values depending on the self-perceived health status of the respondent: 1 for very bad, 2 for bad, 3 for fair, 4 for good and 5 for very good. On the other hand, the variable that will order the quantiles to run the technique will be income. While income is only one dimension of socioeconomic status, we can use it as a good proxy as, according to Shavers (Reference Shavers2007), income signifies the consistent influx of financial resources throughout a specific timeframe. Individuals with higher income levels are more inclined to possess the financial capacity to cover healthcare expenses and access superior nutrition, such as a wider assortment of fresh fruits and vegetables. Additionally, they have the means to secure better housing, education, and recreational opportunities. Advocacy of income as a valid measure of socio-economic status can also be found in Galobardes et al. (Reference Galobardes, Shaw, Lawlor, Lynch and Smith2006), Do (Reference Do2009), Kim (Reference Kim2011) and Hoff and Laursen (Reference Hoff and Laursen2019).

Once all the variables have been presented, it is clarified that there are no problems of missing data in the regression because only data from those respondents who have provided complete information have been used for the estimation of the EHIS. There are some missing data with respect to the BMI variable, but since it is a control variable rather than a fundamental variable in the analysis and there is still enough information available for the results to be efficient, no additional actions have been taken.

Estimation Technique

The analysis technique used in this paper will be quantile regression, also known as least-absolute-value models (LAV or MAD) and minimum L1-norm models. This technique was introduced in the statistical community mainly by Koenker and Hallock (Reference Koenker and Hallock2001) and allows us to analyse how the effects of the explanatory variables change on the dependent variable as we move through the different percentiles into which the analysed sample is divided. Although there are few studies, quantile regression has been used successfully in medical research (Arenz et al. Reference Arenz, Rückerl, Koletzko and von Kries2004; Harder et al. Reference Harder, Bergmann, Kallischnigg and Plagemann2005; Owen et al. Reference Owen, Martin, Whincup, Smith and Cook2005; Beyerlein et al. Reference Beyerlein, Toschke and Von Kries2008). In all these cases, the authors showed the importance of performing the analyses through percentiles rather than through mean values (Beyerlein Reference Beyerlein2014). Likewise, tests have been run to verify that the results will be efficient and have been adequately passed. Table 5 shows the VIF of the variables that show that the results will be valid because they are less than 5, and therefore, it is assumed that there are no collinearity problems that could perturb the estimates.

Table 5. Variance Inflation Factors (VIFs).

The objective of quantile regression is the same as ordinary least squares (OLS) linear regression, i.e., to model the relationship between the variables analysed. However, unlike OLS estimation, quantile regression allows the possibility of estimating different regression lines for different quantiles of the endogenous variable. The quantiles we will estimate are $\theta $ = 0.25; $\theta $ = 0.5; and $\theta $ = 0.75. Thus, the model specification is presented in equation (1):

(1) $${Y_i} = {X_{i\;}}{\beta _\theta } + {u_{\theta i}}$$

where, ${Y_i}\;$ is the endogenous variable, ${X_{i\;}}\;$ represents the matrix of exogenous variables, ${\beta _\theta }$ is the parameter to estimate corresponding to the quantile $\theta $ and ${u_{\theta i}}\;$ is the random disturbance corresponding to the quantile $\theta $ . Analogous to the OLS estimation technique, which states that $E\left( {{y_i}|{x_i}} \right) = {X_{i\;}}{\hat \beta _{{\rm{OLS}}}}$ and hence that $E\left( {{u_i}|{X_i}} \right) = 0$ , in quantile regression it is assumed that $Quan{t_\theta }\left( {{y_i}|{x_i}} \right) = {X_{i\;}}{\beta _\theta }$ which implies that $Quan{t_\theta }\left( {{u_{\theta i}}|{x_i}} \right) = 0,\;$ this being the only assumption made about the random perturbation in this technique. In parallel to the way quantile regression is approached from the OLS technique, its problem can be posed from the same technique. After its development, the parameter estimation problem in quantile regression is expressed as in (2):

(2) $${\rm{Min}}\;{\beta _\theta } \in \mathbb{R}\;\left[ {\mathop \sum \nolimits_{{Y_i} \geq {X_{i{\beta _\theta }\;}}} \theta \left| {{Y_i} - {X_{i\;}}{\beta _\theta }} \right| + \mathop \sum \nolimits_{{Y_i} \geq {X_{i{\beta _\theta }\;}}} \left( {1 - \theta } \right)\left| {{Y_i} - {X_{i\;}}{\beta _\theta }} \right|} \right]$$

In this way, it is easy to see how what is done is a minimization of the absolute deviations weighted with asymmetric weights, i.e., each deviation corresponding to observation i is given more or less weight depending on the quantile whose regression line is being estimated. The advantage of using absolute values instead of squared deviations is that outliers do not alter the estimation so much, since it penalizes the errors linearly, whereas other techniques such as OLS increase the importance of outliers that increase the errors quadratically by squaring the errors. This characteristic of quantile regression makes it especially useful when the data have some kind of censoring, as in this case of the income variable, since what is really relevant is whether the estimated value is above ( ${y_i} \lt {X_{i\;}}{\beta _\theta }$ ) or below ( ${y_i} \geq {X_{i\;}}{\beta _\theta }$ ) the real data, not its magnitude. With reference to the introduction of asymmetric weights, taking into account that several regression lines passing through different points of the distribution will be estimated, the function of the asymmetric weights is precisely to place these different lines by weighting differently the positive and the negative residuals.

The equation to be estimated is as follows:

$$\begin{gathered} SPH{S_i} = \alpha + \beta _1^\theta Age25 - {34_i} + \beta _2^\theta Age35 - {44_i} + \beta _3^\theta Age45 - {54_i} + \beta _4^\theta Age55 - {64_i} \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ \beta _5^\theta Age65 - {74_i} + \beta _6^\theta Age75 - {84_i} + \beta _7^\theta Age{85_i} + \beta _8^\theta Mal{e_i} + \beta _9^\theta BM{I_i} \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ \beta _{10}^\theta EuropeanBor{n_i} + \beta _{11}^\theta URBANzon{e_i} + \beta _{12}^\theta Spous{e_i} + \beta _{13}^\theta Employe{e_i} \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ \beta _{14}^\theta PeopleHousehol{d_i} + \beta _{15}^\theta Educatio{n_i} + \beta _{16}^\theta Ncigarett{e_i} + \beta _{17}^\theta CardioDay{s_i} \\ \,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,\,+ \beta _{18}^\theta FreqAlcoho{l_i} + \beta _{19}^\theta UnitsFrutVe{g_i} + \beta _{20}^\theta FluVaccin{e_i} + \beta _{21}^\theta C{O_{2i}} \\ \!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!\!+ \beta _{22}^\theta HD{I_i} + \beta _{23}^\theta Gin{i_i} + \varepsilon _i^\theta \\ \end{gathered}$$

where $\theta $ defines the quantile for which the estimation is being performed; i denotes the individual to which the data belong and $\varepsilon _i^\theta $ is the model error term of the same quantile.

This equation will then be estimated using the logistic regression conditional on each quantile. This regression will allow us to perform a robustness analysis on the data obtained previously in the quantile regression, although it will be less powerful because it will not use all the data available in the quantile regression, which uses all the data even if it is only providing results for one of the quantiles. Likewise, for logistic regression a binary variable is needed. For this reason, for the logistic regressions, the variable SPHSbi will be used, which will be a binary version of SPHS. This binary version of SPHS (SPHSbi) will take the value 1 if the original SPHS variable takes the value 4 (good) or 5 (very good) and zero otherwise.

Results

The results shown in Table 6 are notable for being consistent with what is expected for each variable and the quartile to which it refers. That said, it should be noted that the coefficient of determination is low, which implies that there are effects that are not captured by all the variables included in the model. Nevertheless, the individual variables are significant, so that we can affirm that there is a relationship between them and self-perceived health and therefore serve as a basis for drawing conclusions. In addition, the constant term of the regression shows positive results and is considerably higher than the other coefficients, which indicates that individuals tend to claim that they have very good levels of self-perceived health. This can also be seen in Tables 1, 2, 3 and 4 of descriptive statistics, where the mean of the SPHS variable ranges from 3.49 to 3.9 in a variable that ranges between 0 and 5. Likewise, the frequency of the variable SPHS has been calculated and by adding the percentages of categories 4 and 5 (42.61% and 21.93% respectively) we can affirm that 64.54% of the population believe they have a good level of self-perceived health or higher. On the other hand, 11.91% say they have a poor state of health. And the rest, 23.55%, remain in a state of neutrality.

Table 6. Estimation results for each quartile.

Note: * Significant at 10%; ** significant at 5%; *** significant at 1%.

Table 6 also shows the results of the robustness analysis in which the same equation used in the quantile regression is estimated, but this time using a logistic regression conditional on the data belonging to individuals of a certain quantile. Although these estimates also show significant results, they use only the data resulting from segmenting the data by quartiles, in contrast to what occurs in quantile regression where, although only one quantile is being calculated, all the data are taken into consideration. These regressions will be used to contrast the results obtained through quantile regression.

Discussion

From Shields and Shooshtari (Reference Shields and Shooshtari2001) to Cohen and Flood (Reference Cohen and Flood2022) there is a consensus that people tend to feel less healthy as they get older. This perception is captured by the variables: Age25-34, Age35-44, …, Age85. This set of variables, which is significant in all the quantiles, includes the age brackets of the sample, omitting Age15-24, which corresponds to the youngest bracket. In this way, it is interpreted that the older the subject is, the less self-perceived health they report in the survey in general terms. This is mainly due to the following reasons: accumulation of health problems (Idler and Benyamini Reference Idler and Benyamini1997), increased awareness of health limitations (Jylhä Reference Jylhä2009) and shifting comparative standards, i.e. older adults may compare their health status with that of younger, healthier people, leading them to perceive a deterioration in their health (Schnittker and Bacak Reference Schnittker and Bacak2014). Finally, taking into account the coefficients obtained for the age variables, which are among the highest in absolute value, it can be seen that age is one of the most important determinants for approximating the self-perceived health of a subject.

The variable Male is significant in the whole sample, concluding that as a rule men report higher levels of self-perceived health than women. This occurs because women tend to value aspects related to their physique more than men, generating complexes and fears that men develop less or do not suffer from. This fact has been evidenced in other studies such as that of Godoy-Bermúdez et al. (Reference Godoy-Bermúdez, Rojo-Gallego-Burin, Delgado-Márquez, Martín-Martín, Sánchez-Martínez and del Amo-González2022) in which they also point out that women, unlike men, do not feel disadvantaged by being unemployed. Furthermore, according to Danylova (Reference Danylova2020), and linking this variable to the next one, exposure to visual media presents an idealized concept of beauty that can have a detrimental impact on one’s self-perception. The beauty industry’s emphasis on youth and thinness contributes to a negative body image, leading to increased dissatisfaction among both women and men, albeit with a greater impact on women. Danylova (Reference Danylova2020) argues that the internalization of modern standards of female beauty, centred on youthful appearance, hampers women’s psychological development and creates internal conflicts that disrupt the harmony of the psyche. This disintegration of self-image gives rise to profound contradictions. In addition, the Male variable also has a coefficient that is not negligible compared with the others, which shows that age and sex, factors that are given to us and that we cannot confront, such as the passage of time, have a great impact on our self-perceived health, according to pioneering studies such as those by Shields and Shooshtari (Reference Shields and Shooshtari2001) and Cornelisse-Vermaat et al. (Reference Cornelisse-Vermaat, Antonides, Van Ophem and Van Den Brink2006) or the more recent studies by Tadiri et al. (Reference Tadiri, Gisinger, Kautzky-Willer, Kublickiene, Herrero, Norris and Pilote2021) and Su et al. (Reference Su, D’Arcy, Li and Meng2022). Finally, it can be seen that for the 0.25-quantile the coefficient is 0.052 while for the 0.5-quantile and 0.75-quantile it is 0.088 and 0.087 respectively. Showing that men with more income feel healthier than their lower-income counterparts, therefore, it is beginning to be intuited that people with more income show better levels of self-perceived health. Furthermore, although our results highlight these differences, it is worth noting that the issue of gender differences in health is truly complex. Studies such as Schmitz and Lazarevič (Reference Schmitz and Lazarevič2020) assert that, in general, the presence of a health gap between men and women cannot be considered a universal finding, as the gap tends to widen, shrink, or even reverse with age, depending on the indicator and the country. Additionally, decades ago, Macintyre et al. (Reference Macintyre, Hunt and Sweeting1996) concluded that sex differences in morbidity were not a generic issue either; it was important to differentiate between psychological disorders and physical conditions, as these influenced females differently. Finally, Bambra et al. (Reference Bambra, Albani and Franklin2021), in relation to COVID-19, once again reaffirm that although women are more likely to be diagnosed with COVID-19, it is men who have a higher mortality rate. In conclusion, this variable in this study aims to be a control variable rather than a study focused on gender differences, as the issue of gender differences in health deserves its own investigation.

As can be foreseen before starting the analysis, the BMI variable reports negative and significant results in the whole sample, i.e., the more obese a person is, the less self-perceived health they report in the surveys. This validates that society knows and feels that obesity is a problem for the health of the individual, as well as a risk factor for a multitude of diseases as attested by Samadoulougou et al. (Reference Samadoulougou, Idzerda, Letarte, McKay, Quesnel-Vallée and Lebel2022), who highlight that obesity is often associated with disorders related to depression and anxiety. This is also influenced by Danylova’s (Reference Danylova2020) beauty industry, in which people with higher BMIs are those furthest removed from modern standards of beauty. Finally, the coefficient associated with BMI increases as the wealth quantile increases, with coefficients –0.017, –0.032 and –0.044 from the lowest to the highest income. Therefore, it is shown that the penalty on self-perceived health with respect to BMI is greater as income increases. Thus, being overweight or obese is more stigmatizing for people with high income.

The variable EuropeanBorn shows positive results across the whole sample, showing evidence in favour of respondents born in an EU country, regardless of whether they reside in their country of birth or in another European country, feeling healthier than respondents who were not born in an EU country. Moreover, the difference in self-perceived health increases as income increases. Thus, in the first quantile, Europeans report 0.19 more points than non-Europeans, while in the third quantile, the difference is 0.30. This division between Europeans and non-Europeans in self-perceived health that is reaffirmed in this article was already exposed a decade ago by Nielsen and Krasnik (Reference Nielsen and Krasnik2010). In the Nielsen and Krasnik (Reference Nielsen and Krasnik2010) study it was also found that immigrants and minority ethnic groups had systematically lower self-perceived health regardless of age, gender and other socioeconomic factors. It should also be noted that this variable is the second most relevant variable in terms of magnitude in determining the respondent’s self-perceived health. Finally, Nappo (Reference Nappo2022) points out that this is based on job security, which reduces the likelihood of claiming good health, assuming that immigrants have less job security than natives of European countries. Also, immigrants in particular are the most exposed to discrimination owing to possible cultural clashes and language barriers to communication, in addition to the fact that these immigrants have other unfavourable conditions compared with natives, such as not living with their whole family and not feeling the social support of their environment.

Despite what was expected, the variable URBANzone has turned out to be non-significant, so we cannot draw any conclusions. However, this variable has not been omitted from the regression because it is useful to control for where the population lives to improve the explanatory power of the other variables in the model.

Contrary to the findings of Shields and Shooshtari (Reference Shields and Shooshtari2001) in their study, the Spouse variable shows that those who live with their partner perceive greater self-perceived health, except for those in the first wealth quantile, who do not show significance in this variable. These results also reaffirm the work of Saravanakumar et al. (Reference Saravanakumar, Garrett, Van Wissen, Montayre and McBride-Henry2022) and build on the evidence shown by Su et al. (Reference Su, D’Arcy, Li and Meng2022) that married individuals generally report higher satisfaction than single individuals. This is because, as postulated by Umberson et al. (Reference Umberson, Crosnoe and Reczek2010), marriage often provides social support and a sense of emotional well-being, which can positively influence a person’s overall health and well-being. This study expanded on the work of Williams and Umberson (Reference Williams and Umberson2004) in which they argued that married people may also have access to shared resources and greater economic stability, which may contribute to improved health outcomes. Moreover, as with the previous variables, income amplifies the effect of the variable, with a coefficient of 0.087 for 0.5-quantile and 0.119 for 0.75-quantile. Thus, there is a higher correlation between living with a partner and self-perceived health among people with higher income.

Similarly, Employee also shows positive coefficients, i.e., individuals who are employed report higher self-perceived health. This is explained by the fact that it is more socially acceptable to have a job, and that it can be a source of motivation and self-improvement. Thus, those who work are in better health than those who are not at work, who may feel useless and depressed, as well as burdened by economic problems and survival methods. These results are supported by previous studies such as Godoy-Bermudez et al. (Reference Godoy-Bermúdez, Rojo-Gallego-Burin, Delgado-Márquez, Martín-Martín, Sánchez-Martínez and del Amo-González2022) or Tattarini and Grotti (Reference Tattarini and Grotti2022). In addition, this is the most important variable due to its magnitude in determining the self-perceived health of the individual for the reasons given above. This variable is one of the most relevant in determining self-perceived health in our regression because it shows the highest coefficient in absolute value, even higher than the coefficients for age and gender. For the 0.25-quantile the coefficient is 0.29 while for the 0.5-quantile and 0.75-quantile the coefficient is 0.38. Thus, being employed is associated with higher levels of self-perceived health for those with higher levels of income, probably due to the economic stability it provides, among other factors.

Next, we have the variable PeopleHousehold which captures how many individuals the respondent shares the household with. This variable is significant for the first (with a coefficient of 0.027) and second (with a coefficient of 0.031) quartiles, so it can be deduced that people with high purchasing power do not feel their health is increased by living with more people. This effect, which is found in people with less wealth, may be justified by the security and support provided by having support people nearby. According to Shields (Reference Shields2008) people who are socially isolated or have few connections are more likely to suffer from physical and mental ailments, and therefore to die prematurely. For this reason, this ratio could be a case of self-perceived health being enhanced by social interaction since, remember, humanity is social by nature, and this helps us to feel better, as recently reaffirmed by Saravanakumar et al. (Reference Saravanakumar, Garrett, Van Wissen, Montayre and McBride-Henry2022).

Education shows standard results that those with higher levels of education are expected to report higher levels of self-perceived health to the extent that they are likely to feel more well-being than those with lower levels of education. We find that for the 0.25-quantile the coefficient is 0.056 while for the 0.5-quantile and 0.75-quantile the coefficients are 0.087 and 0.089, respectively, i.e. again we find the amplifying effect of income on the determinants of self-perceived health. These results are supported by Kaleta et al. (Reference Kaleta, Makowiec-Dabrowska, Dziankowska-Zaborszczyk and Jegier2006) who specifically found that women with primary education are 2.5 times more likely to have a poor self-perceived health status than women with a university education. Recently, Wärnberg et al. (Reference Wärnberg, Pérez-Farinós, Ajejas-Bazán, Pérez-López, Benavente-Marín, Crespo-Oliva and Barón-López2021) have generalized for both men and women that lower levels of education are associated with poorer self-perceived health status. It should be noted that this variable, more than any other, should be taken with caution, since causality is bidirectional, not only are those who have a higher level of education more likely to obtain highly paid jobs, but also those who belong to families with high purchasing power are more likely to offer a high level of education to their descendants, so this variable would require a study of its own that covers it in greater depth with other techniques that allow this relationship to be better clarified.

With respect to the variable Ncigarette, as expected, it shows negative results on self-perceived health as Axon et al. (Reference Axon, Jang, Son and Pham2022) point out. This result is evident from the fact that the harmful effects of smoking on health are well known, as medical studies have shown, as pioneered by Fielding (Reference Fielding1985) and more recently claimed by Schäfer et al. (Reference Schäfer, Santos, Quadra, Dumith and Meller2022). However, this coefficient is significant only for individuals in the first quantile, perhaps because they do not have sufficient income to afford a better diet to subtly mitigate its effects. Even so, the coefficient that is significant has the smallest value of all of Table 6, this value being –0.003. So, a priori, although smoking is a factor to be taken into account in self-perceived health, its impact is less than that of other determinants.

Likewise, CardioDays shows that, except for individuals in the third quantile, those who engage in physical sporting activities feel healthier, and this feeling increases the more days they practise these activities, as Lamb et al. (Reference Lamb, Roberts and Brodie1990) pointed out and has been contrasted by later authors such as Piko (Reference Piko2000) and Alić et al. (Reference Alić, Basioli Kasap and Jenko Miholić2021). Thus, we find that for the 0.25-quantile the coefficient is 0.041 and for the 0.5-quantile the coefficient is 0.78, so that for middle-income individuals sporting activity seems to offer better results in terms of self-perceived health, and this difference may be due to the fact that those with higher incomes practise this sport activity with better equipment, which improves their perception. Based on the value of the coefficients of this variable when compared with the others, it is evident that doing physical activities that increase the heart rate has a great influence on the self-perceived health status of the individual, since the coefficients are among the highest in the 0.25-quantile and 0.5-quantile. In quantile three, where the coefficient is not significant, we could think that less sporting activities are practised at a general level and for this reason this effect is not included.

The alcohol consumption variable FreqAlcohol is certainly unexpected according to common sense, since it significantly indicates that the higher the frequency of drinking alcohol, the higher the self-perceived health, with this effect increasing as the individual’s wealth increases (from the lowest to the highest income level, the coefficients obtained are 0.041, 0.054, 0.057, 0.058, 0.058 and 0.059). This observation can also be found in Olsson et al. (Reference Olsson, Currow and Ekström2022) where they find that alcohol consumption is associated with an improvement in self-perceived health. However, as Samadoulougou et al. (Reference Samadoulougou, Idzerda, Letarte, McKay, Quesnel-Vallée and Lebel2022) point out, alcohol consumption is a risk factor for obesity, i.e., for having a high body mass index, which has been shown to have a detrimental effect on self-perceived health. It may be the case that alcohol consumption, discounting its effects on the individual’s weight, does report health satisfaction, especially among higher-income individuals, who can afford higher-priced alcoholic beverages, which may lead them to believe that they are healthier than their cheaper analogues.

With respect to diet, the UnitsFrutVeg variable indicates that those who eat more units of fruit and vegetables feel healthier, as Martins et al. (Reference Martins, Capitão, Fialho, Feteira-Santos, Virgolino, Santos and Santos2022) report in the first quantile of wealth (with a coefficient of 0.016). This observation is to be expected were it not for the fact that in the second and third quantile the result is not significant, perhaps because the diets of wealthier people are more varied and they not only feel healthier for eating fruit and vegetables, but also other foods such as fish, and other foods that are not considered fruit or vegetables, such as chia or oats. This would be in line with Jayawardena et al. (Reference Jayawardena, Sooriyaarachchi, Chourdakis, Jeewandara and Ranasinghe2020) who argue that self-perceived health will increase with all foods contributing to a balanced nutritional status. In order to incorporate dietary issues into self-perceived health in a more accurate way, a study focused on these issues would be required, as food will undoubtedly influence our perception of our health, and considering only fruit and vegetables is an approximation given by the database used.

The FluVaccine variable, indicating how long it has been since the last dose of flu vaccine, was positive but not significant, so we cannot be sure that those who are vaccinated against flu report higher levels of self-perceived health, the most we can surmise is that the vaccinated group are more actively concerned about their health than those who are not vaccinated, either willingly or because of a need arising from a personal condition such as diabetes or asthma. The study by Davis and Geneus (Reference Davis and Geneus2016) shows that those with physical limitations or non-mental health conditions are more likely to be vaccinated against influenza.

Finally, the variables CO 2 , Gini and HDI only show significant results in the third quantile, i.e., it can be stated that only individuals with higher income are affected by the macroeconomic situation of their country on their self-perceived health. That said, as expected, CO2 emissions with a coefficient of –0.013 and economic inequality with a coefficient of –0.011 will be felt as a loss of self-perceived health. while living in a country with high levels of human development (i.e., the HDI results with a coefficient of 0.412) is linked to higher levels of self-perceived health. Similar evidence can be found in Prędkiewicz et al. (Reference Prędkiewicz, Bem, Siedlecki, Kowalska and Robakowska2022) where they point out that there is indeed a relationship between macroeconomic conditions and population health. Thus, if we assume that individuals with higher incomes are the owners of businesses, a good economic situation with high levels of development in a stable welfare state will have a positive impact on their entrepreneurial activity, and this will be reflected positively in their self-perceived health status. For this reason, we believe that in particular for the 0.75-quantile regression, the coefficient associated with HDI is higher than that of being employed, because the income of this group is more related to the macroeconomic environment, either because they are the owners or top managers of the companies that provide them with income, hence the link between macrovariables and self-perceived health is greater, and in particular with the country’s level of development.

In addition to the above, which attempts to explain the results obtained from the quantile regressions shown in Table 6, Figure 1 shows the individual plots of each of the estimated coefficients of the regression variables along the different quantiles ordered with respect to wealth. In other words, instead of showing the coefficient for one point of income, the whole progression of the same coefficient with respect to income is shown. Since the results have been discussed and contrasted with previous studies in the paragraphs above, some observations that are provided by Figure 1 and cannot be gleaned from the interpretation of the coefficients alone will be highlighted below.

Figure 1. Graphs of the quantile regression coefficients.

From these graphs, the shape of the coefficients for the age variables is striking. They all have convex shapes whose inflection point shifts further to the right, i.e. they show higher levels of wealth in order to be able to take off in terms of self-perceived health. We think that this is due to the stress factor, because the older the person, the greater the amount of income needed to satisfy basic needs such as the household, support the family, especially if the family is made up of children, etc.

With the variable Male in general terms, men report greater self-perceived health than women, especially at higher income levels, which could be due to the fact that women with higher income levels probably feel that they sacrifice motherhood in order to be able to fulfil their working hours effectively. It is also noteworthy that at higher income levels, the levels of self-perceived health of men and women seem to converge.

With respect to the BMI variable, a sinusoidal shape is found which indicates that those with lower income levels are more penalized by suffering from obesity problems than their higher income counterparts, who do not seem to feel as disadvantaged by having a high body mass index.

The variables EuropeanBorn, URBANzone, PeopleHousehold and FluVaccine seem to show the same behaviour regardless of income, all of them having a large margin of error.

The Spouse and Employee variables have the same behaviour, people with low-income levels report very high self-perceived health and then the level decreases, stagnates and remains constant. Both confirm that people with lower income levels are more vulnerable and require social support and economic stability to feel secure compared with those with higher incomes, who are less dependent on their jobs or have more stable jobs and feel more secure.

The Education, CardioDays and UnitsFrutVeg variables also show similar behaviours, showing that those with low purchasing power do not show good levels of self-perceived health due to these factors, i.e., if you have a low-income level, having an education, practising sport and eating healthy will not give you high levels of self-perceived health, perhaps because you long for a better economic situation. The opposite is true for those on middle and high incomes, who do enjoy high levels of self-perceived health due to these factors. This is until they reach the highest incomes, who again stop valuing these variables to assess their self-perceived health.

Finally, the macroeconomic variables: CO 2 , HDI and Gini report levels close to 0 for low incomes and their effects are clearly perceived for high income levels, reaffirming what was previously stated in Table 6.

Robustness Analysis

The results of the robustness analysis are shown in Table 7. As evidenced by the tests performed (Count R 2, Pearson test, ROC curve) the results of the logistic regression are suitable, since the Pearson test is significant, and both the count R 2 and ROC curve show that the model fit is good.

Table 7. Logistic regression for each quartile.

With respect to the coefficients obtained for each of the variables, once again the same results were found as those shown above using quantile regression. In this way, the results obtained by changing the study technique are seconded, being able to offer greater plausibility in the results discussed above. In other words, our conclusions would not change in spite of changing the study technique, once the pertinent tests have been carried out and it has been shown that both techniques offer appropriate results.

Limitations

This study is based on surveys collected in The European Health Interview Survey (EHIS), which is undoubtedly a large sample to work with, and is based on interviews, which guarantees reliable information and reasonable results. However, precisely because the data are obtained through surveys and are not directly observable, it must be recognized that there is a certain margin of error in that the answer given by the survey may differ somewhat from reality. However, it is assumed that respondents have been rational and sincere, so this bias should be minimal considering that we have worked with 316,277 observations.

Another limitation of the data is that there are no questions in the EHIS that collect information on whether the individual suffers or has suffered from stress or anxiety. These two disorders are very frequent in today’s society and undoubtedly have consequences on health and, of course, on self-perceived health as attested by authors such as Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Wang, Yang and Fan2016), Mangrio et al. (Reference Mangrio, Zdravkovic and Sjögren Forss2020), Buneviciene et al. (Reference Buneviciene, Bunevicius, Bagdonas and Bunevicius2022). Therefore, not having these variables in our study is a limitation that we hope will be solved in the future by adding this type of question to the surveys.

On the other hand, limitations can be found in the approach taken in the analysis. Our analysis focuses on universal patterns at the European level, which is why we have taken the ‘inside–outside’ approach to the population, so we do not take into account the country of residence of the individual, only whether they are European or not (living in the same country of birth or in another European country). This limitation opens the way to future lines of research focused on studying patterns in more limited geographical areas or centred on a single country. In addition, the analysis technique, i.e. quantile regression, like other statistical methods, cannot establish causal relationships on its own. It is a very powerful technique for exploring associations between variables and identifying how they vary across different quantiles of the outcome variable but does not intrinsically establish causality.

Finally, the last limitation is that this study is based on surveys that have been carried out exclusively in European countries, and these results could be different if the surveys had been obtained from countries in Africa, Asia, or the Americas.

Conclusions and Policy Implications

Our results show that the main determinants of an individual’s self-perceived health are age, nationality, and employment status, with those having the highest self-perceived health being young people born in EU countries who are part of the country’s working population. In addition, although less relevant, it is evidenced that women, as a general rule, tend to feel less healthy. However, this general interpretation would require its own in-depth study because, as the cited studies indicate, gender differences in health are a truly complex issue, and simplifying these differences under the statement ‘women are less healthy’ would unquestionably be naive and simplistic. Where there is some consensus, however, is that women are more prone to morbidity than men, as indicated by Bambra et al. (Reference Bambra, Albani and Franklin2021) regarding COVID-19. In addition, as might be expected, those individuals with poor habits such as not engaging in activities that increase heart rate, smoking or having high body mass indexes will undoubtedly report lower levels of self-perceived health than those with healthy habits.

Furthermore, in response to one of the starting hypotheses of this study, whether the macroeconomic context affects self-perceived health, we can affirm that there is evidence that only people with a high level of wealth show sensitivity of their self-perceived health status to the socioeconomic situation of their country, as evidenced by quantile regression. However, the robustness analysis using logistic regression does provide evidence that anyone, regardless of their income, could have their self-perceived health status affected by macro magnitudes.

The policy implications of this study lie in two fundamental aspects: there is a need to incorporate questions on anxiety and stress in population surveys because of their great importance for population health and their increasing frequency, as evidenced by the growing attention paid to these issues and highlighted by studies such as those of Mahmud et al. (Reference Mahmud, Mohsin, Dewan and Muyeed2023) and Schulz et al. (Reference Schulz, Hahlweg, Job and Supke2023). The lack of questions that directly cover both disorders is, we believe, a limitation for the study of self-perceived health, and perhaps other issues. Furthermore, based on the results obtained, we can point out that it would be desirable to invest more in the promotion of healthy habits among the population and to make the labour market more flexible, which would reduce unemployment among the population. It is clear that the variables of being employed and performing activities that increase heart rate are of great importance in self-perceived health status.

Funding

This study was funded by the European Union, Junta de Andalucia and University of Almeria, with reference number UAL-FEDER 2020, UAL2020-SEJ-D1999.

Acknowledgements

The authors Almudena Guarnido-Rueda and Ignacio Amate-Fortes are grateful for all the suggestions received during the research stay at the Economics Department of UC Santa Barbara.

About the Authors

Ignacio Amate-Fortes (corresponding author): with a degree in Economics from the University Complutense of Madrid and a PhD in Economics from the University of Almeria, Ignacio Amate-Fortes is an associate professor in the Department of Economics and Business at the University of Almeria. Furthermore, he leads the research group of ‘Inequality and Socioeconomic Development’ at the University of Almeria. He is the author of numerous articles in the fields of Inequality, Public Economics, Health Economics, Gender and Socioeconomic Development.

Almudena Guarnido-Rueda: with a degree in Economics and Business Administration from CEU ‘Luis Vives’ of Madrid and a PhD in Economics from the University of Almeria, Almudena Guarnido-Rueda is an associate professor in the Department of Economics and Business at the University of Almeria. She is the author of numerous articles in the fields of Inequality, Public Policy Evaluation, Health Economics, Gender, Regulation and Socioeconomic Development.

Diego Martínez-Navarro: with a degree in Economics from the University of Ameria and a PhD in Economics from the University of Almeria, Diego Martínez-Navarro is an assistant professor in the Department of Economics and Business at the University of Almeria. He is the author of numerous articles in the fields of Inequality, Development and Health Economics.

Francisco J. Oliver-Márquez: with a degree in Economics from the University of Ameria and a PhD in Economics from the University of Almeria, Francisco J. Oliver-Márquez is an assistant professor in the Department of Economics and Business at the University of Almeria. He is the author of numerous articles in the fields of Financial Literacy, Health Economics and Public Economics.

References

Aheto, JMK (2020) Simultaneous quantile regression and determinants of under-five severe chronic malnutrition in Ghana. BMC Public Health 20(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alić, J, Basioli Kasap, E and Jenko Miholić, S (2021) Relationship between students physical activity level and self-perceived health status. Medica Jadertina 51(4), 347355.Google Scholar
Amate-Fortes, I, Guarnido-Rueda, A and Molina-Morales, A (2020) World analysis of the determinants of the inequality in health. Is the measurement of inequality important? Revija za socijalnu politiku 27(1), 8398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arenz, S, Rückerl, R, Koletzko, B and von Kries, R (2004) Breast-feeding and childhood obesity—a systematic review. International Journal of Obesity 28(10), 12471256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axon, DR, Jang, A, Son, L and Pham, T (2022) Determining the association of perceived health status among United States older adults with self-reported pain. Aging and Health Research 2(1), 100051.Google Scholar
Bambra, C, Albani, V and Franklin, P (2021) COVID-19 and the gender health paradox. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 49(1), 1726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyerlein, A, Toschke, AM and Von Kries, R (2008) Breastfeeding and childhood obesity: shift of the entire BMI distribution or only the upper parts? Obesity 16(12), 27302733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyerlein, A (2014) Quantile regression—opportunities and challenges from a user’s perspective. American Journal of Epidemiology 180(3), 330331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buneviciene, I, Bunevicius, R, Bagdonas, S and Bunevicius, A (2022) The impact of pre-existing conditions and perceived health status on mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Public Health 44(1), e88e95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Case, A and Paxson, C (2010) Causes and consequences of early-life health. Demography 47(Suppl 1), S65S85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Case, A, Paxson, C and Vogl, T (2007) Socioeconomic status and health in childhood: a comment on Chen, Martin and Matthews: ‘Socioeconomic status and health: do gradients differ within childhood and adolescence?’ (62: 9, 2006, 2161–2170). Social Science & Medicine 64(4), 757761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, CH, Wang, J, Yang, CS and Fan, JY (2016) Nurse practitioner job content and stress effects on anxiety and depressive symptoms, and self-perceived health status. Journal of Nursing Management 24(5), 695704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, PM, Gerdtham, UG, Johannesson, M, Bingefors, K and Smith, L (2002) On the measurement of relative and absolute income-related health inequality. Social Science & Medicine 55(11), 19231928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, D and Flood, C (2022) Health economics. In Health Studies (pp. 269294). Singapore: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornelisse-Vermaat, JR, Antonides, G, Van Ophem, JA and Van Den Brink, HM (2006) Body mass index, perceived health, and happiness: their determinants and structural relationships. Social Indicators Research 143158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlgren, G and Whitehead, M (1991) Policies and Strategies to Promote Social Equity. Health Institute of Future Studies, Stockholm.Google Scholar
Danylova, T (2020) The modern-day feminine beauty ideal, mental health, and Jungian archetypes. Mental Health: Global Challenges Journal 3(1), 3844.Google Scholar
Davis, RE and Geneus, CJ (2016) Impact of health-related quality of life and social determinants on yearly influenza immunization in the United States. Value in Health 19(3), A221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Do, DP (2009) The dynamics of income and neighborhood context for population health: do long-term measures of socioeconomic status explain more of the black/white health disparity than single-point-in-time measures? Social Science & Medicine 68(8), 13681375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fielding, JE (1985) Smoking: health effects and control. New England Journal of Medicine 313(9), 555561.Google ScholarPubMed
Fleury-Bahi, G, Préau, M, Annabi-Attia, T, Marcouyeux, A and Wittenberg, I (2015) Perceived health and quality of life: the effect of exposure to atmospheric pollution. Journal of Risk Research 18(2), 127138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galobardes, B, Shaw, M, Lawlor, DA, Lynch, JW and Smith, GD (2006) Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1). Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 60(1), 712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrison, SM and Rodgers, JL (2019) Decomposing the causes of the socioeconomic status-health gradient with biometrical modeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 116(6), 10301047. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000226 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Godoy-Bermúdez, A, Rojo-Gallego-Burin, A, Delgado-Márquez, L, Martín-Martín, JJ, Sánchez-Martínez, MT and del Amo-González, MPL (2022) Gender differences in perceived health in relation to working conditions and socio-economic status in Spain, 2014-2017. medRxiv. CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, MS, Wheeler, AJ, Burnett, RT, Mayo, NE, Valois, MF, Brophy, JM and Giannetti, N (2015) Physiological and perceived health effects from daily changes in air pollution and weather among persons with heart failure: a panel study. Journal of Exposure Science & Environmental Epidemiology 25(2), 187199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harber-Aschan, L, Calderón-Larrañaga, A, Darin-Mattson, A, Hu, X, Fratiglioni, L and Dekhtyar, S (2020) Beyond the social gradient: the role of lifelong socioeconomic status in older adults’ health trajectories. Aging (Albany NY) 12(24), 24693.Google ScholarPubMed
Harder, T, Bergmann, R, Kallischnigg, G and Plagemann, A (2005) Duration of breastfeeding and risk of overweight: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology 162(5), 397403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E and Laursen, B (2019) Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Handbook of Parenting (pp. 421447). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L, Li, L, Ji, J and Sanderson, M (2020) Identifying and understanding determinants of high healthcare costs for breast cancer: a quantile regression machine learning approach. BMC Health Services Research 20(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Q (2020) How does news media exposure amplify publics’ perceived health risks about air pollution in China? A conditional media effect approach. International Journal of Communication 14, 20.Google Scholar
Idler, EL and Benyamini, Y (1997) Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 38(1), 2137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jayawardena, R, Sooriyaarachchi, P, Chourdakis, M, Jeewandara, C and Ranasinghe, P (2020) Enhancing immunity in viral infections, with special emphasis on COVID-19: A review. Diabetes & Metabolic Syndrome: Clinical Research & Reviews 14(4), 367382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jylhä, M (2009) What is self-rated health and why does it predict mortality? Towards a unified conceptual model. Social Science & Medicine 69(3), 307316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaleta, D, Makowiec-Dabrowska, T, Dziankowska-Zaborszczyk, E and Jegier, A (2006) Physical activity and self-perceived health status. International Journal of Occupational Medicine and Environmental Health 19(1), 61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J (2011) Socioeconomic inequalities in self-rated health among middle-aged and older adults. Social Work in Health Care 50(2), 124142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirman, A and Teschl, M (2010) Selfish or selfless? The role of empathy in economics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 365(1538), 303317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenker, R and Hallock, KF (2001) Quantile regression. Journal of Economic Perspectives 15(4), 143156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, KL, Roberts, K and Brodie, DA (1990) Self-perceived health among sports participants and non-sports participants. Social Science & Medicine 31(9), 963969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, L, Hu, L, Ji, J, Mckendrick, K, Moreno, J, Kelley, AS, … and Aldridge, M (2022) Determinants of total end-of-life health care costs of medicare beneficiaries: a quantile regression forests analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A 77(5), 10651071.Google ScholarPubMed
Link, BG and Phelan, J (1995) Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 1995(special number), 8094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macintyre, S, Hunt, K and Sweeting, H (1996) Gender differences in health: are things really as simple as they seem? Social Science & Medicine 42(4), 617624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahmud, S, Mohsin, M, Dewan, MN and Muyeed, A (2023) The global prevalence of depression, anxiety, stress, and insomnia among general population during COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Trends in Psychology 31(1), 143170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mangrio, E, Zdravkovic, S and Sjögren Forss, K (2020) The association between self-perceived health and sleep-quality and anxiety among newly arrived refugees in Sweden: a quantitative study. Journal of Immigrant and Minority Health 22(1), 8286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, R, Capitão, C, Fialho, M, Feteira-Santos, R, Virgolino, A, Santos, RR … and Santos, O (2022) Are beliefs and attitudes about COVID-19 associated with self-perceived changes in food consumption? Results from a nationwide survey during lockdown. Appetite 168, 105681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nappo, N (2022) Self-perceived job insecurity and self-reported health: differences between native-born and migrant workers based on evidence from the Sixth European Working Conditions Survey. PLOS One 17(4), e0267252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, SS and Krasnik, A (2010) Poorer self-perceived health among migrants and ethnic minorities versus the majority population in Europe: a systematic review. International Journal of Public Health 55(5), 357371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, A and Layte, R (2014) Socio-economic Inequalities in Child Health in Ireland. The Economic and Social Review 45(1), 2564.Google Scholar
Olsson, M, Currow, DC and Ekström, MP (2022) Exploring the most important factors related to self-perceived health among older men in Sweden: a cross-sectional study using machine learning. BMJ Open 12(6), e061242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, CG, Martin, RM, Whincup, PH, Smith, GD and Cook, DG (2005) Effect of infant feeding on the risk of obesity across the life course: a quantitative review of published evidence. Pediatrics 115(5), 13671377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palloni, A, Milesi, C, White, RG and Turner, A (2009) Early childhood health, reproduction of economic inequalities and the persistence of health and mortality differentials. Social Science & Medicine 68(9), 15741582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelan, JC, Link, BG and Tehranifar, P (2010) Social conditions as fundamental causes of health inequalities: theory, evidence, and policy implications. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(1 suppl), S28S40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piko, B (2000) Health-related predictors of self-perceived health in a student population: the importance of physical activity. Journal of Community Health 25(2), 125137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prędkiewicz, P, Bem, A, Siedlecki, R, Kowalska, M and Robakowska, M (2022) An impact of economic slowdown on health. New evidence from 21 European countries. BMC Public Health 22(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rahman, A and Hossain, M (2020) Quantile regression approach to estimating prevalence and determinants of child malnutrition. Journal of Public Health 30, 117.Google Scholar
Riess, H (2017) The science of empathy. Journal of Patient Experience 4(2), 7477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samadoulougou, S, Idzerda, L, Letarte, L, McKay, R, Quesnel-Vallée, A and Lebel, A (2022) Self-perceived health status among adults with obesity in Quebec: a cluster analysis. Annals of Epidemiology 67, 4349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saravanakumar, P, Garrett, NKG, Van Wissen, K, Montayre, J and McBride-Henry, K (2022) Social connectedness and self-perceived health of older adults in New Zealand. Health & Social Care in the Community 30(3), e647e656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schäfer, AA, Santos, LP, Quadra, MR, Dumith, SC and Meller, FO (2022) Alcohol consumption and smoking during Covid-19 pandemic: association with sociodemographic, behavioral, and mental health characteristics. Journal of Community Health 47(4), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitz, A and Lazarevič, P (2020) The gender health gap in Europe’s ageing societies: universal findings across countries and age groups? European Journal of Ageing 17, 509520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnittker, J and Bacak, V (2014) The increasing predictive validity of self-rated health. PLOS One 9(1), e84933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, W, Hahlweg, K, Job, AK and Supke, M (2023) Prevalence, persistence, and course of symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress of mothers and fathers. Results of an 18-year longitudinal study. Journal of Affective Disorders 344, 301310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharaf, MF, Mansour, EI and Rashad, AS (2019) Child nutritional status in Egypt: a comprehensive analysis of socioeconomic determinants using a quantile regression approach. Journal of Biosocial Science 51(1), 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shavers, VL (2007) Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. Journal of the National Medical Association 99(9), 1013.Google ScholarPubMed
Shields, M and Shooshtari, S (2001) Determinants of self-perceived health. Health Reports 13(1), 3552.Google ScholarPubMed
Shields, M (2008) Community belonging and self-perceived health. Health Reports 19(2), 51.Google ScholarPubMed
Solt, F (2020) Measuring income inequality across countries and over time: the standardized world income inequality database. Social Science Quarterly 101(3), 11831199. SWIID Version 9.3, June 2022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stowasser, T, Heiss, F, McFadden, D, Winter, J and Willis, RJ (2011) 8.’Healthy, Wealthy, and Wise?’ Revisited: An Analysis of the Causal Pathways from Socioeconomic Status to Health (pp. 267320). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Stringhini, S, Carmeli, C, Jokela, M, Avendaño, M, Muennig, P, Guida, F… and Tumino, R (2017) Socioeconomic status and the 25×25 risk factors as determinants of premature mortality: a multicohort study and meta-analysis of 1·7 million men and women. The Lancet 389(10075), 12291237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Su, Y, D’Arcy, C, Li, M and Meng, X (2022) Determinants of life satisfaction and self-perceived health in nationally representative population-based samples, Canada, 2009 to 2018. Applied Research in Quality of Life 17, 32853310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tadiri, CP, Gisinger, T, Kautzky-Willer, A, Kublickiene, K, Herrero, MT, Norris, CM … and Pilote, L (2021) Determinants of perceived health and unmet healthcare needs in universal healthcare systems with high gender equality. BMC Public Health 21(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tattarini, G and Grotti, R (2022) Gender roles and selection mechanisms across contexts: a comparative analysis of the relationship between unemployment, self-perceived health and gender. Sociology of Health & Illness 44(3), 641662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Umberson, D, Crosnoe, R and Reczek, C (2010) Social relationships and health behavior across the life course. Annual Review of Sociology 36, 139157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vidhya, K, Gupta, S, Lekshmi, R, Bhardwaj, K, Kusum, K, Kalyani, VC and Gupta, A (2022) Assessment of patient’s knowledge, attitude, and beliefs about cancer: An institute-based study. Journal of Education and Health Promotion 11.Google ScholarPubMed
Wärnberg, J, Pérez-Farinós, N, Ajejas-Bazán, MJ, Pérez-López, J, Benavente-Marín, JC, Crespo-Oliva, E … and Barón-López, FJ (2021) Lack of social support and its role on self-perceived health in a representative sample of Spanish adults. Another aspect of gender inequality. Journal of Clinical Medicine 10(7), 1502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, K and Umberson, D (2004) Marital status, marital transitions, and health: a gendered life course perspective. Journal of Health and Social behavior 45(1), 8198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziebarth, N (2010) Measurement of health, health inequality, and reporting heterogeneity. Social Science & Medicine 71(1), 116124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the first quartile (Q1).

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the second quartile (Q2).

Figure 2

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the third quartile (Q3).

Figure 3

Table 4. Descriptive statistics for the maximum values.

Figure 4

Table 5. Variance Inflation Factors (VIFs).

Figure 5

Table 6. Estimation results for each quartile.

Figure 6

Figure 1. Graphs of the quantile regression coefficients.

Figure 7

Table 7. Logistic regression for each quartile.