Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T23:05:39.587Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multisensory integration for spatial orientation in trait anxiety subjects: absence of visual dependence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2020

I. Viaud-Delmon*
Affiliation:
CNRS UMR 7593, Pavillon Clérambault, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, 47, boulevard de l’Hôpital, 75013 Paris, France
A. Berthoz
Affiliation:
Laboratoire de Physiologie de la Perception et de l’Action, CNRS-Collège de France, 11, place Marcelin Berthelot, 75005 Paris, France
R. Jouvent
Affiliation:
CNRS UMR 7593, Pavillon Clérambault, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, 47, boulevard de l’Hôpital, 75013 Paris, France
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (I. Viaud-Delmon).
Get access

Summary

Studies suggest a greater reliance on visual information for maintaining balance in anxious subjects. Nevertheless, links between this supposed preferred visual processing and spatial orientation have not yet been evaluated. Two groups of subjects differing in their level of trait anxiety were formed. Equipped with a head-mounted visual display, they learned a virtual corridor using passive translation but active rotation, both with normal and with two different conflicting sensory conditions. After two visual navigation trials in the corridor, they were blindfolded and asked to reproduce the same trajectory from memory. In addition, subjects drew a map of the remembered corridor. Anxious subjects were comparable to non-anxious subjects when asked to reproduce the trajectory from memory, but exhibited a deficit when drawing a map of the corridor they were in. The results do not support the hypothesis that anxious subjects use preferentially one type of sensory cue over another for spatial orientation, but instead manifest difficulties in constructing more global representations of space.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Éditions scientifiques et médicales Elsevier SAS 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Asmundson, GJLarsen, DKStein, MBPanic disorder and vestibular disturbance: an overview of empirical findings and clinical implications. J Psychosom Res 1998;44:107–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthoz, A Viaud-Delmon, IMultisensory integration in spatial orientation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999;9:708–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, JStatistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, second ed. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.Google Scholar
Jacob, RGFurman, JMDurrant, JDTurner, SMSurface dependence: a balance control strategy in panic disorder with agoraphobia. Psychosom Med 1997;59:323–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, RGFurman, JMRDurrant, JDTurner, SMPanic, agoraphobia, and vestibular dysfunction. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:503–12.Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, RBHumphris, GLewis, TDoagoraphobics interpret the environment in large shops and supermarkets differently. Br J Clin Psychiatry 1996;35:635–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kallai, JKoczan, GSzabo, IMolnar, PVarga, JAn experimental study to operationally define and measure spatial orientation in panic agoraphobic subjects, generalized anxiety and healthy control groups. Behav Cogn Psychotherapy 1995;23:145–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lecrubier, YSheehan, DWeiller, EAmorim, PBonora, LIHarnett, Ket al.The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur Psychiatry 1997;12:224–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, BSEffects of adverse experiences for brain structure and function. Biol Psychiatry 2000;48:721–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, NCognitive dysfunction resulting from hippocampal hyperactivity—a possible cause of anxiety disorder. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1997;56:603–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, CDGorsuch, RLLushene, RVagg, PRJacobs, GA In: Form, Y editor. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983.Google Scholar
Viaud-Delmon, IIvanenko, IPBerthoz, AJouvent, RAdaptation as a sensorial profile in trait anxiety: a study with virtual reality. J Anxiety Disord 2000;14:583–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viaud-Delmon, ISiegler, IIsraël, IJouvent, RBerthoz, AEye deviation during rotation in darkness in trait anxiety: an early expression of perceptual avoidance. Biol Psychiatry 1999;47: 112–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, RFRepresenting a stable environment by egocentric updating and invariant representations. Spatial Cognition and Computation 1999;1:431–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yardley, LBritton, JLear, SBird, JLuxon, LRelationship between balance system function and agoraphobic avoidance. Behav Res Ther 1995;33:435–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Yardley, LLuxon, LBird, JLear, SBritton, JVestibular and posturographic test results in people with symptoms of panic and agoraphobia. J Audiol Med 1994;3:48–65.Google Scholar
Yardley, LRedfern, MSPsychological factors influencing recovery from balance disorders. J Anxiety Disord 2001;15:107–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.