Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T07:53:03.594Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mediation effect of anxious attachment on relationship between childhood trauma and suicidal ideation sensitive to psychological pain levels

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2023

Hannah Ihme
Affiliation:
Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, UMR 7289, Aix Marseille Université, CNRS, Marseille, France
Philippe Courtet
Affiliation:
IGF,Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, INSERM, Montpellier, France Department of Emergency Psychiatry and Acute Care, Lapeyronie Hospital, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France
Nathan Risch
Affiliation:
IGF,Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, INSERM, Montpellier, France
Jonathan Dubois
Affiliation:
IGF,Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, INSERM, Montpellier, France Department of Emergency Psychiatry and Acute Care, Lapeyronie Hospital, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France
Raoul Belzeaux*
Affiliation:
Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, UMR 7289, Aix Marseille Université, CNRS, Marseille, France Pôle Universitaire de Psychiatrie, CHU de Montpellier, Montpellier, France
Emilie Olié
Affiliation:
IGF,Univ. Montpellier, CNRS, INSERM, Montpellier, France Department of Emergency Psychiatry and Acute Care, Lapeyronie Hospital, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France
*
Corresponding author: Raoul Belzeaux; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Background

Childhood trauma (CT), depression, and psychological pain are known predictors of suicidal ideation. Recent literature additionally highlights the importance of the attachment system.

Methods

We aimed to predict suicidal ideation through CT, attachment, and psychological and social pain by using mediation models aiming to predict suicidal ideation through CT (predictor) and attachment (mediator). In the same models, we introduced psychological or social pain as a moderator of the relationship between attachment, CT, and suicidal ideation. We included 161 depressed patients and assessed depression, attachment, CT, suicidal ideation, psychological pain, and social pain.

Results

We found (1) a complete mediating effect of anxious attachment (a2b2 = 0.0035, CI95% = [0.0010; 0.0069]) on the relationship between CT on suicidal ideation, and (2) a significant complete conditional mediating effect of anxious attachment and psychological pain (index of moderated mediation VAS: 0.0014; CI95% = [0.0002; 0.0032]) but not social pain on the relationship between CT and suicidal ideation. Both models were controlled for history of suicidal attempt, depression severity, and sex.

Conclusions

Our results suggest a developmental profile of suicidal ideation in mood disorder that is characterized by the presence of CT and insecure attachment, especially anxious attachment, that is sensitive to experiences of psychological pain. Nevertheless, we cannot conclude that avoidantly attached individuals do not present the same mechanism, as they may not disclose those ideas.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Suicidal ideation is a preceding risk factor for suicide attempts [Reference Turecki1, Reference Rabasco and Andover2] and death [Reference Franklin, Ribeiro, Fox, Bentley, Kleiman and Huang3]. The presence of suicidal thoughts is common in mood disorders, with a prevalence ranging from 47 to 69% [Reference Sokero, Melartin, Rytsala, Leskela, Lestela-Mielonen and Isometsa4, Reference Brown, Beck, Steer and Grisham5], and has a critical impact on prognosis. Consequently, it seems important to understand which factors, such as environmental stressors or trait-dependent variables, are involved in the genesis of suicidal ideation [Reference Van Heeringen and Mann6, Reference O’Connor and Kirtley7]. Recent literature has focused on the importance of childhood trauma (CT) [Reference Turecki1, Reference Bahk, Jang, Choi and Lee8, Reference Angelakis, Gillespie and Panagioti9] and the neurobehavioral attachment system [Reference Stagaki, Nolte, Feigenbaum, King-Casas, Lohrenz and Fonagy10Reference Cantón-Cortés, Cortés and Cantón14] in the suicidal process.

The attachment system is one of the neurobehavioral systems that affect the functioning of the whole organism throughout life, especially in interpersonal stressful situations. Its expression in adulthood is shaped by early interactions with the primary caregiver in childhood [Reference Bowlby15, Reference Ainsworth16] and therefore marked by the occurrence of CT. CT is defined as any act or series of acts done or omitted by any person 5 years older than the child that result in harm, potential harm, or imminent danger to the child [Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge and Ahluvalia17, Reference Leeb, Paulozzi, Melanson, Simon and Arias18]. Cognitive affective schemata acquired during these early periods of life serve as the basis for interpreting interpersonal relationships later on and thus guide perception, thoughts, and actions [Reference Mikulincer and Shaver19Reference Bosmans, Van Vlierberghe, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Kobak, Hermans and van IJzendoorn21]. A secure attachment is thought to be the result of a responsive and caring environment [Reference Cyr, Euser, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Van Ijzendoorn22], and is expressed through trust in oneself and others, as well as an inner sense of security. The experience of repetitive frightening and frustrating experiences as typical for CT is linked to an insecure attachment characterized by fear and/or avoidance, mistrust, hopelessness, and pessimism [Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy23, Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Allison24].

Avoidant attachment expresses in excessive self-reliance and low interpersonal intimacy. Avoidantly attached individuals tend to deactivate their attachment system in times of stress through suppressing and inhibiting support-seeking tendencies [Reference Bowlby15]. Anxiously attached individuals in line with a strong desire for closeness tend towards hyperactivation strategies, for example, control, force, or intrusion in order to evoke attention or love from attachment figures [Reference Main25, Reference Mikulincer and Shaver26]. Both attachment strategies might prove insufficient in the face of extremely painful experiences such as loss or exclusion [Reference Adam, Sperling and Berman43] and are effectively linked to higher vulnerability to suicidal ideation [Reference Zortea, Gray and O’Connor27, Reference Green, Berry, Danquah and Pratt28].

In general, painful experiences (entrapment, isolation) and the associated affective state, psychological pain, are thought to be a central trigger of suicidal ideation [Reference O’Connor and Kirtley7, Reference O’Connor, Fraser and Whyto29Reference Shneidman32]. Psychological pain refers to an interplay of feelings of shame, guilt, humiliation, dread, fear of “losing oneself,” inner emptiness, confusion, emotional flooding, and social phenomena such as withdrawal or freezing [Reference Shneidman31Reference Orbach, Mikulincer, Sirota and Gilboa-Schechtman35]. Levels of psychological pain are higher in subjects who present suicidal ideation than in those who do not [Reference Ducasse, Holden, Boyer, Artéro, Guillaume and Courtet36]; and predict suicidal behavior in depressed patients even when controlled for depression severity [Reference Olié, Guillaume, Jaussent, Courtet and Jollant37, Reference Alacreu-Crespo, Cazals, Courtet and Olié38]. Furthermore, suicidal ideation can also be triggered by social pain, the affect experienced at separation and exclusion [Reference Eisenberger and Lieberman39, Reference MacDonald and Leary40]. Depending on the definition, expected exclusion [Reference MacDonald and Leary41] or merely actual exclusion [Reference Papini, Fuchs and Torres42] may be included. While psychological pain can be understood as a “broader construct” incorporating feelings of numbing, humiliation, and coping behavior [Reference Orbach, Mikulincer, Sirota and Gilboa-Schechtman35] and must not be tied to the social domain, social pain is distinguishable from psychological pain because it occurs in the social context [Reference Papini, Fuchs and Torres42]. As dread of separation is a key component of the attachment system, it has been hypothesized that especially social pain leads to an attachment crisis, in which all coping strategies (distancing as well as approaching) fail, and which then turns to a suicidal crisis [Reference Adam, Sperling and Berman43]. In contrast to psychological pain, social pain can be ethically induced in an experimental setting [Reference Williams and Jarvis44].

Recent literature has already linked CT, insecure attachment, suicidal ideation, and psychological pain in nonclinical settings. In a cohort of 371 Iranian colleague students, CT influenced suicidal ideation directly but also mediated through psychological pain [Reference Zarrati, Bermas and Sabet45]. In 2,259 Chinese students, a mediating effect of psychological pain on the relationship between emotional abuse and suicidal ideation was found [Reference Li, You, Ren, Zhou, Sun and Liu46]. Likewise in a clinical setting, Martins et al. [Reference Martins, Campos and Morujão47] report a direct effect of CT on suicidal ideation in 102 subjects with substance abuse disorder. This effect, however, vanished when the capacity to manage psychological pain was introduced as mediator. Introducing attachment as mediator, Musetti et al. [Reference Musetti, Pingani, Zagaria, Uberti, Meli and Lenzo12] showed a mediating effect of anxious attachment on the effect of traumatic life events on suicidal ideation in a cohort of 950 Italian adults. However, in a clinical population another study failed to show a mediating effect of attachment but found a direct effect of emotional abuse on current suicidal ideation in 96 mood disorder patients [Reference Ihme, Olié, Courtet, El-Hage, Zendjidjian and Mazzola-Pomietto48].

Objectives

To the best of our knowledge, no study so far has investigated all four concepts, CT, attachment, psychological or social pain, and suicidal ideation. We hypothesize that developmental profiles presenting CT and insecure attachment lead to higher expression of suicidal ideation in mood disorder patients and that this vulnerability is intensified as a function of the experienced intensity of psychological or social pain. We suppose that both types of pain activate deep-rooted attachment-related cognitive schemata and trigger aforementioned attachment strategies [Reference Long, Verbeke, Ein-Dor and Vrtička49]. Therefore we introduce social and psychological pain as a moderator, considering it as a trigger for the attachment system [Reference Long, Verbeke, Ein-Dor and Vrtička49] and especially its potential to lead to an attachment crisis and subsequently a suicidal crisis [Reference Shneidman31, Reference Adam, Sperling and Berman43]. We formulate the following hypotheses:

H1: The relationship between CT and suicidal ideation is mediated by attachment:

  1. 1.1. Insecure attachment (anxious and avoidant) reinforces the effect of CT on suicidal ideation.

  2. 1.2. Secure attachment has a buffering effect on suicidal ideation.

    H2: Psychological pain has a conditional mediating effect on the mediation of CT, attachment, and suicidal ideation, with a mediating effect of attachment sensitive to the level of experienced psychological pain.

H3: Social pain has conditional mediating effect on the mediation of CT, attachment, and suicidal ideation, with a mediating effect of attachment sensitive to the level of experienced social pain.

Methods

Participants and setting

We analyzed retrospective data from 161 depressed inpatients recruited in the Academic Hospital of Montpellier, France. Patients were classified into three groups based on their history of suicide attempts: 43 of them had attempted in the last 8 days (recently), 52 had attempted suicide at least once in their life (previously), and 66 never attempted suicide (never). General inclusion criteria were current MDE and age of majority. Patients engaging in substance abuse within the last 6 months, current (hypo)manic or mixed episode, lifetime schizo-affective disorder and/or schizophrenia as well as chronic neurologic pathology were excluded. Due to the primary objective of physical pain, patients on tricyclics or NSSRI were excluded due to possible analgesic effects. Six patients were excluded due to missing data. The inclusion period of the study was from June 2015 to May 2021. A trained and experienced clinician assessed psychopathology using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI 5.00) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (SCID II) for borderline personality disorder.

Questionnaire data

Childhood trauma

CT was assessed in retrospective by the French version of the short Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ-SF; 28 items) [Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge and Ahluvalia17, Reference Paquette, Laporte, Bigras and Zoccolillo50]. For the CTQ, patients rate the frequency of abusive and neglectful behavior on a 5-point Likert-Scale ranging from 1 (“never true”) to 5 (“very often true”). Items can either be added up to a total trauma score or be grouped into five subtypes of maltreatment: emotional, physical, and sexual abuse and physical and emotional neglect. Physical abuse refers to the intentional harming of the child through physical violence; sexual abuse refers to any sexualised contact between an adult and the child regardless of whether it is done with the child’s consent; and finally, psychological, or emotional abuse refers to any act or speech of demeaning, humiliating or intimidating character from caregiver to child. Physical neglect, on the other hand, refers to domestic situations in which the child’s basic physical needs for food, shelter, clothing, safety, and health care are not met, while emotional neglect refers to caregivers’ failure to meet the child’s basic emotional and psychological needs for love, encouragement, and support. The total trauma score ranges from 25 to 125 and scores for each maltreatment type range from 5 to 25. The scale is widely used; internal consistency ranges from 0.70 to 0.90 and retest reliability from 0.66 to 0.94.

Attachment

Attachment was measured by the French version of the Relationship Scales Questionnaire (RSQ) [Reference Griffin and Bartholomew51, Reference Guédeney, Fermanian and Bifulco52]. The RSQ contains 30 items, that load on three factors: avoidance (7 items, e.g. “I find it hard to depend on other people.”), anxiety (5 items, e.g. “I worry that I will be hurt if I allow myself to become too close to others.”), and security (5 items, e.g. “I find it easy to get emotionally close to others.”). Those factors build subdimensions, and for each subdimension, sum scores can be calculated by adding up respective items see results of the factorial analyses in Guédeney et al., 2010). Patients respond on a 5-point Likert scale: (1) Not at all like me, (3) Somewhat like me, and (5) Very much like me. The factor structure of the RSQ demonstrates good psychometric qualities: moderate internal consistency (Cronbach α 0.66 for avoidance factor, 0.69 for anxiety factor, and 0 .60 for security factor) and good interrater reliability (intraclass correlation avoidance factor-ICC = 0.80; anxiety factor-ICC = 0.85; security factor-ICC = 0.78). Patients living alone without partner were asked to respond either for their last relationship experience or for their typical behavior in close relationships.

Suicidal ideation

We measured suicidal ideation by the suicidal item of the French version of the Beck Depression Inventory – second edition [Reference Beck, Steer and Brown53] that reads as follows: (0) I do not have any thoughts of killing myself. (1) I have thoughts of killing myself, but I would not carry them out. (2) I would like to kill myself. (3) I would like to kill myself if I had the chance. Patients were asked to choose the response that best described their suicidal ideation status. A previous study showed that a single suicide item from a depression rating scale is a valid approach to assess SI compared with the Scale for Suicide Ideation [Reference Desseilles, Perroud, Guillaume, Jaussent, Genty and Malafosse54]. This method was previously used in large clinical studies, such as the STAR*D [Reference Zisook, Trivedi, Warden, Lebowitz, Thase and Stewart55] or more recent studies [Reference Desseilles, Perroud, Guillaume, Jaussent, Genty and Malafosse54, Reference Nobile, Olié, Dubois, Guillaume, Gorwood and Courtet56, Reference Joiner, Rudd and Rajab57].

Depression severity

Depression severity was measured by the clinician-rated Inventory for Depressive Symptomatology (IDS-C) [Reference Rush, Giles, Schlesser, Fulton, Weissenburger and Burns58]. The IDS-C asks the clinician to evaluate the patient’s typical depressive symptoms on 30 items regarding the symptom severity during the last 7 days (e.g. item 6 – Mood (Irritability): (0) Does not feel irritable. (1) Feels irritable less than half the time. (2) Feels irritable more than half the time. (3) Feels extremely irritable virtually all the time.). The sum score provides information about symptom severity. The English version shows good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.88) and high external validity (rHRSD = 0.92, rBDI = 0.61).

Psychological pain

Psychological pain was evaluated by a visual analog scale, PPP-VAS [Reference Olié, Guillaume, Jaussent, Courtet and Jollant37]. Resembling the scales commonly used in the assessment of physical pain [Reference Price, McGrath, Rafii and Buckingham59], PPP-VAS is a well-established tool to measure psychological pain in suicidal cohorts [Reference Alacreu-Crespo, Cazals, Courtet and Olié38]. By means of the PPP-VAS current, mean, and worst psychological pain can be evaluated. Worst psychological pain predicted significantly suicidal events in a prior study [Reference Alacreu-Crespo, Cazals, Courtet and Olié38], therefore we only included worst psychological pain in our statistical analysis. Participants rated the worst intensity of psychological pain during the last 2 weeks on a scale from 0 (none) to 10 (maximum possible pain).

Social pain

Social pain was assessed by the Need-Threat Scale (NTS) after the subjects played the Cyberball game. The Cyberball game is a validated paradigm of social exclusion during which participants are instructed that they would play with two other players an online ball game. But instead, they play with a preset computer program and are given a cover story to ensure that they believe the other players are human. The Cyberball game comprised 50 throws with two successive conditions. In the first condition, participants played with the other two players and received the ball as many times as the virtual players (1/3 of the throws). In the second condition, participants were excluded by the two other players during the 20 last throws. Successively, participants filled out the NTS. The NTS assesses 20 subjectively experienced consequences of being excluded during the game, including ratings of self-esteem (“I felt liked”), belongingness (“I felt rejected”), meaningfulness (“I felt invisible”), and control (“I felt powerful”), on a scale ranging from 1 = “not at all” to 5 = “very much”. To create the score of social pain, the total score was reverse-coded (100 – total score). The score ranges between 0 and 80. The higher the score was, the more intense was the perception of social exclusion and social pain.

Statistical analysis

First, we calculated the Spearman’s Rho correlation coefficients to display shared variance between the metric variables that we wanted to include in the mediation analyses, namely CT, attachment, suicidal ideation, depression, and psychological and social pain.

Then, we calculated multiple mediation analyses. In the first models, we tested the H1, the mediation effect of attachment on the relationship between CT and suicidal ideation by employing the SPSS Macro PROCESS Version 3.5 by Hayes [Reference Hayes60], model 4. PROCESS operates on the principle of ordinary least square regressions; confidence intervals and interference statistic are calculated through bootstrapping with 5000 samples.

Second, the hypothesized moderated mediation models (H2 and H3) were tested in separate models using a bootstrapping approach to assess the significance of the indirect effects at differing levels of the moderator [Reference Hayes60]. CT served as the predictor variable, with the three factors of attachment as parallel mediators. As in the simple mediation analysis, the outcome variable was suicidal ideation, additionally here, the level of psychological pain and social pain was the proposed moderator. The term conditional indirect effect refers to the fact that path coefficients – effects of the mediator to the outcome variable (b) and the direct effect of the predictor CT on the outcome suicidal ideation (c’) depend on a moderator (shown in Figures 2 and 3). Statistically those coefficients are expressed as a function of the moderator variable and are therefore reported by inserting model values for the moderator. PROCESS calculates the b coefficients automatically by inserting 3 values for m: one value marking the 16th (low), 50th (medium), and 64th (high) percentile respectively. Concerning the interference statistic, an index of moderated mediation was used to test the significance of the moderated mediation, that is, the difference of the indirect effects across levels of psychological and social pain [Reference Hayes60]. The models were calculated using the SPSS Macro PROCESS Version 4.1 by Hayes [Reference Hayes60], model 15, with bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals using bootstrapping with 5,000 samples. Significant effects are supported by the absence of zero within the confidence intervals.

For all models, we corrected the standard errors for heteroscedasticity [Reference Davidson and MacKinnon61], and report unstandardized regression coefficients. Effects were controlled for the severity of depression, the history of suicide attempt (recently, previously, never), and sex, which were included as covariates. In two separate models, we additionally added borderline personality disorder as binary variable as covariate. We calculated models for the CTQ total score and all CT subtypes, but only display in detail the results for CTQ total score in the paper.

Results

Description of the cohort

Our cohort was predominantly female (72.5%), single (65.2%), depressed (66.5%), and had a high school diploma (73.3%). 66.5% of patients were diagnosed with a unipolar, 33.5% with a bipolar depression. 23.% of the patients were diagnosed with borderline personality disorder. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the cohort are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Description of socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the whole sample (n = 161)

Abbreviations: BDI, Beck depression inventory; CTQ, childhood trauma questionnaire; IDS-C, clinician-rated Inventory for depressive symptomatology; PPP-VAS, physical psychological pain visual analogical scale; RSQ, relationship scale questionnaire.

Correlation coefficients can be found in Figure 1. Values of the trauma subtypes were highly correlated with each other, security factor presented a mediocre negative correlation with the avoidance factor social pain, and a positive correlation with the anxiety factor of attachment. Further, attachment anxiety was mediocrely correlated to the avoidance factor, the total trauma score, physical neglect, emotional abuse and neglect, and suicidal ideation. Suicidal ideation was mediocrely correlated to psychological pain, depression, total trauma score, and emotional abuse. Last, social pain was mediocrely positively correlated to emotional abuse and the total trauma score, and negatively to the security factor of attachment.

Figure 1. Bivariate Spearman’s Rho correlations, with only significant coefficients being displayed. Strength and direction of the relationship (positive or negative) of the correlation is labeled and additionally indicated by color (negative correlation depicted in red, positive correlation in blue) and size of the square. Correlation coefficients were interpreted according to Cohen’s conventions [Reference Cohen62]. A Rho coefficient |ρ| ≥ 0.10 indicates a weak relationship, |ρ| ≥ 0.30 a mediocre relationship, and |ρ| ≥ 0.50 a strong relationship.

Mediation analyses

H1: Mediation effect of anxious attachment on relationship between CT and suicidal ideation

The first mediation analysis conducted using ordinary least squares path analysis showed that CT (total trauma score) had a complete mediated effect through anxious attachment factor on suicidal ideation a2b2 = 0.0035, CI95% = [0.0010; 0.0069]. We found neither a direct nor an indirect effect for the avoidant or secure attachment factor (shown in Figure 2). In a second analysis, we calculated the same model for all subtypes of CT and found the same complete indirect effect of anxious attachment factor for emotional abuse and neglect, and physical abuse (shown in Supplementary material).

Figure 2. Mediation model on the effect of childhood trauma on suicidal ideation through its effect through three parallel attachment mediators. Regression coefficients are labeled with significant levels *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Significant paths are in bold.

H2: Moderated mediation effect of anxious attachment on relationship between CT and suicidal ideation, with psychological pain as moderator

Next, we tested the hypothesized moderated mediation model, in which psychological pain moderates the effect of path b (shown in Figure 3). Higher CT was associated with higher attachment anxiety, a2 = 0.0576, SE = 0.0186, p = 0.003. Psychological pain was found to moderate the effect of anxious attachment and suicidal ideation (Unstandardised interaction = 0.0238, SE = .0105, t = 2.2646, p = 0.0250). We found a moderate mediation effect of psychological pain on the relationship between CT, attachment anxiety, and suicidal ideation (index of moderated mediation = 0.0014, CI95% = [0.0002;0.0032]). This indicates that individuals with a developmental profile of CT and anxious attachment reported higher suicidal ideation the higher they reported psychological pain levels. The conditional indirect effect was strongest in those reporting the highest psychological pain (64th percentile; effect = 0.0046, SE = 0.0020, 95% CI = [0.0013; 0.0090]) and did not reach significance in those reporting the lowest psychological pain (16th percentile, effect = 0.0019, SE = 0.0013, 95% CI = [−0.0003; 0.0048]). Psychological pain moderated the effect of attachment avoidance on suicidal ideation (Unstandardised interaction = −0.0232, SE = .0111, t = −2.0864, p = .0387). However, there was no indication of moderated mediation of psychological pain on the effect of CT over avoidance and security factor on suicidal ideation. Similar results were obtained for separated models of moderated mediation with emotional abuse and neglect, and physical abuse as predictors (shown in Figure 3, and Supplementary material).

Figure 3. Mediation models on the conditional effect of childhood trauma on suicidal ideation through its effect through three parallel mediators, factors avoidance, anxiety, and security in function of the moderator psychological pain. Regression coefficients are labeled with significant levels *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Regression coefficients for b are only displayed for the effect of RSQ-anxiety on suicidal ideation, as it was the only path where significant index of moderate mediation was found. The calculated b and c’ coefficients are marked at the side of the diagram. Significant paths are in bold.

Results remained stable even if borderline personality disorder was included as additional covariate (see Supplementary material).

H2: No moderated mediation effect of attachment on relationship between CT and suicidal ideation, with social pain as moderator

Last, we tested the hypothesized moderated mediation model, in which we introduced social pain as moderator. No effects were found when the total trauma score served as predictor, and the index of moderated mediation showed a tendency of significance = 0.0002, CI95% = [0;0.0004]. Consecutively, we did not test for any trauma subtype. Results remained stable even if borderline personality disorder was included as additional covariate (see Supplementary material).

Discussion

Our results support hypotheses H1 and H2 suggesting a heightened vulnerability to suicidal ideation in anxious attachment which is worsened by psychological pain, but insensitive to social pain. Our results on the mediating effect of anxious attachment on the relationship between CT and suicidal ideation replicate results previously published by several research teams [Reference Bahk, Jang, Choi and Lee8, Reference Musetti, Pingani, Zagaria, Uberti, Meli and Lenzo12, Reference Lessard and Moretti63, Reference Riggs and Jacobvitz64] but contradict findings from a previous study from our lab [Reference Ihme, Olié, Courtet, El-Hage, Zendjidjian and Mazzola-Pomietto48]. However, to the best of our knowledge this is the first study reporting a moderated mediation effect of attachment and psychological pain on the relationship between CT and suicidal ideation in a clinical setting.

A heightened vulnerability to suicidal ideation in anxious attachment could reflect an imbalanced push-pull dynamic between associative and reflective cognitive processes [Reference Beevers65, Reference Haeffel, Abramson, Brazy, Shah, Teachman and Nosek66]. In situations of heightened stress, for example, induced through psychological pain or triggered through circumstances that resemble the original context, fast-associative processes guide perception [Reference Collins and Feeney67, Reference Mogg, Bradbury and Bradley68], attention, and cognition [Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy23, Reference Luyten and Fonagy69, Reference Wells, Vanderlind, Selby and Beevers70]. In the case of anxious attachment, those associative processes are based on care-related schemata that stem from experiences of inconsistent, impulsive, frightening, or insensitive care [Reference Abrutyn41, Reference Long, Verbeke, Ein-Dor and Vrtička49] and entail a self-perception as helpless, incompetent and dependent [Reference Long, Verbeke, Ein-Dor and Vrtička49, Reference Mikulincer and Shaver71]. Similar maladaptive personal schemata (e.g. “I am deeply flawed” or “I am a failure”) are suspected as core feature of depressive symptomatology [Reference Beck72], and as the sequalae of traumatic childhood experiences (“cognitive scars” [Reference Wells, Vanderlind, Selby and Beevers70]), especially emotional trauma [Reference Iffland and Neuner73]. As our cohort presents depressive symptoms and high levels of emotional trauma those maladaptive schemata are likely to exist in our cohort and might be activated in subjects that indicated a high level of psychological pain.

Further, reflective processes, especially reflective functioning – the capacity to understand the behavior of oneself or another as expression of underlying thoughts, beliefs, affective and motivational states [Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy23] – are less accessible under extreme stress [Reference Bateman and Fonagy74]. In anxious attachment, this manifests in maladaptive coping strategies that aim to evoke care from others but also amplify the individual’s own distress [Reference Mikulincer and Shaver71] in a number of ways, including an overestimation of threat, consistent pessimistic believes about their own stress management capacities, low self-worth and overgeneralization of past interpersonal injuries [Reference Main25, Reference Long, Verbeke, Ein-Dor and Vrtička49, Reference Mikulincer and Shaver71]. Heightened suicidal ideation in anxious attachment could thus result from a) the activation of dysfunctional schemata (“cognitive scars”) especially under psychological pain, and b) maladaptive stress-increasing coping strategies. Together and over time, this could aggravate already persisting negative thought spirals that finally accumulate in suicidal ideation.

Thus, our analyses on anxious attachment might have also reached significance due to a higher disclosure of suicidal thoughts in general. A higher disclosure is in coherence with the general orientation towards eliciting support from others that is present in anxious attachment and was found to be associated with clinical anxiety [Reference Høyen, Solem, Cohen, Prestmo, Hjemdal and Vaaler75, Reference Husky, Zablith, Alvarez Fernandez and Kovess-Masfety76]. In contrast, subjects that scored higher on avoidant attachment might not have disclosed ideation even though it was present. Non-disclosure of suicidal ideation is common in depressive cohorts [Reference Høyen, Solem, Cohen, Prestmo, Hjemdal and Vaaler75, Reference Cukrowicz, Duberstein, Vannoy, Lin and Unützer77] and might be linked in our cohort to increased cognitive disengagement which is a predominant strategy in avoidant attachment. Cognitive disengagement might serve as a protective factor against suicidal ideation in the short term but could increase the risk for suicidal acts in the long term due to isolation and lack of close relationships [Reference Adam, Sperling and Berman43, Reference Ihme, Olié, Courtet, El-Hage, Zendjidjian and Mazzola-Pomietto48, Reference Canetto and Sakinofsky79]. Our interpretation of higher disclosure and the activation of maladaptive schemata also seems in line with the fact that the present cohort presents higher values on suicidal ideation and emotional trauma and therefore might be more vulnerable to the here proposed mechanism compared to the cohort of a former study [Reference Ihme, Olié, Courtet, El-Hage, Zendjidjian and Mazzola-Pomietto48]. Further, our cohort presents average levels of secure attachment tendencies. Those did not dampen the effect of CT on suicidal ideation – contrary to our hypothesis - but it might also contribute to the disclosure of suicidal ideation especially in contrast to avoidant attachment.

Last, we did not find a moderating effect of social pain on the aforementioned mediation effect of attachment on CT and suicidal ideation. It is conceivable that our experimental manipulation (Cyberball paradigm) simply missed its mark, for example, because the patients did not engage with it or that hospitalization buffered the effect of the experimental manipulation. Also, social pain may not be “intense” or of large personal valence necessary to activate dysfunctional schemas and suicidal ideation.

Strengths and limitations of the present study

With 72.7% our cohort presents a high amount of females who are supposedly under higher risk to display suicidal ideation [Reference Canetto and Sakinofsky79]. Even though we tested for heightened sensitivity by including sex as covariate we cannot fully exclude the possibility that the effect found here is not gender biased. Furthermore, the proposed explanation of maladaptive cognitive schemata is well supported by the literature, nevertheless, we did not assess schemata directly. In this vein, recent papers have especially discussed the role of reflective functioning as a trait vulnerability in insecurely attached individuals [Reference Bateman and Fonagy74] however no consensus has been found so far [Reference Musetti, Pingani, Zagaria, Uberti, Meli and Lenzo12, Reference Green, Berry, Danquah and Pratt13]. Future studies might further investigate the role of associative schema and reflective processes in combination with other personality traits in predicting suicidal ideation [Reference Green, Berry, Danquah and Pratt13].

Further, we need to highlight that we analyze data in cross-sectional way and conclude on a developmental process in retrospect. The gold standard for studying developmental processes is typically long-term, prospective research. Prospective studies provide different figures on CT than retrospective ones [Reference Baldwin, Reuben, Newbury and Danese80]. However, it is generally assumed that participants disclose traumatic experiences more readily in retrospect [Reference Baldwin, Reuben, Newbury and Danese80]. Furthermore, a negative bias in the recall of autobiographical memory in depression could bias the admission of CT in our depressed cohort. However, a recent meta-analysis reported that the overall effect size of negative recall of explicit memory in depression is small and mostly bound to the emotional valence of experiences [Reference Everaert, Vrijsen, Martin-Willett, van de Kraats and Joormann81]. The CTQ was especially created to question about the frequency of abuse and neglect rather than emotional valence to balance out a biased view. Additionally, the CTQ shows good psychometric qualities, is widely used in depressive cohorts, and we correct our statistical model for depression severity. We therefore believe that CT was realistically captured and that the association we report reflects a real association in the cohort.

The described study is part of a bigger study during which physical pain through thermal stimuli was also assessed. The latter was assessed in a counterbalanced way before or after the Cyberball game, which could have impacted social pain ratings. We did not control for any such effect. Furthermore, we did not control for any medications. It might be possible that medical treatment influenced the perception of psychological pain. Last, we did not investigate intent of suicidal ideation. Suicidal ideation is more common and luckily only a fraction of those with ideation pass from idea to act [Reference Kessler, Berglund, Nock, Wang and Page82].

Conclusion

This study suggests a developmental profile of suicidal ideation in mood disorder that is characterized by the presence of CT and insecure attachment, especially anxious attachment, that is sensitive to experiences of psychological pain. Nevertheless, we cannot conclude that avoidantly attached individuals do not present the same mechanism, as they may not disclose those ideas. Future research should therefore focus on a detailed assessment of attachment, dysfunctional cognitive schemata, and reflective functioning.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2023.2452.

Data availability statement

Information is available on request.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the patients who participated in the study and our dear colleagues, especially Kathlyne Dupuis-Maurin who collaborated in the data collection. We also thank Dr. Katherine Bryant for proofreading the manuscript.

Author contribution

E.O. obtained the funding. N.R. and E.O. collected the data. H.I., P.C., J.D., R.B., and E.O. designed the study. H.I. undertook the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft. All authors contributed to the writing and the correction of the manuscript.

Financial support

This work has received funding from AOI Montpellier Nimes (2013), as well as support from the French government under the Programme Investissements d’Avenir, Initiative d’Excellence d’Aix-Marseille Université via A*Midex (AMX-19-IET-004) and ANR (ANR-17-EURE-0029) funding. The funders had no further role in the study design, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; nor in the writing or the decision to submit the article.

Competing interest

The authors declare none.

Ethical standard

Ethical approval was obtained (Sud Méditerranée IV - n°ID-RCB: 2013-A01029–36). An expense allowance of 60 euros was paid, and patients submitted their written consent.

References

Turecki, G. The molecular bases of the suicidal brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15:802–16. doi:10.1038/nrn3839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabasco, A, Andover, MS. The interaction of dissociation, pain tolerance, and suicidal ideation in predicting suicide attempts. Psychiatry Res. 2020;284:112661. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, JC, Ribeiro, JD, Fox, KR, Bentley, KH, Kleiman, EM, Huang, X, et al. Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: a meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychol Bull. 2017;143:187232. doi:10.1037/bul0000084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokero, TP, Melartin, TK, Rytsala, HJ, Leskela, US, Lestela-Mielonen, PS, Isometsa, ET. Suicidal ideation and attempts among psychiatric patients with major depressive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2003;64:1094–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, GK, Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Grisham, JR. Risk factors for suicide in psychiatric outpatients: a 20-year prospective study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:371–7. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Heeringen, K, Mann, JJ. The neurobiology of suicide. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1:6372. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70220-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, RC, Kirtley, OJ. The integrated motivational-volitional model of suicidal behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2018;373. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0268.Google ScholarPubMed
Bahk, YC, Jang, SK, Choi, KH, Lee, SH. The relationship between childhood trauma and suicidal ideation: role of maltreatment and potential mediators. Psychiatry Investig. 2017;14:3743. doi:10.4306/pi.2017.14.1.37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angelakis, I, Gillespie, EL, Panagioti, M. Childhood maltreatment and adult suicidality: a comprehensive systematic review with meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2019;49:1057–78. doi:10.1017/S0033291718003823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stagaki, M, Nolte, T, Feigenbaum, J, King-Casas, B, Lohrenz, T, Fonagy, P, et al. The mediating role of attachment and mentalising in the relationship between childhood maltreatment, self-harm and suicidality. Child Abus Negl. 2022;128:105576. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danner Touati, C, Miljkovitch, R, Sirparanta, A, Deborde, AS. The role of attachment to the foster parent with regard to suicidal risk among adult survivors of childhood maltreatment. Child Abus Negl. 2022;128. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104886.Google Scholar
Musetti, A, Pingani, L, Zagaria, A, Uberti, D, Meli, S, Lenzo, V, et al. Insecure adult attachment and reflective functioning as mechanisms of the relationship between traumatic life events and suicidal ideation: a path analysis. Front Psychol. 2022;13. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.985148.Google ScholarPubMed
Green, J, Berry, K, Danquah, A, Pratt, D. Attachment security and suicide ideation and behaviour: the mediating role of reflective functioning. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18:119. doi:10.3390/ijerph18063090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantón-Cortés, D, Cortés, MR, Cantón, J. Child sexual abuse and suicidal ideation: the differential role of attachment and emotional security in the family system. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17. doi:10.3390/ijerph17093163.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss. Volume 1. New York: Basic Books; 1969.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. Attachments across the life span. Bull N Y Acad Med. 1985;61(9):792.Google ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, DP, Stein, JA, Newcomb, MD, Walker, E, Pogge, D, Ahluvalia, T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abus Negl. 2003;27:169–90. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00541-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeb, RT, Paulozzi, LJ, Melanson, C, Simon, TR, Arias, I. Child maltreatment surveillance: uniform definitions for public health and recommended data elements, version 1.0. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;1:409–38.Google Scholar
Mikulincer, M, Shaver, PR. The attachment behavioral system in adulthood: activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2003;35:53152. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(03)01002-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M, Kaplan, N, Cassidy, J. Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: a move to the level of representation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2016;50:66104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosmans, G, Van Vlierberghe, L, Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, Kobak, R, Hermans, D, van IJzendoorn, MH. A learning theory approach to attachment theory: exploring clinical applications. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2022;25:591612. doi:10.1007/s10567-021-00377-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cyr, C, Euser, EM, Bakermans-Kranenburg, MJ, Van Ijzendoorn, MH. Attachment security and disorganization in maltreating and high-risk families: a series of meta-analyses. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22:87108. doi:10.1017/S0954579409990289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P, Campbell, C, Allison, E, Fonagy, P. The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: state of the art and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2020;7:297325. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071919-015355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P, Allison, E. Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: a new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. J Personal Disord. 2015;29:575609. doi:10.1521/pedi.2015.29.5.575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Main, M. Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: recent studies, changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Hum Dev. 1990;33:4861. doi:10.1159/000276502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikulincer, M, Shaver, PR. “Can’t buy me love”: an attachment perspective on social support and money as psychological buffers. Psychol Inq. 2008;19:167–73. doi:10.1080/10478400802631295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zortea, TC, Gray, CM, O’Connor, RC. The relationship between adult attachment and suicidal thoughts and behaviors: a systematic review. Arch Suicide Res. 2021;25:3873. doi:10.1080/13811118.2019.1661893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, J, Berry, K, Danquah, A, Pratt, D. The role of psychological and social factors in the relationship between attachment and suicide: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother. 2020;27:463–88. doi:10.1002/cpp.2445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, RC, Fraser, L, Whyto, M-C. A comparison of specific positive future expectancies and global hopelessness as predictors of suicidal ideation in a prosp. J Affect Disord. 2008;110:207–14. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.01.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klonsky, DE, May, AM. The three-step theory (3ST): a new theory of suicide rooted in the “ideation-to-action” framework. Int J Cogn Ther. 2015;8:114–29. doi:10.1521/ijct.2015.8.2.114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shneidman, ES. Perspectives on suicidology. Further reflections on suicide and psychache. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 1998;28:245–50.Google ScholarPubMed
Shneidman, ES. Commentary: suicide as psychache. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1993;181:145–7. doi:10.1097/00005053-199303000-00001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orbach, I, Mikulincer, M, Gilboa-Schechtman, E, Sirota, P. Mental pain and its relationship to suicidality and life meaning. Suicide Life-Threatening Behav. 2003;33:231–41. doi:10.1521/suli.33.3.231.23213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maltsberger, JT. The descent into suicide. Int J Psychoanal. 2004;85:653–68. doi:10.1516/3C96-URET-TLWX-6LWU.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orbach, I, Mikulincer, M, Sirota, P, Gilboa-Schechtman, E. Mental pain: a multidimensional operationalization and definition. Suicide Life-Threatening Behav. 2003;33:219–30. doi:10.1521/suli.33.3.219.23219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ducasse, D, Holden, RR, Boyer, L, Artéro, S, Guillaume, S, Courtet, P, et al. It is illegal to post this copyrighted PDF on any website. J Clin Psychiatry. 2017;78:16108. doi:10.4088/JCP.16r10732.Google Scholar
Olié, E, Guillaume, S, Jaussent, I, Courtet, P, Jollant, F. Higher psychological pain during a major depressive episode may be a factor of vulnerability to suicidal ideation and act. J Affect Disord. 2010;120:226–30. doi:10.1016/J.JAD.2009.03.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alacreu-Crespo, A, Cazals, A, Courtet, P, Olié, E. Brief assessment of psychological pain to predict suicidal events at one year in depressed patients. Psychother Psychosom. 2020;89:320–3. doi:10.1159/000506957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, NI, Lieberman, MD. Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:294300. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, G, Leary, MR. Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychol Bull. 2005;131:202–23. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abrutyn, S. Toward a sociological theory of social pain. J Theory Soc Behav. 2023;121. doi:10.1111/jtsb.12371.Google Scholar
Papini, MR, Fuchs, PN, Torres, C. Behavioral neuroscience of psychological pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;48:5369. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adam, K. Suicidal behavior and attachment: a developmental model. In: Sperling, M, Berman, W, editors. Attachment in adults: clinical & developmental perspectives. New York: Guilford Press; 1994, p. 275–98.Google Scholar
Williams, K, Jarvis, B. Cyberball: a program for use in research on interpersonal ostracism and acceptance. J Behav Res Methods. 2006;38:174–80. doi:10.3758/BF03192765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zarrati, I, Bermas, H, Sabet, M. The relationship between childhood trauma and suicide ideation: mediating role of mental pain. Ann Mil Heal Sci Res. 2019;17. doi:10.5812/amh.89266.Google Scholar
Li, X, You, J, Ren, Y, Zhou, J, Sun, R, Liu, X, et al. A longitudinal study testing the role of psychache in the association between emotional abuse and suicidal ideation. J Clin Psychol. 2019;75:2284–92. doi:10.1002/jclp.22847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, LC, Campos, RC, Morujão, IS. The mediating role of tolerance for psychological pain in the relationship of childhood trauma to suicidal ideation in individuals with a substance use disorder. Br J Clin Psychol. 2022;61:197213. doi:10.1111/bjc.12338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ihme, H, Olié, E, Courtet, P, El-Hage, W, Zendjidjian, X, Mazzola-Pomietto, P, et al. Childhood trauma increases vulnerability to attempt suicide in adulthood through avoidant attachment. Compr Psychiatry. 2022;117. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2022.152333.Google ScholarPubMed
Long, M, Verbeke, W, Ein-Dor, T, Vrtička, P. A functional neuro-anatomical model of human attachment (NAMA): insights from first- and second-person social neuroscience. Cortex. 2020;126:281321. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paquette, D, Laporte, L, Bigras, M, Zoccolillo, M. Validation de la version francaise du CTQ et prévalence de l’histoire de maltraitance. Sante Ment Que. 2004;29:201–20. doi:10.7202/008831ar.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, D, Bartholomew, K. Relationship scales questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994.Google Scholar
Guédeney, N, Fermanian, J, Bifulco, A. Construct validation study of the relationship scales questionnaire (RSQ) on an adult sample. Encéphale. 2010;36:6976. doi:10.1016/j.encep.2008.12.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, AT, Steer, RA, Brown, G. Becks depression inventory - II. APA Psyc tests. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. doi:10.1037/t00742-000.Google Scholar
Desseilles, M, Perroud, N, Guillaume, S, Jaussent, I, Genty, C, Malafosse, A, et al. Is it valid to measure suicidal ideation by depression rating scales? J Affect Disord. 2012;136:398404. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zisook, S, Trivedi, MH, Warden, D, Lebowitz, B, Thase, ME, Stewart, JW, et al. Clinical correlates of the worsening or emergence of suicidal ideation during SSRI treatment of depression: an examination of citalopram in the STAR*D study. J Affect Disord. 2009;117:6373. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nobile, B, Olié, E, Dubois, J, Guillaume, S, Gorwood, P, Courtet, P. Characteristics and treatment outcome of suicidal depression: two large naturalistic cohorts of depressed outpatients. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2022;56:347–64. doi:10.1177/00048674211025697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joiner, TE, Rudd, MD, Rajab, MH. Agreement between self- and clinician-rated suicidal symptoms in a clinical sample of young adults: explaining discrepancies. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999;67:171–6. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.67.2.171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rush, JA, Giles, DE, Schlesser, MA, Fulton, CL, Weissenburger, J, Burns, C. The inventory for depressive symptomatology (IDS): preliminary findings. Psychiatry Res. 1986;18:6587. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(86)90060-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, DD, McGrath, PA, Rafii, A, Buckingham, B. The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain. Pain. 1983;17:4556. doi:10.1016/0304-3959(83)90126-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2017.Google Scholar
Davidson, R, MacKinnon, JG. Estimation and inference in econometrics. New York: Oxford University Press; 1993.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsday, NJ: Jacob Cohen; 1988.Google Scholar
Lessard, JC, Moretti, MM. Suicidal ideation in an adolescent clinical sample: attachment patterns and clinical implications. J Adolesc. 1998;21:383–95. doi:10.1006/jado.1998.0169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riggs, SA, Jacobvitz, D. Expectant parents’ representations of early attachment relationships: associations with mental health and family history. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70:195204. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.1.195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beevers, CG. Cognitive vulnerability to depression: a dual process model. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25:9751002. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.03.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haeffel, GJ, Abramson, LY, Brazy, PC, Shah, JY, Teachman, BA, Nosek, BA. Explicit and implicit cognition: a preliminary test of a dual-process theory of cognitive vulnerability to depression. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:1155–67. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, NL, Feeney, BC. Working models of attachment shape perceptions of social support: evidence from experimental and observational studies. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;87:363–83. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mogg, K, Bradbury, KE, Bradley, BP. Interpretation of ambiguous information in clinical depression. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44:1411–9. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P, Fonagy, P. The stress-reward-mentalizing model of depression: an integrative developmental cascade approach to child and adolescent depressive disorder based on the research domain criteria (RDoC) approach. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;64:8798. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2017.09.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, TT, Vanderlind, WM, Selby, EA, Beevers, CG. Childhood abuse and vulnerability to depression: cognitive scars in otherwise healthy young adults. Cogn Emot. 2014;28:821–33. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.864258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikulincer, M, Shaver, PR. Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Curr Opin Psychol. 2019;25:610. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:969–77. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iffland, B, Neuner, F. Varying cognitive scars – differential associations between types of childhood maltreatment and facial emotion processing. Front Psychol. 2020;11:112. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateman, A, Fonagy, P. 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality disorder: mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:631–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Høyen, KS, Solem, S, Cohen, LJ, Prestmo, A, Hjemdal, O, Vaaler, AE, et al. Non-disclosure of suicidal ideation in psychiatric inpatients: rates and correlates. Death Stud. 2022;46:1823–31. doi:10.1080/07481187.2021.1879317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Husky, MM, Zablith, I, Alvarez Fernandez, V, Kovess-Masfety, V. Factors associated with suicidal ideation disclosure: results from a large population-based study. J Affect Disord. 2016;205:3643. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cukrowicz, KC, Duberstein, PR, Vannoy, SD, Lin, EH, Unützer, J. What factors determine disclosure of suicide ideation in adults 60 and older to a treatment provider? Suicide Life-Threat Behav. 2014;44:331–7. doi:10.1111/sltb.12075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Struck, N, Krug, A, Feldmann, M, Yuksel, D, Stein, F, Schmitt, S, et al. Attachment and social support mediate the association between childhood maltreatment and depressive symptoms. J Affect Disord. 2020;273:310–7. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.04.041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canetto, SS, Sakinofsky, I. The gender paradox in suicide. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 1998;28:123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, JR, Reuben, A, Newbury, JB, Danese, A. Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:584–93. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Everaert, J, Vrijsen, JN, Martin-Willett, R, van de Kraats, L, Joormann, J. A meta-analytic review of the relationship between explicit memory bias and depression: depression features an explicit memory bias that persists beyond a depressive episode. Psychol Bull. 2022;148:435–63. doi:10.1037/bul0000367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, RC, Berglund, P, Nock, M, Wang, PS, Page, P. Trends in suicide ideation, plans, gestures, and attempts in the United States, 1990–1992 to 2001–2003. JAMA. 2005;293:2487–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Description of socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the whole sample (n = 161)

Figure 1

Figure 1. Bivariate Spearman’s Rho correlations, with only significant coefficients being displayed. Strength and direction of the relationship (positive or negative) of the correlation is labeled and additionally indicated by color (negative correlation depicted in red, positive correlation in blue) and size of the square. Correlation coefficients were interpreted according to Cohen’s conventions [62]. A Rho coefficient |ρ| ≥ 0.10 indicates a weak relationship, |ρ| ≥ 0.30 a mediocre relationship, and |ρ| ≥ 0.50 a strong relationship.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Mediation model on the effect of childhood trauma on suicidal ideation through its effect through three parallel attachment mediators. Regression coefficients are labeled with significant levels *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Significant paths are in bold.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Mediation models on the conditional effect of childhood trauma on suicidal ideation through its effect through three parallel mediators, factors avoidance, anxiety, and security in function of the moderator psychological pain. Regression coefficients are labeled with significant levels *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Regression coefficients for b are only displayed for the effect of RSQ-anxiety on suicidal ideation, as it was the only path where significant index of moderate mediation was found. The calculated b and c’ coefficients are marked at the side of the diagram. Significant paths are in bold.

Supplementary material: File

Ihme et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 714.9 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.