Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T04:58:44.969Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lateralization of attention in adults with ADHD: Evidence of pseudoneglect

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2020

Bartosz Helfer*
Affiliation:
National Heart and Lung Institute, Faculty of Medicine, Imperial College London, London, United Kingdom
Stefanos Maltezos
Affiliation:
Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Science, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom Adult Autism and ADHD Service, South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
Elizabeth Liddle
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry and Applied Psychology, Institute of Mental Health, Faculty of Medicine & Health Sciences, University of Nottingham, United Kingdom
Jonna Kuntsi
Affiliation:
Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
Philip Asherson
Affiliation:
Social Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
*
Author for correspondence: Bartosz Helfer, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Background.

We investigated whether adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) show pseudoneglect—preferential allocation of attention to the left visual field (LVF) and a resulting slowing of mean reaction times (MRTs) in the right visual field (RVF), characteristic of neurotypical (NT) individuals —and whether lateralization of attention is modulated by presentation speed and incentives.

Method.

Fast Task, a four-choice reaction-time task where stimuli were presented in LVF or RVF, was used to investigate differences in MRT and reaction time variability (RTV) in adults with ADHD (n = 43) and NT adults (n = 46) between a slow/no-incentive and fast/incentive condition. In the lateralization analyses, pseudoneglect was assessed based on MRT, which was calculated separately for the LVF and RVF for each condition and each study participant.

Results.

Adults with ADHD had overall slower MRT and increased RTV relative to NT. MRT and RTV improved under the fast/incentive condition. Both groups showed RVF-slowing with no between-group or between-conditions differences in RVF-slowing.

Conclusion.

Adults with ADHD exhibited pseudoneglect, a NT pattern of lateralization of attention, which was not attenuated by presentation speed and incentives.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Hemispheric asymmetry of the brain is universal in the animal kingdom, is well-supported by anatomical and molecular research [Reference Duboc, Dufourcq, Blader and Roussigné1], and offers cognitive survival advantages [Reference Duboc, Dufourcq, Blader and Roussigné1]. In humans, functional lateralization shows positive correlations with cognitive abilities as shown in functional magnetic resonance imagining (fMRI) studies [Reference Gotts, Jo, Wallace, Saad, Cox and Martin2]. Research involving patients after corpus callosotomy documented that left human hemisphere is dominant for language function, logical thinking, or local processing, whereas the right hemisphere is oriented toward visuospatial attention and global processing [Reference Gazzaniga3]. These differences are underlined by specific changes in gray matter volume or white matter density [Reference Good, Johnsrude, Ashburner, Henson, Friston and Frackowiak4].

Because humans are binocular, and their brains have an optic chiasm, signals from the left visual hemifield (LVF) and the right visual hemifield (RVF) are contra‑lateralized in the brain, such that information from LVF of both eyes is sent to the right hemisphere and vice versa. However, when it comes to allocation of attention, the right hemisphere tends to attend to both visual fields, whereas the left hemisphere allocates attention predominantly to RVF [Reference Mangun, Luck, Plager, Loftus, Hillyard and Handy5].

The right hemispheric dominance for visuospatial attention [Reference Heilman and Van Den Abell6,Reference Shulman, Pope, Astafiev, McAvoy, Snyder and Corbetta7] is often referred to as “pseudoneglect,” a phenomenon where neurotypical (NT) individuals show small but robust attentional bias to the left [Reference Bowers and Heilman8,Reference Jewell and McCourt9]. However, studies in children and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) suggest atypical hemispheric asymmetries at both structural and functional levels [Reference Bradshaw and Sheppard10Reference Rubia12].

Over three decades ago, it was noted that children with ADHD resemble adults with right hemispheric dysfunction [Reference Voeller and Heilman13]. Since then, a link between ADHD and a rightward attentional bias was documented in boys with ADHD in a line bisection task [Reference Manly, Cornish, Grant, Dobler and Hollis14], as well as in young adults with ADHD who made more LVF (but not RVF) omission errors in a cancellation tasks compared to NT individuals [Reference Jones, Craver-Lemley and Barrett15]. A similar pattern of higher mean LVF versus RVF omission errors was found in adults with ADHD compared to NT individuals in a letter cancellation task [Reference Sandson, Bachna and Morin16].

The cognitive‑energetic model of ADHD focuses on the role of arousal, postulating that cognitive deficits seen in people with ADHD result from a reduced energetic state [Reference Sergeant17]. According to this model, the optimization of under-arousal (low energetic state) in people with ADHD results in reduction of attentional lapses and faster, less variable, and more accurate responses. Findings described above gave rise to a hemispheric hypoarousal hypothesis of attentional dysfunction, which was further supported by findings showing that methylphenidate normalizes performance in the line bisection task in children with ADHD [Reference Sheppard, Bradshaw, Mattingley and Lee18]. Neuroscientific evidence lends some support to this hypothesis, as healthy children with a dopamine transporter (DAT1) risk variant for ADHD [Reference Gizer, Ficks and Waldman19] were found to have poorer attention in the LVF in a visual orienting task [Reference Bellgrove, Chambers, Johnson, Daibhis, Daly and Hawi20]. This is potentially important as in children with ADHD, inattention to stimuli in the LVF can be improved (including normalization of performance in the line bisection task) by methylphenidate treatment which blocks the DAT1 [Reference Sheppard, Bradshaw, Mattingley and Lee18,Reference Volkow, Wang, Fowler, Gatley, Logan and Ding21]. The DAT1 variant [Reference Gizer, Ficks and Waldman19] was also reported to be associated with disturbed patterns of activation in fMRI studies in both adults and children with ADHD, including the left dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) [Reference Brown, Biederman, Valera, Makris, Doyle and Whitfield-Gabrieli22]. ACC plays a crucial role in many higher level functions, including attention allocation, reward processing [Reference Stevens, Hurley and Taber23], or boredom [Reference Danckert and Merrifield24]. Additionally, a previous study focusing on patients with spatial neglect identified structural abnormalities within right putamen, caudate nucleus, pulvinar, and superior temporal gyrus as the neurological basis for spatial neglect [Reference Karnath, Himmelbach and Rorden25]. Analogously, recent meta-analyses in children and adults with ADHD found reduced volumes in the right putamen [Reference Norman, Carlisi, Lukito, Hart, Mataix-Cols and Radua26] and consistent under‑activation in the right caudate nucleus during cognitive tasks [Reference Rubia12].

Crucially however, and somewhat consistently with the cognitive‑energetic model [Reference Sergeant17], level of arousal might have a modulating effect on the symptoms of spatial neglect both in patients with brain lesions and people with ADHD. It has been established that increasing alertness can help to overcome the perceptual spatial neglect in right-hemisphere patients [Reference Robertson, Mattingley, Rorden and Driver27]. It has been found that under lower perceptual load in a flanker task, children with ADHD exhibit hyper‑distractibility in the RVF compared to the LVF (i.e., higher interference effect for RVF vs. LVF target displays)—an opposite pattern of interference effects compared to NT children (who show greater interference for LVF vs. RVF target displays) [Reference Chan, Mattingley, Huang-Pollock, English, Hester and Vance28].

Recent studies investigating adults with ADHD identified neuroanatomical correlates of the dominant role of the right hemisphere in visuospatial attention. For example, the superior longitudinal fasciculus shows hemispheric asymmetry in volume [Reference De Schotten, Dell’Acqua, Forkel, Simmons, Vergani and Murphy29], and the white matter microstructure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus is compromised in adults with ADHD [Reference Hamilton, Levitt, O’Neill, Alger, Luders and Phillips30]. Additionally, the reduced microstructural integrity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus is associated with reaction time abnormalities typical of adults with ADHD [Reference Wolfers, Onnink, Zwiers, Arias-Vasquez, Hoogman and Mostert31].

Apart from classic tasks mentioned above, such as the line bisection task or the letter/shape cancellation tasks, hemispheric differences can be studied using other simple cognitive tasks, where stimuli are randomized and presented separately in the LVF or RVF. Based on speed and accuracy of responses in the LVF and RVF, it is possible to make inferences about the underlying hemispheric processes [Reference Hale, Loo, Zaidel, Hanada, Macion and Smalley11]. Therefore, in this study we employed a lateralized version of the Fast Task [Reference Kuntsi, Andreou, Ma, Borger and van der Meere32], a four-choice reaction-time task where stimuli were presented in the LVF and RVF, to investigate differences in mean reaction time (MRT) and reaction time variability (RTV) in adults with ADHD and NT adults between slow/no-incentive and fast/incentive conditions.

Increased intra‑individual variability in reaction times (RTV) might be a marker of ADHD as it is one of a few cognitive performance measures consistently producing reliable results in people with ADHD during speeded reaction time tasks [Reference Karalunas, Geurts, Konrad, Bender and Nigg33Reference Kuntsi35]. It has been proposed that the increased RTV in people with ADHD might represent fluctuations in attention related to disrupted sensitivity to reward and an underlying arousal deficit [Reference Kuntsi and Klein36]. RTV and MRT in individuals with ADHD can be investigated with the Fast Task, where a slow/no-incentive condition is followed by a fast/incentive condition in which the event rate is increased and performance incentivized [Reference Kuntsi, Andreou, Ma, Borger and van der Meere32,Reference Andreou, Neale, Chen, Christiansen, Gabriels and Heise37]. Improvements or normalization in the ADHD group in RTV and MRT under the fast/incentive condition is well-documented in two large meta-analyses showing small to medium effect sizes [Reference Karalunas, Geurts, Konrad, Bender and Nigg33,Reference Kofler, Rapport, Sarver, Raiker, Orban and Friedman34].

Therefore, we expect that in this study adults with ADHD will show overall increased MRT and RTV relative to NT group, as well as a relative improvement in both measures under the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task. In the lateralization analyses, we expect a typical pattern of pseudoneglect in the NT group, that is, a preferential allocation of attention to the LVF and a resulting prolongation of reaction times in the RVF (i.e., RVF‑slowing). As suggested by the research discussed above, we expect an opposite pattern of results for the ADHD group, that is, inattention to the left (rightward bias) resulting in longer reaction times in the LVF (LVF‑slowing). Furthermore, we hypothesize that this pattern of results would be more pronounced in the slow/no-incentive condition of the Fast Task relative to the fast/incentive condition, with a possibility of adults with ADHD normalizing the rightward bias in the fast/incentive condition, due to the purported effect an elevated level of arousal has on normalizing spatial neglect [Reference Robertson, Mattingley, Rorden and Driver27,Reference Chan, Mattingley, Huang-Pollock, English, Hester and Vance28,Reference George, Dobler, Nicholls and Manly38].

Methods

Sample

The sample for this study consisted of 43 adults with ADHD and 46 NT adults, who volunteered to participate and did not differ in IQ (ADHD = 109.3 ± 15.7, NT = 108.4 ± 11.7, t(87) = 0.312, p = 0.756). We used G*Power 3.1 for sample size estimation. Previous meta-analytic analyses of RTV [Reference Kofler, Rapport, Sarver, Raiker, Orban and Friedman34] reported bias-corrected Hedges’ g = 0.57. With power = 0.80 and alpha = 0.05, the projected sample was N = 80, indicating suitability of the achieved sample size. Adults with ADHD were recruited from South London and Maudsley Adult ADHD Outpatient Service. Ethics approval was granted by the Joint South London and Maudsley (SLaM R&D Number: R&D2016/039) and Institute of Psychiatry Research Ethics Committee (REC Reference: 15/LO/2067). All study participants gave full informed consent. Table 1 shows patient background characteristics.

Table 1. Background characteristics of the study sample.

Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; SD, standard deviation.

a Measured by the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale [Reference Barkley39].

b Measured by the Barkley ADHD Functional Impairment Scale [Reference Barkley40].

Clinical measures

Adults with ADHD were diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–fifth edition criteria [Reference APA41] using the Diagnostic Interview for Adult ADHD [Reference Kooij42,Reference Ramos-Quiroga, Nasillo, Richarte, Corrales, Palma and Ibanez43]—a structured clinical interview assessing the symptoms of ADHD in childhood and adulthood in adults. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence was used to measure IQ (Wechsler Reference Wechsler2011). Adults with ADHD who were taking ADHD medication underwent 24–48-h washout phase. NT participants in the control group did not meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD. This was established via a short clinical interview and by applying the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale, a self-rating questionnaire for adult ADHD symptoms [Reference Barkley39], and the Barkley Functional Impairment Scale, a 10-item self-rating questionnaire for functional impairment related to ADHD [Reference Barkley40]. We excluded participants with major co-occurring medical or mental health disorders including autism spectrum disorder, current episode of depression, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, addiction disorder, schizophrenia, antisocial personality disorder, anxiety with panic attacks, and any signs of psychosis or hypomania/mania.

Cognitive testing

We used a variant of the Fast Task [Reference Kuntsi, Andreou, Ma, Borger and van der Meere32,Reference Andreou, Neale, Chen, Christiansen, Gabriels and Heise37] where the stimuli were lateralized to the LVF or RVF, that is, positioned in a concentric pattern around a central fixation cross. Participants were presented with four empty circles arranged in the upper-left, upper-right, bottom-left, and bottom-right corner of the screen. After a delay period, a circle designated as the target signal for that trial was filled in (colored in yellow). Participants were asked to make a compatible choice by pressing one of four corresponding buttons on a small numeric keyboard. Following a response, the circles disappeared from the screen and a fixed inter-trial interval of 2,500 ms followed. First, a practice session was administered, during whichparticipants had to respond correctly to five consecutive trials. Then, two conditions followed, a slow/no-incentive condition and a fast/incentive condition. The slow/no-incentive condition consisted of 72 trials with a foreperiod of 8 s. The fast condition consisted of 80 trials with 1-s foreperiod and incentives. In both conditions, all trials contained targets, which were equally and randomly distributed across both visual fields. If a participant responded quicker than their MRT during the slow/no-incentive condition (based on the middle 94% of responses, excluding extremely fast and slow responses) for three consecutive trials, they won a smiley face. The number of won smiley faces appeared during the inter-trial interval instead of the fixation point and represented a reward. The Fast Task took about 20 min to complete. See Figure 1 for an illustration of the experimental paradigm.

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental paradigm (the Fast Task) with a slow/no-incentive condition and a fast/incentive condition.

Equipment and data recording

Data were recorded and the task administered using BGaze Player by Braingaze. All study participants used a chin-rest to facilitate recording.

Data processing

RTV was calculated as standard deviation of MRT for each study participant. In the lateralization analyses, MRT was calculated separately for the LVF and RVF for each condition and each study participant.

Statistical analyses

We used a 2 x 2 x 2 mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) to investigate the effect of MRT and RTV between visual field (RVF vs. LVF) and task condition (slow/no-incentive condition vs. fast/incentive condition) as within-subject factors in both groups (ADHD group vs. NT group) with Bonferroni‑corrected post-hoc tests. If the assumption of homogeneity of variances was violated in any analysis (assessed by Levene’s test for equality of variances, p < 0.05), we reported Welch ANOVA and post-hoc tests with the Games-Howell correction.

Results

Mean reaction time

We found a main effect of task condition on MRT, F(1, 83) = 182.811, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.688. Post-hoc tests showed speeding of MRT from slow/no-incentive to fast/incentive condition with a mean difference of 156 ms. We also found a main effect of group, F(1, 83) = 5.372, p = 0.023, partial η 2 = 0.061, with MRT 89 ms slower in the ADHD group between both conditions. There was statistically significant group-by-condition interaction, F(1, 83) = 7.288, p = 0.008, partial η 2 = 0.081, so that the difference between the ADHD group and the NT group was larger in the slow/no-incentive condition (mean difference = 120 ms), than in the fast/incentive condition (mean difference = 58 ms). Please see Figure 2 for the MRT data across task conditions and groups.

Figure 2. Boxplots showing mean reaction time (MRT) data from the slow/no-incentive and the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MRT, mean reaction time; NT, neurotypical.

Reaction time variability

A similar pattern of results was found for the RTV. There was a main effect of task condition, F(1, 83) = 28.517, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.256. Post hoc tests showed a reduction in RTV from slow/no-incentive to fast/incentive condition. We also found a main effect of group, F(1, 83) = 9.508, p = 0.003, partial η 2 = 0.103, with average RTV higher in the ADHD group. There was also a statistically significant group-by-condition interaction, F(1, 83) = 6.158, p = 0.015, partial η 2 = 0.069, so that the difference between the ADHD group and the NT group was larger in the slow/no-incentive condition, than in the fast/incentive condition. Please see Figure 3 for the RTV data across task conditions and groups.

Figure 3. Boxplots showing reaction time variability (RTV) data from the slow/no-incentive and the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; NT, neurotypical; RTV, reaction time variability.

Lateralization analyses

There was no statistically significant three-way interaction between visual field (LVF or RVF), task condition (slow/no-incentive vs. fast/incentive condition), and group (ADHD vs. NT) for MRT, F(1, 83) = 0.327, p = 0.569, partial η 2 = 0.004, as well as for RTV, F(1, 81) = 1.841, p = 0.179, partial η 2 = 0.022. There was also no statistically significant two-way interaction between visual field and group, for both MRT, F(1, 83) = 0.009, p = 0.924, partial η 2 = 0.000, and RTV, F(1, 81) = 0.559, p = 0.457, partial η 2 = 0.007. Finally, there was no statistically significant interaction between condition and visual field in the MRT analysis, F(1, 83) = 2.832, p = 0.096, partial η 2 = 0.033, as well as in the RTV analysis, F(1, 81) = 1.182, p = 0.280, partial η 2 = 0.014. Consistently with the above MRT and RTV analyses, we found a statistically significant two-way interaction between condition and group in the MRT, F(1, 83) = 7.551, p = 0.007, partial η 2 = 0.083, as well as RTV analysis, F(1, 81) = 8.403, p = 0.005, partial η 2 = 0.094. There was a main effect of condition in the MRT analysis, F(1, 83) = 184.706, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.690, as well as in the RTV analysis, F(1, 81) = 27.528, p < 0.001, partial η 2 = 0.254.

We found a main effect of visual field in the MRT, F(1, 83) = 3.978, p = 0.049, partial η 2 = 0.046, but not in the RTV analysis, F(1, 81) = 2.937, p = 0.090, partial η 2 = 0.035. Only the overall effect of RVF‑slowing was statistically significant, and there was no significant interaction with group or condition. See Figures 46 for summary of MRT data presented in this section.

Figure 4. Boxplots showing mean reaction time (MRT) data from the left and right visual field in the slow/no-incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MRT, mean reaction time; NT, neurotypical.

Figure 5. Boxplots showing mean reaction time (MRT) data from left and right visual field in the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MRT, mean reaction time; NT, neurotypical.

Figure 6. Mean reaction time in milliseconds in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical group (NT) between the slow/no-incentive condition and the fast/incentive condition in the right visual field (RVF) and the left visual field (LVF). Error bars represent standard error. Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; LVF, left visual field; NT, neurotypical; RVF, right visual field.

Discussion

In lateralization analyses, we found the expected pattern of results for NT adults, consistent with pseudoneglect. Adults with ADHD showed the same pattern of lateralization of attention as NT adults. There was no evidence to support the rightward bias resulting in longer MRT in the LVF in the ADHD group.

We found increased MRT and RTV in adults with ADHD relative to NT adults and a significant improvement in both groups across both measures under the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task. The difference between adults with ADHD and NT adults was larger in the slow/no-incentive condition, than in the fast/incentive condition. These findings are in line with results of a recent meta-analysis investigating MRT and RTV in adults with ADHD [Reference Kofler, Rapport, Sarver, Raiker, Orban and Friedman34]. Our results from MRT and RTV analyses showing improvement in the fast/incentive condition in the ADHD group might be linked to theories and results, including from Fast Task, indicating disturbed arousal as the source of task-unrelated activity leading to decreased cognitive performance [Reference Sergeant17,Reference Du Rietz, James, Banaschewski, Brandeis, Asherson and Kuntsi44]. The fact that performance is improved under more optimal arousal state in the fast/incentive condition suggests that internal processes became more task-oriented.

Our lateralization findings stand in contrast with some previous studies investigating hemispheric and visual field effects in people with ADHD. There are several factors that might account for this discrepancy. Previous studies focused on children with ADHD, and only one involved young adults (college students) [Reference Jones, Craver-Lemley and Barrett15]. Another study used MRT to evaluate rightward bias in ADHD and identified an increased lateralized interference effect (i.e., a difference between congruent and incongruent trials in the RVF), but found that reaction time data for target position showed differences only in response times between central versus peripheral targets, but not between LVF versus RVF targets [Reference Chan, Mattingley, Huang-Pollock, English, Hester and Vance28]. Some earlier studies used cancellation or line bisection tasks, which focus on omission errors rather than reaction time [Reference Manly, Cornish, Grant, Dobler and Hollis14,Reference Jones, Craver-Lemley and Barrett15]. These differences might account for the fact that we did not find the expected rightward bias (LVF-slowing). A closer scrutiny of the older studies reveals that some of the boys diagnosed with ADHD in fact made extreme line dissection errors in the opposite direction as expected (consistent with leftward bias) [Reference Manly, Cornish, Grant, Dobler and Hollis14]. In a study involving adults with ADHD, the case–control difference in LVF omission errors was only observed in a letter cancellation task and not in a shape cancellation task, suggesting a possible confounding by undiagnosed dyslexia [Reference Jones, Craver-Lemley and Barrett15].

As more studies involving samples of adults with ADHD are lacking, we can only speculate whether the behavioral manifestations of spatial neglect in people with ADHD simply normalize with age. Given a large overall slowing in MRT in adults with ADHD, a left-hemispheric disturbance may partially account for processing speed deficits and attentional lapses in the RVF. Future research on lateralization of attention in adults with ADHD could investigate whether the underlying mechanisms leading to pseudoneglect in adults with ADHD is also no different to NT individuals. Below we offer a simple working hypothesis based on some recent findings regarding the default mode network (DMN).

The DMN includes nodes that are in both the left and the right hemisphere, but neuroimaging research suggests that this network is partially left-lateralized [45–47]. The DMN consists of interconnected cortical regions, including ventromedial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex, which are activated (positively correlated) during rest and deactivated (anticorrelated) in response to attentional task demands [Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter48]. Individuals with ADHD have disturbed DMN connectivity leading to hyperactivation of the DMN during cognitive tasks [Reference Christakou, Murphy, Chantiluke, Cubillo, Smith and Giampietro49], which results in a negative influence on task performance measures [Reference Posner, Park and Wang50]. Such “DMN interference” [Reference Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos51] has been demonstrated in adults with ADHD [Reference Mowinckel, Alnaes, Pedersen, Ziegler, Fredriksen and Kaufmann52] and interferes with normal vigilance as reflected in increased MRT [Reference Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos51,Reference Peterson, Potenza, Wang, Zhu, Martin and Marsh53]. Crucially, it has been found that increased activity in the DMN found in people with ADHD is lateralized to the left hemisphere during cognitive task performance [Reference Hale, Kane, Kaminsky, Tung, Wiley and McGough54] and that the activity in the left-lateralized areas of DMN in people with ADHD is highest in tasks using slow event rates [Reference Metin, Krebs, Wiersema, Verguts, Gasthuys and Meere55]. Following this interpretation, overactivity of the DMN in the left hemisphere might lead to interference with on-task activity, resulting in poorer performance in the RVF. Moreover, the slow/no-incentive condition of the Fast Task is designed to induce low-arousal state and is reliably regarded by study participants as boring [Reference Kuntsi, Andreou, Ma, Borger and van der Meere32,Reference Andreou, Neale, Chen, Christiansen, Gabriels and Heise37], and both low arousal and the feeling of boredom are strongly correlated with increased DMN activation [Reference Danckert and Merrifield24,Reference Fan, Xu, Van Dam, Eilam-Stock, Gu and Luo56,Reference Raffaelli, Mills and Christoff57]. To investigate whether DMN might play a role in lateralization of attention in adults with ADHD, a replication of our study in an fMRI scanner would be necessary.

Financial Support

This study was sponsored by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 643051. Bartosz Helfer and Philip Asherson were both supported under the same grant. This study reflects the authors’ views and none of the funders holds any responsibility for the information provided.

Conflict of Interest

Professor Asherson has received funds for consultancy on behalf of King’s College London to Shire, Eli-Lilly, and Novartis, regarding the diagnosis and treatment of people with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); has received educational/research awards from Shire, Eli-Lilly, Novartis, Vifor Pharma, GW Pharma, and QbTech; and was speaker at sponsored events for Shire, Eli-Lilly, and Novartis. All funds are used for studies of people with ADHD. The other authors report no conflicts of interest.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated in this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgment

We would like to thank the National Adult ADHD Clinic at the South London and Maudsley Hospital (SLaM) and all study participants.

References

Duboc, V, Dufourcq, P, Blader, P, Roussigné, M. Asymmetry of the brain: development and implications. Annu Rev Genet. 2015;49(1):647672. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genet-112414-055322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotts, SJ, Jo, HJ, Wallace, GL, Saad, ZS, Cox, RW, Martin, A. Two distinct forms of functional lateralization in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:E3435E3444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gazzaniga, MS. Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(8):653659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Good, CD, Johnsrude, I, Ashburner, J, Henson, RN, Friston, KJ, Frackowiak, RS. Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14(3):685700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mangun, GR, Luck, SJ, Plager, R, Loftus, W, Hillyard, SA, Handy, T, et al.Monitoring the visual world: hemispheric asymmetries and subcortical processes in attention. J Cogn Neurosci. 1994;6(3):267275. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.3.267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilman, KM, Van Den Abell, T. Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology. 1980;30(3):327330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shulman, GL, Pope, DLW, Astafiev, SV, McAvoy, MP, Snyder, AZ, Corbetta, M. Right hemisphere dominance during spatial selective attention and target detection occurs outside the dorsal fronto-parietal network. J Neurosci. 2010;30(10):36403651. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-09.2010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, D, Heilman, KM. Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia. 1980;18(4–5):491498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jewell, G, McCourt, ME. Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(1):93110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradshaw, JL, Sheppard, DM. The neurodevelopmental frontostriatal disorders: evolutionary adaptiveness and anomalous lateralization. Brain Lang. 2000;73(2):297320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, TS, Loo, SK, Zaidel, E, Hanada, G, Macion, J, Smalley, SL. Rethinking a right hemisphere deficit in ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2009;13(1):317. doi: 10.1177/1087054708323005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K. Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and its clinical translation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:100. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voeller, KK, Heilman, KM. Attention deficit disorder in children: a neglect syndrome? Neurology. 1988;38(5):806808. doi: 10.1212/wnl.38.5.806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, T, Cornish, K, Grant, C, Dobler, V, Hollis, C. Examining the relationship between rightward visuo-spatial bias and poor attention within the normal child population using a brief screening task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(12):13371344. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01432.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, KE, Craver-Lemley, C, Barrett, AM. Asymmetrical visual-spatial attention in college students diagnosed with ADD/ADHD. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2008;21(3):176178. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e318185e6a9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandson, TA, Bachna, KJ, Morin, MD. Right hemisphere dysfunction in ADHD: visual hemispatial inattention and clinical subtype. J Learn Disabil. 2000;33(1):8390. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sergeant, J. The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24(1):712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheppard, DM, Bradshaw, JL, Mattingley, JB, Lee, P. Effects of stimulant medication on the lateralisation of line bisection judgements of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;66(1):5763. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.66.1.57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gizer, IR, Ficks, C, Waldman, ID. Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Hum Genet. 2009;126(1):5190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellgrove, MA, Chambers, CD, Johnson, KA, Daibhis, A, Daly, M, Hawi, Z, et al.Dopaminergic genotype biases spatial attention in healthy children. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(8):786792. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, ND, Wang, GJ, Fowler, JS, Gatley, SJ, Logan, J, Ding, YS, et al.Dopamine transporter occupancies in the human brain induced by therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(10):13251331. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, AB, Biederman, J, Valera, E, Makris, N, Doyle, A, Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, et al.Relationship of DAT1 and adult ADHD to task-positive and task-negative working memory networks. Psychiatry Res. 2011;193(1):716. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.01.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, FL, Hurley, RA, Taber, KH. Anterior cingulate cortex: unique role in cognition and emotion. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011;23(2):121125. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.23.2.121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danckert, J, Merrifield, C. Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Exp Brain Res. 2018;236(9):25072518. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4617-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karnath, HO, Himmelbach, M, Rorden, C. The subcortical anatomy of human spatial neglect: putamen, caudate nucleus and pulvinar. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 2):350360. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, LJ, Carlisi, C, Lukito, S, Hart, H, Mataix-Cols, D, Radua, J, et al.Structural and functional brain abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparative meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(8):815825. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, IH, Mattingley, JB, Rorden, C, Driver, J. Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness. Nature. 1998;395(6698):169172. doi: 10.1038/25993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, E, Mattingley, JB, Huang-Pollock, C, English, T, Hester, R, Vance, A, et al.Abnormal spatial asymmetry of selective attention in ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(9):10641072. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02096.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Schotten, MT, Dell’Acqua, F, Forkel, SJ, Simmons, A, Vergani, F, Murphy, DG, et al.A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(10):12451246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, LS, Levitt, JG, O’Neill, J, Alger, JR, Luders, E, Phillips, OR, et al.Reduced white matter integrity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroreport. 2008;19(17):17051708. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283174415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfers, T, Onnink, AMH, Zwiers, MP, Arias-Vasquez, A, Hoogman, M, Mostert, JC, et al.Lower white matter microstructure in the superior longitudinal fasciculus is associated with increased response time variability in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40(5):344–51. doi:10.1503/jpn.140154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuntsi, J, Andreou, P, Ma, J, Borger, NA, van der Meere, JJ. Testing assumptions for endophenotype studies in ADHD: reliability and validity of tasks in a general population sample. BMC Psychiatry. 2005;5:40. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-5-40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karalunas, SL, Geurts, HM, Konrad, K, Bender, S, Nigg, JT. Annual research review: reaction time variability in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders: measurement and mechanisms of a proposed trans-diagnostic phenotype. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(6):685710. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kofler, MJ, Rapport, MD, Sarver, DE, Raiker, JS, Orban, SA, Friedman, LM, et al.Reaction time variability in ADHD: a meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2013;33(6):795811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuntsi, J. Commentary: from noise to insight? Reaction time variability in ADHD and autism spectrum disorders–a commentary on Karalunas et al. (2014). J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(6):711713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuntsi, J, Klein, C. Intraindividual variability in ADHD and its implications for research of causal links. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2012;9:6791. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreou, P, Neale, BM, Chen, W, Christiansen, H, Gabriels, I, Heise, A, et al.Reaction time performance in ADHD: improvement under fast-incentive condition and familial effects. Psychol Med . 2007;37(12):1703–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, M, Dobler, V, Nicholls, E, Manly, T. Spatial awareness, alertness, and ADHD: the re-emergence of unilateral neglect with time-on-task. Brain Cogn. 2005;57(3):264275. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkley, RA. Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV). New York, NY: Guilford Press, 2011.Google Scholar
Barkley, RA. Barkley Functional Impairment Scale (BFIS). New York, NY: Guilford Press, 2011.Google Scholar
APA, . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2013.Google Scholar
Kooij, JJS. Adult ADHD: diagnostic assessment and treatment. 3rd ed. London, UK: Springer, 2013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramos-Quiroga, JA, Nasillo, V, Richarte, V, Corrales, M, Palma, F, Ibanez, P, et al.Criteria and concurrent validity of DIVA 2.0: a semi-structured diagnostic interview for adult ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2019;23:1126–35. doi:10.1177/1087054716646451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Du Rietz, E, James, SN, Banaschewski, T, Brandeis, D, Asherson, P, Kuntsi, J. Autonomic arousal profiles in adolescents and young adults with ADHD as a function of recording context. Psychiatry Res. 2019;275:212220. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.03.039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agcaoglu, O, Miller, R, Mayer, AR, Hugdahl, K, Calhoun, VD. Lateralization of resting state networks and relationship to age and gender. Neuroimage. 2015;104:310325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, JA, Zielinski, BA, Ferguson, MA, Lainhart, JE, Anderson, JS. An evaluation of the left-brain vs. right-brain hypothesis with resting state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e71275. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0071275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, N, Eichele, T, Pearlson, G, Kiehl, K, Yu, Q, Calhoun, VD. Lateral differences in the default mode network in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(4):654664. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, RL, Andrews-Hanna, JR, Schacter, DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:138. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christakou, A, Murphy, CM, Chantiluke, K, Cubillo, AI, Smith, AB, Giampietro, V, et al.Disorder-specific functional abnormalities during sustained attention in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and with autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(2):236244. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, J, Park, C, Wang, Z. Connecting the dots: a review of resting connectivity MRI studies in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24(1):315. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9251-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, EJ, Castellanos, FX. Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31(7):977986. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mowinckel, AM, Alnaes, D, Pedersen, ML, Ziegler, S, Fredriksen, M, Kaufmann, T, et al.Increased default-mode variability is related to reduced task-performance and is evident in adults with ADHD. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;16:369382. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.03.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, BS, Potenza, MN, Wang, Z, Zhu, H, Martin, A, Marsh, R, et al.An FMRI study of the effects of psychostimulants on default-mode processing during Stroop task performance in youths with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(11):12861294. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08050724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hale, TS, Kane, AM, Kaminsky, O, Tung, KL, Wiley, JF, McGough, JJ, et al.Visual network asymmetry and default mode network function in ADHD: an fMRI study. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5:81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metin, B, Krebs, RM, Wiersema, JR, Verguts, T, Gasthuys, R, van der Meere, JJ, et al.Dysfunctional modulation of default mode network activity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124(1):208–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J, Xu, P, Van Dam, NT, Eilam-Stock, T, Gu, X, Luo, Y, et al.Spontaneous brain activity relates to autonomic arousal. J Neurosci. 2012;32(33):1117611186. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1172-12.2012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raffaelli, Q, Mills, C, Christoff, K. The knowns and unknowns of boredom: a review of the literature. Exp Brain Res. 2018;236(9):24512462. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-4922-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D.Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence—Second Edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, 2011.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Background characteristics of the study sample.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental paradigm (the Fast Task) with a slow/no-incentive condition and a fast/incentive condition.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Boxplots showing mean reaction time (MRT) data from the slow/no-incentive and the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MRT, mean reaction time; NT, neurotypical.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Boxplots showing reaction time variability (RTV) data from the slow/no-incentive and the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; NT, neurotypical; RTV, reaction time variability.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Boxplots showing mean reaction time (MRT) data from the left and right visual field in the slow/no-incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MRT, mean reaction time; NT, neurotypical.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Boxplots showing mean reaction time (MRT) data from left and right visual field in the fast/incentive condition of the Fast Task in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical (NT) group. Boxes represent interquartile range with the median and mean (x). Whiskers indicate the maximum and minimum values (excluding the outliers which are represented by the dots). Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; MRT, mean reaction time; NT, neurotypical.

Figure 6

Figure 6. Mean reaction time in milliseconds in the group of adults with ADHD and the neurotypical group (NT) between the slow/no-incentive condition and the fast/incentive condition in the right visual field (RVF) and the left visual field (LVF). Error bars represent standard error. Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; LVF, left visual field; NT, neurotypical; RVF, right visual field.

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.