Introduction
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which is a neurodevelopmental anomaly with an onset in early childhood, affects approximately 5% of school-age children [Reference Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman and Rohde1] as well as 2.5% of adults worldwide [Reference Simon, Czobor, Balint, Meszaros and Bitter2]. Despite the high prevalence, the figures may still be underestimated because of the challenge of making an accurate diagnosis for preschool children [Reference Charach, Carson, Fox, Ali, Beckett and Lim3]. Although the core symptoms of ADHD including inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity are hallmarks of the disease among school-age children (6–12 years of age) and adults [4], the symptoms are more obscure among preschool children (<6 years of age) who may present with disruptive behavior such as tantrums and aggression as well as psychosocial difficulties (e.g., parent–child conflict) [Reference Lavigne, Cicchetti, Gibbons, Binns, Larsen and DeVito5].
Both pharmacological (e.g., psychostimulants and non-stimulants) and non-pharmacological treatments (e.g., behavior therapy) against the symptoms of ADHD are commonly recommended clinical strategies for those diagnosed with ADHD [Reference Catala-Lopez, Hutton, Nunez-Beltran, Page, Ridao and Macias Saint-Gerons6, Reference Coghill, Banaschewski, Cortese, Asherson, Brandeis and Buitelaar7]. Focusing on pharmacologic regimens, a previous large-scale meta-analysis supported the prescription of methylphenidate (i.e., a psychostimulant) for children diagnosed with ADHD [Reference Cortese, Adamo, Del Giovane, Mohr-Jensen, Hayes and Carucci8]. Psychostimulants including methylphenidate and amphetamine derivatives have also been recommended as the first-line treatment according to clinical guidelines for school-aged children diagnosed with ADHD [Reference Cortese9]. Nevertheless, there are concerns about the quality of previous randomized controlled trials (RCT) investigating the efficacy of psychostimulants and also their long-term efficacy [Reference Storebø, Ramstad, Krogh, Nilausen, Skoog and Holmskov10, Reference Punja, Shamseer, Hartling, Urichuk, Vandermeer and Nikles11]. Pharmacological treatment for ADHD in preschool children has posed an even greater challenge for clinicians, given the limited evidence in support of its efficacy and the uncertainties about medication-related side effects in this particular population [Reference Wolraich, Hagan, Allan, Chan, Davison and Earls12]. A previous study focusing on preschoolers reported greater evidence of effectiveness in parent behavior training compared to methylphenidate for the treatment of preschoolers at risk for ADHD. [Reference Charach, Carson, Fox, Ali, Beckett and Lim3]. Consistently, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends parent training in behavior management rather than the pharmacologic approach as the first line of treatment taking into consideration the advantage and efficacy of parent training, the increased susceptibility of young children to medication-related side effects compared to older children as well as the unclear long-term effects of ADHD medications [Reference Wolraich, Hagan, Allan, Chan, Davison and Earls12]. As a result, psychostimulants were only considered for those who exhibited unsatisfactory responses to behavioral therapies [Reference Cortese9].
Nevertheless, not only are preschool children with symptoms of ADHD equally susceptible to psychosocial impairments and developmental problems compared to school-aged children [Reference Halperin and Marks13] but they also have a higher risk of future psychiatric comorbidities and lower school readiness than that in their healthy counterparts [Reference Webster-Stratton, Jamila Reid and Stoolmiller14, Reference Sonuga-Barke, Daley, Thompson and Swanson15]. Although behavioral therapies were found to be effective and are generally recommended as a first-line intervention [Reference Kendall, Taylor, Perez and Taylor16], behavioral problems remain an issue in a significant portion of children who received only behavioral treatments [Reference Charach, Carson, Fox, Ali, Beckett and Lim3]. Moreover, given that parents of ADHD children are themselves at a higher risk of ADHD [Reference Larsson, Chang, D’Onofrio and Lichtenstein17], the effectiveness of parent training may be compromised [Reference Chronis-Tuscano, Wang, Woods, Strickland and Stein18]. Furthermore, previous studies found that not only were children with ADHD prone to injuries [Reference Kang, Lin and Chung19, Reference Altun and Altun20], but this risk was also higher in the preschool population [Reference Kang, Lin and Chung19]. Indeed, a previous survey also showed that more than 50% of clinicians may recommend medication use for preschool children diagnosed with ADHD due to concerns over the impact of impulsiveness on their physical safety. To date, the issue of the efficacy and safety of psychostimulant prescription for ADHD in preschool children remains debated.
Therefore, the aims of this study were to investigate the efficacy of psychostimulants against the core and global symptoms of ADHD, the associated adverse events, and the factors affecting their effectiveness among preschool children.
Materials and Methods
Data sources and search strategy
This systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted according to the PRISMA statement guidelines [Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff and Altman21]. We searched electronic databases including PubMed, the Cochrane Library, EMBASE, ScienceDirect, and ClinicalTrials.gov from inception to March 2022 for eligible RCTs. The protocol of this study was registered with the PROSPERO systematic review protocol registry (Number CRD42022329966).
The literature search was conducted by three independent researchers (YC Cheng, YS Cheng, and CK Sun) by using mainly the following two sets of key terms: (preschool) AND (“psychostimulants” or “methylphenidate” or “amphetamine”). The keywords and limitations used for different databases are provided in Supplementary Table S1. All articles with titles meeting the inclusion criteria were retrieved and reviewed in full text. All original studies investigating the effects of psychostimulants were eligible for review. Additional eligible studies were identified by examining the reference lists of the primary articles and relevant reviews.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
We aimed at determining the therapeutic efficacy of psychostimulants against the symptoms of attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity as well as global symptoms in preschool children with attentional problems. Eligible studies were as follows: (1) RCTs in human, (2) those recruiting preschool children (defined as those ≤6 years of age) who suffered from any attentional or developmental problems, and (3) clinical trials that compared the therapeutic effectiveness against the symptoms of ADHD including inattention, hyperactivity/impulsive and global symptoms between the psychostimulant and control groups. Studies that enrolled children aged equal to or older than 7 years of age, those that were not RCTs, and those that were not in clinical trials were excluded.
Data extraction and quality assessment
Three investigators (YC Cheng, YS Cheng, and CK Sun) independently extracted relevant information from included studies and evaluated the methodological quality of eligible trials using the Cochrane Collaboration risk of bias tools. Data acquired from the studies included the last name of the first author, year of publication, total number of study participants, type of study, sample size of participant in intervention and control groups, gender, intelligence quotient (IQ), and age as well as psychostimulant dosage and duration. The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool was used to evaluate seven domains of risk: selection bias (sequence generation and concealment), performance bias (blinding of participants and assessors), detection bias (blinding of outcome assessment), attrition bias (incomplete outcome data), selective outcome reporting, and other bias. The risk of bias of each item was rated as low, high, or unclear (if there was insufficient information). Disagreements among the three investigators were resolved through discussion. The overall qualities of evidence for different outcomes were rated according to the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluations (GRADE) scoring system [Reference Guyatt, Oxman, Vist, Kunz, Falck-Ytter and Alonso-Coello22].
Efficacy outcomes
The primary outcomes were changes in symptoms of ADHD (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity/ impulsivity, and global symptoms) evaluated by using any clinically validated rating scale from parents’ and teacher’s observation or post-intervention improvements in neuropsychological performance, which were measured by standardized assessment tools such as continuous performance test (CPT). Secondary outcomes were risks of occurrence of adverse events in the psychostimulant group compared with those in the placebo group.
For studies in which relevant data were missing, their authors were contacted by email in an attempt to obtain the necessary information. All potentially relevant manuscripts were independently reviewed by two investigators (YC Cheng and YS Cheng). Areas of disagreement or uncertainty were adjudicated by a third investigator (CK Sun).
Statistical analysis
To calculate the overall effect size, we calculated the standardized mean differences (SMDs) using the formula for Hedges’ g with 95% confidence intervals for continuous outcomes. For dichotomous outcomes, we calculated the odds ratio (OR). Hedges’ g is related to Cohen’s d and can be interpreted using the same conventions: small (0.2), medium (0.5), and large (0.8) [Reference Cohen23]. SMD was calculated to compare the difference in changes in scores after treatment between the psychostimulant and placebo groups. A positive effect size indicated a superior effect of the intervention group in comparison with that in the control group. The standard deviation (SD) of the changes in scores from baseline was calculated using the formula: SD = square root [(SD pretreatment) 2 + (SD post-treatment) 2 − (2R × SD pretreatment × SD post-treatment)], assuming a correlation coefficient (R) = 0.5, when the studies did not report them. When only the standard error of the mean (SEM) was reported, SD was calculated by multiplying the SEM by the square root of the sample size. For studies using median and range, mean and SD were estimated using the formula according to the Cochrane guidelines [Reference Hozo, Djulbegovic and Hozo24, Reference Higgins, Thomas, Chandler, Cumpston, Li and Page25]. In studies that used multiple doses of psychostimulants, we combined the means and SDs for the different dosage groups to give single values for the intervention group. For adverse effects presented as binary outcomes, the effect size was calculated using the OR.
The degree of heterogeneity or inconsistency across the included trials was assessed with I 2 test [Reference Higgins and Thompson26]. To offer more generalizable results, a random effects model was used on the assumption of a variation in true effect size. Publication bias was examined using a funnel plot and Egger’s regression test was used if there were more than 10 datasets [Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder27]. Leave-one-study-out sensitivity analysis was performed by the sequential exclusion of one trial at a time to examine the robustness of the pooled effects. To explore the potential effect of trial-level modifiers, we considered several covariates in meta-regression approaches including mean age, mean daily dosage of methylphenidate, female proportion, treatment duration, and IQ. All meta-analytic computations were performed with the R software (R x64 version 4.1.2, The Cochrane Collaboration, Oxford, UK).
Results
Baseline characteristics of included studies
Figure 1 is a flowchart summarizing the review process in accordance with the PRISMA statement [Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff and Altman28]. Of the 595 original studies screened, nine met the inclusion criteria for qualitative synthesis [Reference Short, Manos, Findling and Schubel29–Reference Conners37]. The reasons for study exclusions are provided in Supplementary Table S2. A summary of the characteristics of the nine included studies for qualitative synthesis and their results are presented in Table 1. The population size of the included studies ranged from 28 to 330 with a total of 544 being preschool children (mean age = 4.86 years, female proportion = 11.98%, median treatment duration = 4.33 weeks). There was a wide variation in dosing strategies across the included studies, including a fixed dose based on body weight, a fixed daily dose, or the best dose approach, instead of reporting a mean dose or dose range. Details on the dosage used in the included studies are summarized in Table 1. The duration of intervention across the included trials was between one and 6 weeks. The results of quality assessment of our included trials assessed by the authors based on the seven domains of risk described in the Cochrane Collaboration tool are presented in Supplementary Figure S1, S2. Overall, most studies had a fair quality in double blinding but did not report clear procedures about their randomization process. Many studies are given a high risk of bias in other biases because they used placebo lead-in [Reference Ghuman, Aman, Lecavalier, Riddle, Gelenberg and Wright32], safety lead-in [Reference Greenhill, Kollins, Abikoff, McCracken, Riddle and Swanson30, Reference Abikoff, Vitiello, Riddle, Cunningham, Greenhill and Swanson34], or excluded participants who failed to respond or tolerate psychostimulants [Reference Childress, Kollins, Foehl, Newcorn, Mattingly and Kupper31, Reference Ghuman, Aman, Lecavalier, Riddle, Gelenberg and Wright32].
Abbreviations: AD, autistic disorder; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ADHD-RS-IV, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder rating scale-IV; ASQ, abbreviated symptoms questionnaire; bid, twice a day; CLAM, Conners, Loney, and Milich rating scales; CNS, central nervous system; CPRS, conners parent rating scale; CPRS-r DSM-IV-ADHD, conners parent rating scales–revised DSM-IV-ADHD; CPT, continuous performance test; CTRS, conners teacher rating scale; d, day; DD, developmental delay; DSM-5, the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edition; DSM-llI-R, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, third edition, revised; DSM-IV, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition; DSM-IV-TR, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, fourth edition, text revision; IQ, intelligence quotient; H/I, hyperactivity/impulsivity; MAS, mixed amphetamine salts; MPH, methylphenidate; MPH IR, methylphenidate immediate release; MPH-MLR, extended-release methylphenidate; N, number, N/A, not available; RCT, randomized controlled trial; PCIT, parent child interaction therapy; PDD-NOS, pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified; SKAMP, Swanson, Kotkin, Atkins, M-Flynn, and Pelham rating scale; SWAN, strengths and weaknesses of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and normal behavior scale; tid, three times a day.
Pooled effects of psychostimulant treatment on global ADHD symptoms
The results of meta-analysis of six trials involving are summarized in Table 2. Our findings showed better treatment efficacy of psychostimulants than placebo for global symptoms of ADHD from parent’s observation in preschool children (Hedges’ g = 0.6152, 95% CI [0.3228; 0.9075], p < 0.0001, I2 = 57.1%) (Figure 2A). With respect to improvements in global symptoms of ADHD from teacher’s observation, our results also demonstrated significantly better treatment efficacy of psychostimulants than placebo (Hedges’ g = 0.6563, 95% CI [0.2110; 1.1016], p = 0.0039, I2 = 78.2%) (Figure 2B). Visual inspection of the funnel plot revealed some degree of asymmetry (Supplementary Figure S3a,b).
Abbreviations: ADHD-RS-IV, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder rating scale-IV; CLAM, Conners, Loney, and Milich rating scales; CPRS-r DSM-IV-ADHD, conners parent rating scales–revised DSM-IV-ADHD; CPT, continuous performance test; CTRS, conners teacher rating scale; SWAN, strengths and weaknesses of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and normal behavior scale; SKAMP, Swanson, Kotkin, Atkins, M-Flynn, and Pelham rating scale; ASQ, abbreviated symptoms questionnaire; CPRS, conners parent rating scale.
p<0.05 statistical singificance ***
Efficacy of psychostimulants against inattention
Analysis of inattentive symptoms showed a significantly better treatment effect of psychostimulants compared with that of placebo from both parents’ (n = 3, Hedges’ g = 0.3268, 95% CI [0.1455; 0.5080], p = 0.0004, I2 = 27.8%) (Figure 2C) and teacher’s observations (n = 5, Hedges’ g = 0.2950, 95% CI [0.1015; 0.4886], p = 0.0028, I2 = 0.0%) (Figure 2D). Visual inspection of funnel plot revealed symmetry (Supplementary Figure S3c,d).
Efficacy of psychostimulants against hyperactivity and impulsivity
Analysis of four studies focusing on the effect of psychostimulants against the symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity revealed a significantly better therapeutic effect of psychostimulants than that of placebo from parents’ observation (Hedges’ g = 0.4510, 95% CI [0.2293; 0.6727], p < 0.0001, I2 = 27.5%) (Figure 2E). In addition, the results from three studies investigating the effect of psychostimulants on the symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity demonstrated a significant beneficial effect from teacher’s observation (Hedges’ g = 0.5933, 95% CI [0.0539; 1.1327], p = 0.0311; I2 = 63.4%) (Figure 2F). Visual inspection of the funnel plot revealed symmetry in results from parents’ observation but some degree of asymmetry in results from teachers’ observation (Supplementary Figure S3e,f).
Efficacy of psychostimulants against omission and commission
Only two out of the nine included studies investigated the effect of psychostimulants on attentional performance including omission and commission by using standardized computerized tests [Reference Musten, Firestone, Pisterman, Bennett and Mercer33, Reference Conners37]. While one study used a CPT [Reference Conners37], the other adopted the Gordon Diagnostic System (GDS) [Reference Musten, Firestone, Pisterman, Bennett and Mercer33]. Our results showed that the therapeutic effect of psychostimulants was not significantly better than that of placebo on omission (Hedges’ g = 0.2518, 95% CI [−0.1131; 0.6168], p = 0.1762, I2 = 0.0%) (Figure 2G) and commission (Hedges’ g = 0.1097, 95% CI [−0.2533; 0.4727], p = 0.5536, I2 = 0.0%) (Figure 2H).Visual inspection of the funnel plot revealed symmetry (Supplementary Figure S3g,h).
Secondary analysis of risk of side effects associated with psychostimulant treatment
A comparison of the occurrence of adverse events between the psychostimulant and placebo groups demonstrated that only the risk of decreased appetite was significantly higher in the psychostimulant group than that in the placebo group (n = 6, OR = 2.3899 [1.0807; 5.2852], p = 0.0314). Although the risks of irritability, sleep disturbance, prone to cry, and anxiety were slightly higher in preschool children receiving psychostimulant treatment than those treated with placebos (OR = 1.06, 1.312, 1.359, and 1.318, respectively), none of these elevated risks reached statistical significance (Supplementary Table S3a). Analysis of the three studies with available information about blood pressure and heart rate showed no significant difference in these parameters between the psychostimulant and placebo groups (Supplementary Table S3b). The common side effects described in the included studies are summarized in Supplementary Table S4.
Robustness of evidence and quality of evidence
An examination of the robustness of the outcomes from the current meta-analyses with a leave-one-out sensitivity analysis revealed inconsistencies in results regarding the symptoms of inattention from parents’ observation, the symptoms of inattention from teachers’ observation, and the symptoms of hyperactivity/Impulsivity from teachers’ observation, suggesting a tentative nature of these findings (Supplementary Table S5). GRADE assessment revealed that the qualities of evidence regarding the study outcomes ranged from low to very low, mainly due to problems of inconsistency and imprecision of study results (Supplementary Table S6).
Factors associated with improvements in global symptoms
Our meta-regression analysis demonstrated a positive association between an older age and a better improvement in the global symptoms of ADHD from parents’ observation (Table 3a). Similarly, improvements in the global symptoms of ADHD were found to be positively associated with an older age and a longer duration of treatment but negatively related to female proportion from a teacher’s perspective (Table 3b). Therefore, our results suggested a better efficacy of psychostimulants in older individuals or in those treated for a longer duration. On the other hand, the efficacy of psychostimulants was poorer in studies that included more females, implying that the observed efficacy of psychostimulants may be less obvious in female participants.
* ß, standardized beta coefficient for meta-regression.
Discussion
Although the therapeutic effectiveness of psychostimulants against symptoms of ADHD has been well-documented in children/adolescents as well as the adult population in previous systematic reviews [Reference Cortese, Adamo, Del Giovane, Mohr-Jensen, Hayes and Carucci8, Reference Correll, Cortese, Croatto, Monaco, Krinitski and Arrondo38, Reference Sun, Tseng, Wu, Li, Chen and Stubbs39], none of them focused particularly on preschool children. To our best knowledge, the present meta-analysis, which included nine RCTs and 544 participants, was the first to demonstrate the effectiveness of psychostimulants against the symptoms of ADHD in preschool children. We further found that not only were the observed treatment efficacies of psychostimulants similar between parents and teachers but psychostimulants also seemed to be more effective for symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity than inattention in this population. Moreover, our meta-regression analysis showed that a longer treatment duration, older age, and male gender were associated with better treatment efficacy.
With respect to our primary outcomes, our findings in support of the therapeutic efficacy of psychostimulants for both core symptoms of ADHD (i.e., inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity) as well as the global symptoms of ADHD indicated that the use of psychostimulants may improve not only the core symptoms of ADHD but also the general behaviors of ADHD children. Nevertheless, sensitivity analysis revealed a loss of significant therapeutic effectiveness against the symptoms of inattention from parents’ observation, the symptoms of inattention from teachers’ observation, and the symptoms of hyperactivity/Impulsivity from teachers’ observation after removal of the trial by Greenhill [Reference Greenhill, Kollins, Abikoff, McCracken, Riddle and Swanson30], suggesting that more studies are required to support the therapeutic efficacy of psychostimulants for these symptoms. Compared with the results of a recent network meta-analysis focusing on other age groups [Reference Cortese, Adamo, Del Giovane, Mohr-Jensen, Hayes and Carucci8], the effect size of treatment efficacy of psychostimulants for global symptoms of ADHD in preschool children was slightly smaller than that in school-aged children (ES = 0.66 versus 0.78, respectively), but similar to that in adult patients (ES = 0.66 versus 0.49 to 0.79 for global symptoms, respectively). However, in contrast to this study that had a relatively small sample size (n = 544), the effect sizes of the previous network meta-analysis were derived from large populations of adults (n = 10,296) and children/adolescents (n = 14,342). Moreover, the qualities of current evidence supporting the use of psychostimulants in this population ranged from low to very low, mainly due to inconsistency and imprecision of study outcomes, as well as limited sample sizes. Therefore, although our results support the use of psychostimulants against the symptoms of ADHD in preschool children, further studies are still needed to provide more tangible evidence.
Despite our finding of moderate treatment efficacy regarding the use of psychostimulants for treating the symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity, the effect size was small for the symptom of inattention. Because the symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity among preschool children are school teachers’ major concerns that can be readily identified [Reference Halperin and Marks13], under-reporting of such symptoms from teachers is unlikely. Consistently, our results showed a slightly larger effect size of treatment efficacy for hyperactivity/impulsivity from a teacher’s perspective than that from parents’ observation (ES = 0.59 versus 0.3, respectively). In contrast, inattention was more difficult to identify in preschool children [Reference Halperin and Marks13]; therefore, it is important to be aware that symptom improvements based on raters’ observations may not reliably reflect the changes in underlying neuropsychological functioning. Of the nine included RCTs, only two provided information about the objective attentional performance of the recruited preschool children independent of observers’ judgment. Interestingly, the effect size for commission, which may be more related to impulsivity, was smaller than the effect size for omission, which may be more representative of attentional functioning. Nevertheless, none of them achieved statistical significance probably due to their small sample sizes. Therefore, although psychostimulants seemed more effective for hyperactivity/impulsivity in this study, more studies especially those including neuropsychological testing that involves a more objective assessment of different aspects of attentional functions [Reference Lamar, Raz, Baum, McManus, Weinman, Wallston, West and Newman40], are required to shed light on the mechanisms underlying the observed improvement in behavioral problems.
Our meta-regression further showed that age and gender differences were both associated with the treatment efficacy of psychostimulants against the global symptoms of ADHD. Taking into account that the higher prevalence of hyperactivity and behavioral disturbances among male ADHD children than among their female counterparts in classes [Reference Rucklidge41], behavioral improvements in hyperactivity/impulsivity may be more easily observed in male preschool children. Besides, the efficacy of psychostimulants may be more easily observed in older age groups because inattention is likely to be ignored in preschool children until they enter a more structured learning environment as they get older [Reference Halperin and Marks13]. Finally, our meta-regression finding of an association between a longer duration of treatment and better therapeutic effects of psychostimulants may suggest a benefit of a prolonged treatment for those who exhibited an unsatisfactory response to initial therapy.
Safety issues and potential adverse effects were the most important concerns regarding the use of psychostimulants in preschool children [Reference Greenhill, Kollins, Abikoff, McCracken, Riddle and Swanson30]. Limited information available in our included trials precluded the conduction of an analysis of the overall dropout rates or dropouts due to adverse events. Nevertheless, our secondary analysis identified the risk of decreased appetite as the only side effect that was significantly related to psychostimulant use, while the risks of other side effects including irritability, sleep disturbance, proneness to cry, and anxiety were only slightly and non-significantly higher in the psychostimulant group than in the placebo group (OR all <1.5). In addition, despite the reported hemodynamic impacts of blood pressure and heart rate elevations associated with psychostimulant use [Reference Jackson42], our results did not demonstrate significant differences in these parameters between the psychostimulant treatment and placebo groups. Overall, psychostimulants seemed well-tolerated in general despite the significant correlation with the risk of decreased appetite in this age group. However, given the possible risks of side effects such as a decrease in appetite and potential influence on heart rate and blood pressure [Reference Jackson42], and low quality of current evidence ranging from low to very low in GRADE assessment, judicious prescription of psychostimulants for preschool children is recommended.
There are several limitations in the present meta-analysis. First, the inclusion of only nine RCTs with 544 participants may preclude an accurate assessment of studies outcomes. In particular, given the limited number of included trials, publication bias was assessed only by visual inspection of the funnel plot rather than analyzed by Egger’s test. In addition, imprecisions and inconsistencies of study results together with the small sample sizes may contribute to the low to very low qualities of evidence derived from the current meta-analysis. Moreover, our sensitivity analysis further revealed a significant impact of one study [Reference Greenhill, Kollins, Abikoff, McCracken, Riddle and Swanson30] on the overall results about the symptoms of inattention from both teachers’ and parents’ observations, as well as hyperactivity/impulsivity from a teacher’s perspective. Therefore, more large-scale RCTs are required for verification of our study outcomes. Second, most of the included studies did not provide clear detail about their randomization processes, and authors in six out of the nine studies received some financial support from pharmaceutical companies [Reference Short, Manos, Findling and Schubel29–Reference Ghuman, Aman, Lecavalier, Riddle, Gelenberg and Wright32, Reference Abikoff, Vitiello, Riddle, Cunningham, Greenhill and Swanson34, Reference Chacko, Pelham, Gnagy, Greiner, Vallano and Bukstein35]. Moreover, most studies adopted a cross-over design without washout periods between placebo and psychostimulant interventions [Reference Short, Manos, Findling and Schubel29, Reference Greenhill, Kollins, Abikoff, McCracken, Riddle and Swanson30, Reference Ghuman, Aman, Lecavalier, Riddle, Gelenberg and Wright32, Reference Musten, Firestone, Pisterman, Bennett and Mercer33, Reference Chacko, Pelham, Gnagy, Greiner, Vallano and Bukstein35, Reference Handen, Feldman, Lurier and Murray36]. Nevertheless, most psychostimulants had a short half-life [Reference Kimko, Cross and Abernethy43] that may not bias our findings. Third, some studies used placebo lead-in [Reference Ghuman, Aman, Lecavalier, Riddle, Gelenberg and Wright32] or safety lead-in [Reference Greenhill, Kollins, Abikoff, McCracken, Riddle and Swanson30, Reference Abikoff, Vitiello, Riddle, Cunningham, Greenhill and Swanson34] designs, and two other studies excluded participants who either failed to respond or could not tolerate psychostimulant treatment in the past [Reference Childress, Kollins, Foehl, Newcorn, Mattingly and Kupper31, Reference Ghuman, Aman, Lecavalier, Riddle, Gelenberg and Wright32]. Therefore, it is possible that these studies may underestimate the occurrence of side effects and over-estimate the observed clinical effectiveness of psychostimulants. Fourth, the wide variations in doses and prescription strategies of psychostimulants (that is fixed dose, dosage based on body weight or flexible dose) across the included studies precluded a subgroup analysis to clarify a dose–response relationship in efficacy and also side effects of psychostimulants in the study populations. Fifth, heterogeneity arising from the use of different behavioral rating scales and exclusion of autism or intellectual functioning in some of the trials may also bias our results. Sixth, only six out of the 11 included studies provided information about side effect profiles and categorization (e.g., irritability or emotional lability). Moreover, because the approaches to side effect categorization were not unified across those studies, there was a potential risk of under- or over-reporting of certain side effects. Finally, all trials were conducted in North America; therefore, our results may not be ethnically or geographically extrapolated to other countries.
Conclusion
In summary, our results supported the effectiveness of psychostimulants against the symptoms of ADHD in preschool children, especially for hyperactivity/impulsivity from teachers’ observation. Better therapeutic effects of psychostimulants were observed in those with an older age, male gender, and a longer duration of treatment. Psychostimulants also seemed to have tolerable side effect profiles for most preschool children. However, given the risks of potentially severe adverse effects and limited quality of the current evidence, more research is warranted to support their use in this age group.
Supplementary Materials
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2023.11
Data Availability Statement
Statement Data that supports the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: H-J.C., C-K.S., Y-S.C., F-L.L., Y-C.C.; Data curation: C-K.S., Y-S.C., Y-C.C.; Methodology: H-J.C., M-Y.W., Y-C.C., W.C.; Supervision: R-F.T., Y-C.C., W.C.; Writing—original draft: H-J.C., C-K.S., Y-S.C.; Writing—review and editing: Y-C.C., W.C.
Funding Statement
This work was not supported by any research funding.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare none.
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.