Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:19:38.589Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘Munchausen's syndrome by proxy’ or a ‘miscarriage of justice’? An initial application of functional neuroimaging to the question of guilt versus innocence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2020

Sean A. Spence*
Affiliation:
Academic Clinical Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, The Longley Centre, Norwood Grange Drive, Sheffield S5 7JT, UK
Catherine J. Kaylor-Hughes
Affiliation:
Academic Clinical Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, The Longley Centre, Norwood Grange Drive, Sheffield S5 7JT, UK
Martin L. Brook
Affiliation:
Academic Clinical Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, The Longley Centre, Norwood Grange Drive, Sheffield S5 7JT, UK
Sudheer T. Lankappa
Affiliation:
Academic Clinical Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, The Longley Centre, Norwood Grange Drive, Sheffield S5 7JT, UK
Iain D. Wilkinson
Affiliation:
Academic Clinical Psychiatry, University of Sheffield, The Longley Centre, Norwood Grange Drive, Sheffield S5 7JT, UK
*
Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 (0) 114 22 61519; fax: +44 (0) 114 22 61522. E-mail address: [email protected] (S.A. Spence).
Get access

Abstract

‘Munchausen's syndrome by proxy’ characteristically describes women alleged to have fabricated or induced illnesses in children under their care, purportedly to attract attention. Where conclusive evidence exists the condition's aetiology remains speculative, where such evidence is lacking diagnosis hinges upon denial of wrong-doing (conduct also compatible with innocence). How might investigators obtain objective evidence of guilt or innocence? Here, we examine the case of a woman convicted of poisoning a child. She served a prison sentence but continues to profess her innocence. Using a modified fMRI protocol (previously published in 2001) we scanned the subject while she affirmed her account of events and that of her accusers. We hypothesized that she would exhibit longer response times in association with greater activation of ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices when endorsing those statements she believed to be false (i.e., when she ‘lied’). The subject was scanned 4 times at 3 Tesla. Results revealed significantly longer response times and relatively greater activation of ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices when she endorsed her accusers' version of events. Hence, while we have not ‘proven’ that this subject is innocent, we demonstrate that her behavioural and functional anatomical parameters behave as if she were.

Type
Original article
Copyright
Copyright © European Psychiatric Association 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abe, N.Suzuki, M.Tsukiura, T.Mori, E.Yamaguchi, K.Itoh, M.et al.Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception. Cerebral Cortex 2006;16:192199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asher, R.Munchausen's syndrome. Lancet 1951;1:339341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bass, C.Jones, D.P.H.Fabricated or induced illness. Psychiatry 2006;5(2):6065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davatzikos, C.Ruparel, K.Fan, Y.Shen, D.G.Acharyya, M.Loughead, J.W.et al.Classifying spatial patterns of brain activity with machine learning methods: application to lie detection. Neuroimage 2005;28:663668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
A lying matter. Science 2005;310:227 EditorialGoogle Scholar
Fingarette, H.Self-deception 2nd edn.Berkeley: University of California Press; 2000. (originally published 1969)Google Scholar
Friston, K.J.Holmes, A.P.Worsley, K.J.Poline, J.-B.Frith, C.D.Frackowiak, R.S.J.Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping 1995;2:189210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamer, M.Bauermann, T.Stoeter, P.Vossel, G.Covariations among fMRI, skin conductance and behavioural data during processing of concealed information. Human Brain Mapping 2007 Feb 8; [Epub ahead of print]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganis, G.Kosslyn, S.M.Stose, S.Thompson, W.L.Yurgelun-Todd, D.A.Neural correlates of different types of deception: an fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex 2003;13:830836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ganser, S.J.M.A peculiar hysterical state. Hirsch, S.R.Shepherd, M.Themes and variations in European psychiatry. Virginia: University Press of Virginia; 1897. p. 6777 1974, first published 1897.Google Scholar
Hughes, C.J.Farrow, T.F.D.Hopwood, M.-C.Pratt, A.Hunter, M.D.Spence, S.A.Recent developments in deception research. Current Psychiatry Reviews 2005;1:271279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozel, F.A.Johnson, K.A.Mu, Q.Grenesko, E.L.Laken, S.J.George, M.S.Detecting deception using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry 2005;58:605613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kozel, F.A.Padgett, T.M.George, M.S.A replication study of the neural correlates of deception. Behavioral Neuroscience 2004;118:852856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozel, F.A.Revell, L.J.Lorberbaum, J.P.Shastri, A.Elhai, J.D.Horner, M.D.et al.A pilot study of functional magnetic resonance imaging brain correlates of deception in healthy young men. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 2004;16:295305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langleben, D.D.Loughead, J.W.Bilker, W.B.Ruparel, K.Childress, A.R.Busch, S.I.et al.Telling truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event-related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping 2005;26:262272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langleben, D.D.Schroeder, L.Maldjian, J.A.Gur, R.C.McDonald, S.Ragland, J.D.et al.Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage 2002;15:727732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T.M.C.Liu, H.-L.Chan, C.C.H.Ng, Y.-B.Fox, P.T.Gao, J.-H.Neural correlates of feigned memory impairment. Neuroimage 2005;28:305313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T.M.C.Liu, H.-L.Tan, L.-H.Chan, C.C.H.Mahankali, S.Feng, C.-M.et al.Lie detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping 2002;15:157164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meadow, R.Munchausen syndrome by proxy: the hinterland of child abuse. Lancet 1977;2:343345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meadow, R.Munchausen syndrome by proxy. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1982;57:9298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mele, A.R.Real self-deception. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1997;20:91136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H.E.O'Connell, A.Dementia: the estimation of pre-morbid intelligence levels using the national adult reading test. Cortex 1978;14:234244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunez, J.M.Casey, B.J.Egner, T.Hare, T.Hirsch, J.Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control. Neuroimage 2005;25:267277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pankratz, L.Persistent problems with the Munchausen syndrome by proxy label. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law 2006;34:9095.Google ScholarPubMed
Pearson, H.Lure of lie detectors spooks ethicists. Nature 2006;441:918919.Google Scholar
Phan, K.L.Magalhaes, A.Ziemlewicz, T.J.Fitzgerald, D.A.Green, C.Smith, W.Neural correlates of telling lies: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study at 4 Tesla. Academic Radiology 2005;12:164172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R.Diagnostic, explanatory, and detection models of Munchausen by proxy: extrapolation from malingering and deception. Child Abuse & Neglect 2004;28:225239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanoo, M.Abductor sign: a reliable new sign to detect unilateral non-organic paresis of the lower limb. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 2004;75:121125.Google Scholar
Spence, S.A.Hysterical paralyses as disorders of action. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1999;4:203226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S.A.The deceptive brain. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 2004;97:69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence SA. Playing devil's advocate: the case against fMRI lie detection. Legal and Criminological Psychology, in press.Google Scholar
Spence, S.A.Farrow, T.F.D.Herford, A.E.Wilkinson, I.D.Zheng, Y.Woodruff, P.W.Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans. Neuroreport 2001;12:28492853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S.Farrow, T.Leung, D.Shah, S.Reilly, B.Rahman, A.et al.Lying as an executive function. Halligan, P.W.Bass, C.Oakley, D.A.Malingering and illness deception Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. p. 255266.Google Scholar
Spence, S.A.Hunter, M.D.Farrow, T.F.D.Green, R.D.Leung, D.H.Hughes, C.J.et al.A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 2005;359:17551762.Google Scholar
Talairach, P.Tournoux, J.A.A stereotactic co-planar atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart: Thieme; 1988.Google Scholar
Trivers, R.The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2000;907:114131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrij, A.Detecting lies and deceit: the psychology of lying and the implications for professional practice. Chichester: Wiley; 2000.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.