Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T00:58:38.606Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fiona Coward, Robert Hosfield, Matt Pope and Francis Wenban Smith, eds. Settlement, Society and Cognition: Landscapes in Mind (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2015, 414pp., 93 figs., 25 tables, hbk, ISBN 978-1-107-02688-9)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 January 2017

Thomas Wynn*
Affiliation:
Center for Cognitive Archaeology, University of Colorado, USA

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Reviews
Copyright
Copyright © European Association of Archaeologists 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ambrose, S. 2010. Coevolution of Composite-tool Technology, Constructive Memory, and Language: Implications for the Evolution of Modern Human Behavior. In: Wynn, T. & Coolidge, F.L., guest eds. Working Memory: Beyond Language and Symbolism. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Current Anthropology, 51, Supplement 1 (Wenner-Gren Symposium Series), pp. S135–47.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. 2007. Working Memory, Thought, and Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Deetz, J. 1968. Hunters in Archaeological Perspective. In: Lee, R. & DeVore, I., eds. Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 281–85.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R., Gamble, C. & Gowlett, J. eds. 2014. Lucy to Language: The Benchmark Papers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. 1986. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gobet, F. 1998. Expert Memory: A Comparison of Four Theories. Cognition, 66: 115–52.Google Scholar
Gowlett, J. 2006. The Elements of Design Form in Acheulian Bifaces: Modes, Modalities, Rules and Language. In: Goren-Inbar, N. & Sharon, G., eds. Axe Age: Acheulian Toolmaking from Quarry to Discard. London: Equinox, pp. 203–22.Google Scholar
Haidle, M. 2009. How to Think a Simple Spear. In: de Baune, S.A., Coolidge, F.L. & Wynn, T., eds. Cognitive Archaeology and Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 5774.Google Scholar
Hecht, E., Gutman, D., Khreisheh, N., Taylor, S.V., Kilner, J., Faisal, A.A., Bradley, B.A., Chaminade, T. & Stout, D. 2014. Acquisition of Paleolithic Toolmaking Abilities Involves Structural Remodeling to Inferior Fronto-parietal Regions. Brain Structure and Function, 220(4):2315–31.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. 2000. The Perception of the Environment: Essays on Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. 2013. How Things Shape the Mind: A Theory of Material Engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, M.A. & Parfitt, S.A. 1999. Boxgrove: A Middle Pleistocene Hominid Site at Eartham Quarry, Boxgrove, West Sussex. London: English Heritage.Google Scholar
Roe, D. 1968. British Lower and Middle Palaeolithic Handaxe Groups. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 34:182.Google Scholar
Villa, P. & Roebroeks, W. 2014. Neanderthal Demise: An Archaeological Analysis of the Modern Human Superiority Complex. PLoS ONE, 9 (4):e96424.Google Scholar
Wynn, T. & Coolidge, F.L., guest eds. 2010. Working Memory: Beyond Language and Symbolism. Current Anthropology, 51, Supplement 1. Chicago: Chicago University Press (Wenner-Gren Symposium Series).Google Scholar