Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T18:58:25.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neuroimaging studies exploring the neural basis of social isolation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 April 2021

Niccolò Zovetti
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Psychiatry, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
Maria Gloria Rossetti
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Psychiatry, University of Verona, Verona, Italy Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
Cinzia Perlini
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
Paolo Brambilla*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy Department of Pathophysiology and Transplantation, University of Milan, Milan, Italy
Marcella Bellani
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, Section of Psychiatry, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
*
Author for correspondence: Paolo Brambilla, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

According to the social brain hypothesis, the human brain includes a network designed for the processing of social information. This network includes several brain regions that elaborate social cues, interactions and contexts, i.e. prefrontal paracingulate and parietal cortices, amygdala, temporal lobes and the posterior superior temporal sulcus. While current literature suggests the importance of this network from both a psychological and evolutionary perspective, little is known about its neurobiological bases. Specifically, only a paucity of studies explored the neural underpinnings of constructs that are ascribed to the social brain network functioning, i.e. objective social isolation and perceived loneliness. As such, this review aimed to overview neuroimaging studies that investigated social isolation in healthy subjects. Social isolation correlated with both structural and functional alterations within the social brain network and in other regions that seem to support mentalising and social processes (i.e. hippocampus, insula, ventral striatum and cerebellum). However, results are mixed possibly due to the heterogeneity of methods and study design. Future neuroimaging studies with longitudinal designs are needed to measure the effect of social isolation in experimental v. control groups and to explore its relationship with perceived loneliness, ultimately helping to clarify the neural correlates of the social brain.

Type
Epidemiology for Behavioural Neurosciences
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re- use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

The human nature is thought to be rooted in its social interactions and relationships, which play a crucial role in survival and reproduction and support the development and preservation of physical and mental health (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Capitanio and Cacioppo2014). In humans, extreme cases of social isolation can lead to the complete avoidance of social contexts including work, education and those involving significant others such as friends or relatives. This condition is known as Hikikomori syndrome or complete withdrawal, a phenomenon that in 2013 was estimated to affect at least one million Japanese (Saito and Angles, Reference Saito and Angles2013). The evidence to date shows that this phenomenon is growing in both eastern and western countries, possibly influenced by cultural and environmental factors such as parental style and urbanicity (Tateno et al., Reference Tateno, Park, Kato, Umene-Nakano and Saito2012) with severe consequences for the national health systems (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, Reference Cacioppo and Cacioppo2018).

Specifically, social isolation (and related loneliness) has been associated with increased mortality risk and depressive symptomatology, poorer cognitive performance and executive functioning, faster cognitive decline, increased threat sensitivity and alterations in the cardiovascular and neuroendocrine systems in healthy individuals (Cacioppo and Hawkley, Reference Cacioppo and Hawkley2009; Bhatti and ul Haq, Reference Bhatti and ul Haq2017).

Conversely, abundant social relationships and interactions were shown to exert a protective effect against dementia, neurocognitive decline and mortality. More importantly, an active social life has been found to correlate with healthy behaviours such as adequate sleep, diet and exercise and discourage unhealthy behaviours such as excessive eating (Kiely et al., Reference Kiely, Simon, Jones and Morris2000; House, Reference House2001). While the study of social isolation and loneliness from a clinical perspective is important for clinicians and psychiatrists, the investigation of their neuronal correlates is also crucial as it may help to understand the neurobiological phenotype of the Hikikomori syndrome and other psychiatric disorders of which social isolation and loneliness are core symptoms (e.g. depression and schizophrenia). Therefore, we aimed to describe neurobiological correlates of social isolation by means of neuroimaging studies in healthy controls. Due to the paucity of studies measuring social isolation with quantifiable methods (i.e. confinement), we also included the studies that explored the neurobiological correlates of (self-reported) loneliness as the latter is considered a psychological correlate of objective social isolation (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, Reference Cacioppo and Cacioppo2018).

The data search was conducted through PubMed, Scopus and Web of Science databases. The following keywords were used for the search: (‘neuroimaging’ OR ‘magnetic resonance imaging’ OR ‘electroencephalography’ OR ‘Positron Emission Tomography’ OR ‘diffusion tensor imaging’) AND (‘social isolation’ OR ‘loneliness’ OR ‘hikikomori’ OR ‘complete withdrawal’). The inclusion criteria were: (i) original articles published in peer-reviewed journals between January 2000 and October 2020; (ii) English language; (iii) the inclusion of healthy participants with no psychiatric or neurologic conditions; and (iv) the application of neuroimaging technique (i.e. magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), electroencephalography (EEG), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), positron emission tomography (PET)). Preclinical studies and case-report were excluded. A flow diagram illustrating the studies selection process is presented in Fig. 1. The literature search retrieved 182 records. After title and abstract screening, 162 articles were excluded because they clearly did not meet inclusion criteria. The remaining 20 studies were included in this review after a full-text review. Sample characteristics and neuroimaging findings from each study are shown in Table 1.

Fig. 1. A flow diagram of the articles screening and selection process.

Table 1. Neuroimaging studies exploring the neurobiological correlates of objective social isolation and perceived loneliness

BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; EEG, electroencephalogram; EPQ, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; GM, grey matter; L, left; ns, not specified; PET, positron emission tomography; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PiB, Pittsburgh compound B; R, right; sMRI, structural magnetic resonance imaging; UCLA-LS, The University of California Los Angeles, Loneliness Scale; UCLA-3-LS, 3-item version UCLA-LS; vs., versus; WM, white matter.

Seventy per cent of the studies (14 out of 20) explored the neurofunctional correlates of social isolation or loneliness with either fMRI, EEG or PET, while the remaining 30% investigated the neurostructural correlates of those constructs (i.e. grey matter (GM) volumes and white matter (WM) integrity) through structural MRI and DTI.

The University of California Los Angeles Loneliness Scale (UCLA-LS) was used in 16 out of the 20 studies (76%), to measure the feeling of loneliness. The UCLA-LS is a self-report instrument and consists of 20 items measuring general loneliness and satisfaction with social relationships. Participants are instructed to indicate how often they feel the way described by each item on a Likert scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = always).

As shown in Table 1, the extent of loneliness varied across studies. Specifically, the mean UCLA score was available in 12 out of 20 studies and ranged between 23 and 69 corresponding, respectively, to a low-to-high degree of loneliness, with the mean UCLA values ranging between 40 and 44 (moderate loneliness). Other studies however did not report the loneliness scores of their samples or measured objective isolation instead (confinements, polar expeditions).

Studies investigating the association between loneliness and structural brain correlates showed mixed findings. Specifically, higher loneliness scores correlated with reduced GM volumes in the posterior superior temporal sulcus, amygdala, hippocampus/para-hippocampus and cerebellum (Kanai et al., Reference Kanai, Bahrami, Roylance and Rees2012; Düzel et al., Reference Düzel, Drewelies, Gerstorf, Demuth, Steinhagen-Thiessen, Lindenberger and Kühn2019); increased GM volumes in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Kong et al., Reference Kong, Wei, Li, Cun, Xue, Zhang and Qiu2015); and lower WM density in the bilateral inferior parietal lobule, right insula, posterior temporoparietal junction, posterior superior temporal sulcus, dorsomedial and rostro-lateral PFC (Nakagawa et al., Reference Nakagawa, Takeuchi, Taki, Nouchi, Sekiguchi, Kotozaki, Miyauchi, Iizuka, Yokoyama and Shinada2015). Moreover, increased GM cerebellar volumes were observed when comparing individuals with v. without susceptibility to loneliness (Wong et al., Reference Wong, Shao, Wu, Tao, Chen and Lee2019). Lastly, Stahn et al. (Reference Stahn, Gunga, Kohlberg, Gallinat, Dinges and Kühn2019) conducted a longitudinal study to explore the effects of prolonged isolation on structural brain indices and found that after 14 months of isolation, the experimental group (i.e. polar expeditioners) showed lower hippocampal volumes compared to a control group matched by age, sex and baseline hippocampal volume (Stahn et al., Reference Stahn, Gunga, Kohlberg, Gallinat, Dinges and Kühn2019).

Studies investigating the association between loneliness and functional brain correlates analysed the blood oxygenation level-dependant (BOLD) or EEG signals of the samples. Overall, loneliness was associated with different processing of pleasant and unpleasant social stimuli and words with affective value (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Norris, Decety, Monteleone and Nusbaum2009, Reference Cacioppo, Bangee, Balogh, Cardenas-Iniguez, Qualter and Cacioppo2016; Inagaki et al., Reference Inagaki, Muscatell, Moieni, Dutcher, Jevtic, Irwin and Eisenberger2016; Wong et al., Reference Wong, Shao, Wu, Tao, Chen and Lee2019). Specifically, Cacioppo et al. showed that, while viewing pleasant and unpleasant pictures, participants with high v. low perceived loneliness (as measured by the UCLA-LS) had reduced activations of the ventral striatum and the temporoparietal junction and greater activation of the visual cortex (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Norris, Decety, Monteleone and Nusbaum2009). The same authors found also that images depicting social threat stimuli were differentiated more quickly by individuals with higher v. lower loneliness scores and were associated with greater activations in the PFC, visual cortex, temporal gyrus, hippocampus and supramarginal gyrus, only in lonely individuals (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Bangee, Balogh, Cardenas-Iniguez, Qualter and Cacioppo2016). Similarly, Wong et al. (Reference Wong, Shao, Wu, Tao, Chen and Lee2019) observed that loneliness level was positively associated with the right posterior cerebellar functional connectivity with the visual and premotor cortices when processing words with positive v. neutral affective value (Wong et al., Reference Wong, Shao, Wu, Tao, Chen and Lee2019). Lastly, Powers et al. (Reference Powers, Wagner, Norris and Heatherton2013) manipulated experimentally the feeling of social exclusion perceived by the participants by asking them to complete a personality questionnaire and providing them with false feedback randomly assigned. Then, participants underwent an fMRI exam while viewing pictures with affective value. Participants in which social exclusion was experimentally induced showed lower PFC activations when viewing negative social scenes (Powers et al., Reference Powers, Wagner, Norris and Heatherton2013). Conversely, D'Agostino et al. (Reference D'Agostino, Kattan and Canli2019) found no association between loneliness scores and brain activations during an fMRI task showing participants pictures depicting pleasant social stimuli of strangers (D'Agostino et al., Reference D'Agostino, Kattan and Canli2019). This finding contrasted with previous studies reporting decreased ventral striatum activity as a function of loneliness during a similar task (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Norris, Decety, Monteleone and Nusbaum2009). The authors suggested that discrepancies with previous studies and the absence of any association between loneliness and different processing of social cues might have been due to differences in sample size and stimuli presented.

Other studies examined the BOLD activity of the brain at rest through resting-state functional MRI and showed that social isolation and perceived loneliness were associated with increased activity in the right central operculum, right supramarginal gyrus and between default, frontoparietal, attention and perceptual networks (Layden et al., Reference Layden, Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Cappa, Dodich, Falini and Canessa2017; Mwilambwe-Tshilobo et al., Reference Mwilambwe-Tshilobo, Ge, Chong, Ferguson, Misic, Burrow, Leahy and Spreng2019). Two studies found that higher loneliness scores were associated with (i) decreased blood flow from the dorsal to the ventral attentional network, (ii) decreased flow from the affective to the visual network and (iii) low-frequency fluctuations of the activity in the inferior temporal gyrus (Tian et al., Reference Tian, Yang, Chen, Guo, Ding, Tam and Yao2017; Yi et al., Reference Yi, Li, Xiao, Ma, Fan and Dai2018). In line with these results, Feng et al. (Reference Feng, Wang, Li and Xu2019) applied a machine learning discrimination algorithm to the resting-state activations of healthy participants and showed that individual loneliness scores could be predicted by the connectivity of the prefrontal, limbic and temporal networks (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Wang, Li and Xu2019). Specifically, key nodes contributing to the discrimination process were in the PFC, lateral orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), caudate, amygdala and temporal regions.

Finally, prolonged objective isolation (i.e. confinement) was associated with decreased global and parietal cortical activity when measured through EEG (Jacubowski et al., Reference Jacubowski, Abeln, Vogt, Yi, Choukèr, Fomina, Strüder and Schneider2015; Weber et al., Reference Weber, Javelle, Klein, Foitschik, Crucian, Schneider and Abeln2019). Specifically, Jacubowski et al. (Reference Jacubowski, Abeln, Vogt, Yi, Choukèr, Fomina, Strüder and Schneider2015) evaluated the impact of the stress caused by isolation in a group of subjects that lived in confinement for 520 days. While isolated, participants were instructed to engage in physical training for 30 min/day. EEG data were collected before and after each exercise session every 2 weeks, cortisol was collected every 2 months. Results indicated decreased global cortical activity (i.e. α and β activity) and increased salivary cortisol level throughout the isolation period, thus indicating a potential stressful effect of isolation on the brain (Jacubowski et al., Reference Jacubowski, Abeln, Vogt, Yi, Choukèr, Fomina, Strüder and Schneider2015).

Lastly, only two studies investigated the neurobiological correlates of loneliness through PET imaging and found that perceived loneliness was associated with increased amyloid and tau proteins, aggregates commonly associated with the development of several neurocognitive diseases (Donovan et al., Reference Donovan, Okereke, Vannini, Amariglio, Rentz, Marshall, Johnson and Sperling2016; Uquillas et al., Reference Uquillas, Jacobs, Biddle, Properzi, Hanseeuw, Schultz, Rentz, Johnson, Sperling and Donovan2018).

To summarise, current literature indicates that perceived loneliness and objective social isolation were associated with structural and functional changes in several brain regions including prefrontal, temporal and parietal cortices, limbic structures (i.e. hippocampus, amygdala, insula), the striatum and the cerebellum. Part of these brain regions belongs to the social brain network, a complex neural circuit that elaborates social cues, interactions and contexts (Dunbar, Reference Dunbar2009). Specifically, the network encompasses the OFC, the medial PFC, the para-cingulate cortices, the amygdala, the temporal lobes and the posterior superior temporal sulcus (Frith, Reference Frith2007; Adolphs, Reference Adolphs2009).

The OFC and medial PFC are known to be involved in the monitoring of social behaviours and mentalising processes (Beer et al., Reference Beer, John, Scabini and Knight2006). For instance, patients with structural OFC alterations show a poor control of emotions and decision-making processes (Bellani and Brambilla, Reference Bellani and Brambilla2008). Similarly, the medial PFC seems to be involved in the understanding and decoding of others' behaviours in terms of mental states and social interactions (Iacoboni et al., Reference Iacoboni, Lieberman, Knowlton, Molnar-Szakacs, Moritz, Throop and Fiske2004). Also, the anterior cingulate cortex has been suggested to play a role in the processing of social information (Apps et al., Reference Apps, Rushworth and Chang2016). The role of the amygdala in social cognition is well known as its activity has been widely associated with the processing of emotions, prejudice, social judgements and behaviours (Adolphs and Spezio, Reference Adolphs and Spezio2006). Lastly, temporal lobes and the posterior superior temporal sulcus have been extensively studied in the context of the social brain as their alterations are frequently associated with autism spectrum disorders, a condition in which social cognition is severely compromised (Saitovitch et al., Reference Saitovitch, Bargiacchi, Chabane, Brunelle, Samson, Boddaert and Zilbovicius2012). Particularly, the posterior superior temporal sulcus and the temporoparietal junction have been associated with the detection and processing of eye movements (Pelphrey et al., Reference Pelphrey, Morris, Michelich, Allison and McCarthy2005).

Overall, our findings suggest that objective social isolation and perceived loneliness are associated with morpho-functional changes that extend both in and out of the social brain network (i.e. PFC, temporal sulcus, amygdala and temporal cortex, parietal cortex and hippocampus) (Fig. 2). Even though some of the brain regions associated with social isolation and perceived loneliness (e.g. parietal lobule and cortex, hippocampus, cerebellum) were not included in the early theorisation of the human social brain, recent evidence suggests that they also play a role in social cognition (Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro, Reference Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro2008; Laurita and Spreng, Reference Laurita and Spreng2017). For example, the parietal cortex is home to the mirror neurons system, a complex network allowing primates and humans to understand the intention behind an observed motor act (Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro, Reference Rizzolatti and Fabbri-Destro2008) while the hippocampus is not only involved in recalling and storing memories, but also in social navigation, namely the formation of dynamical social relationships including social distances, hierarchies and social bonds (Laurita and Spreng, Reference Laurita and Spreng2017). Lastly, the cerebellum has been suggested to provide a supportive role in social cognition and in higher abstraction mentalising processes (Van Overwalle et al., Reference Van Overwalle, Baetens, Mariën and Vandekerckhove2014). As such, it could be speculated that social cognition (and related disorders) relies on a diffused brain circuit, broader than the social brain network, in which different regions play a central or supplementary role (Bellani and Brambilla, Reference Bellani and Brambilla2008; Crippa et al., Reference Crippa, Vecchio, Ceccarelli, Nobile, Arrigoni and Brambilla2016). However, results are still sparse and need replication.

Fig. 2. Overview of the brain regions more consistently found associated with social isolation and loneliness. Only results that were reported by two or more studies are shown.

A number of limitations should be considered when interpreting the results. First, 85% of the studies were cross-sectional and measured the correlation between self-reported loneliness and brain indices while only three studies had a longitudinal design and could measure the effect of objective isolation on the brain. Moreover, the magnitude and the precision of the observed effects remain ambiguous as none of the studies reported the effect size or the confidence intervals of the results. This, in turn, made it difficult to compare the studies with each other, precluding the generalisability of the findings. Therefore, this review highlights the need to conduct future neuroimaging studies by using longitudinal designs and more quantifiable measures of social isolation (e.g. confinement). This could help clarifying the neurobiological signature of the Hikikomori syndrome and other psychiatric disorders of which social isolation and loneliness are core symptoms (e.g. depression and schizophrenia) and, ultimately, inform future diagnostic system and tailored treatments.

Data

The data that support the findings of this study (search query) are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial support

MGR and PB were partially supported by grants from the Ministry of Health (RF-2016-02364582).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Adolphs, R (2009) The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology 60, 693716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R and Spezio, M (2006) Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. Progress in Brain Research 156, 363378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apps, MAJ, Rushworth, MFS and Chang, SWC (2016) The anterior cingulate gyrus and social cognition: tracking the motivation of others. Neuron 90, 692707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, JS, John, OP, Scabini, D and Knight, RT (2006) Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 871879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellani, M and Brambilla, P (2008) Social cognition, schizophrenia and brain imaging. Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale 17, 117119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhatti, AB and ul Haq, A (2017) The pathophysiology of perceived social isolation: effects on health and mortality. Cureus 9, e994.Google ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT and Cacioppo, S (2018) The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet 391, 426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT and Hawkley, LC (2009) Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, 447454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, JT, Norris, CJ, Decety, J, Monteleone, G and Nusbaum, H (2009) In the eye of the beholder: individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21, 8392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, S, Capitanio, JP and Cacioppo, JT (2014) Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychological Bulletin 140, 1464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, S, Bangee, M, Balogh, S, Cardenas-Iniguez, C, Qualter, P and Cacioppo, JT (2016) Loneliness and implicit attention to social threat: a high-performance electrical neuroimaging study. Cognitive Neuroscience 7, 138159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crippa, A, Vecchio, Del, Ceccarelli, Busti, Nobile, S, Arrigoni, M and Brambilla, F (2016) Cortico-Cerebellar Connectivity in Autism Spectrum Disorder: What Do We Know So Far? Front Psychiatry 7, 20Google Scholar
D'Agostino, AE, Kattan, D and Canli, T (2019) An fMRI study of loneliness in younger and older adults. Social Neuroscience 14, 136148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donovan, NJ, Okereke, OI, Vannini, P, Amariglio, RE, Rentz, DM, Marshall, GA, Johnson, KA and Sperling, RA (2016) Association of higher cortical amyloid burden with loneliness in cognitively normal older adults. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 12301237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, RIM (2009) The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution. Annals of Human Biology 36, 562572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Düzel, S, Drewelies, J, Gerstorf, D, Demuth, I, Steinhagen-Thiessen, E, Lindenberger, U and Kühn, S (2019) Structural brain correlates of loneliness among older adults. Scientific Reports 9, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, C, Wang, L, Li, T and Xu, P (2019) Connectome-based individualized prediction of loneliness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 14, 353365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, CD (2007) The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 362, 671678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
House, JS (2001) Social isolation kills, but how and why? Psychosomatic Medicine 63, 273274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, M, Lieberman, MD, Knowlton, BJ, Molnar-Szakacs, I, Moritz, M, Throop, CJ and Fiske, AP (2004) Watching social interactions produces dorsomedial prefrontal and medial parietal BOLD fMRI signal increases compared to a resting baseline. Neuroimage 21, 11671173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inagaki, TK, Muscatell, KA, Moieni, M, Dutcher, JM, Jevtic, I, Irwin, MR and Eisenberger, NI (2016) Yearning for connection? Loneliness is associated with increased ventral striatum activity to close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11, 10961101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacubowski, A, Abeln, V, Vogt, T, Yi, B, Choukèr, A, Fomina, E, Strüder, HK and Schneider, S (2015) The impact of long-term confinement and exercise on central and peripheral stress markers. Physiology & Behavior 152, 106111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanai, R, Bahrami, B, Roylance, R and Rees, G (2012) Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279, 13271334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiely, DK, Simon, SE, Jones, RN and Morris, JN (2000) The protective effect of social engagement on mortality in long-term care. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 48, 13671372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kong, X, Wei, D, Li, W, Cun, L, Xue, S, Zhang, Q and Qiu, J (2015) Neuroticism and extraversion mediate the association between loneliness and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research 233, 157164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurita, AC and Spreng, RN (2017) The hippocampus and social cognition. The Hippocampus from Cells to Systems. Switzerland AG: Springer, pp. 537558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layden, EA, Cacioppo, JT, Cacioppo, S, Cappa, SF, Dodich, A, Falini, A and Canessa, N (2017) Perceived social isolation is associated with altered functional connectivity in neural networks associated with tonic alertness and executive control. Neuroimage 145, 5873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mwilambwe-Tshilobo, L, Ge, T, Chong, M, Ferguson, MA, Misic, B, Burrow, AL, Leahy, RM and Spreng, RN (2019) Loneliness and meaning in life are reflected in the intrinsic network architecture of the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 14, 423433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagawa, S, Takeuchi, H, Taki, Y, Nouchi, R, Sekiguchi, A, Kotozaki, Y, Miyauchi, CM, Iizuka, K, Yokoyama, R and Shinada, T (2015) White matter structures associated with loneliness in young adults. Scientific Reports 5, 17001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelphrey, KA, Morris, JP, Michelich, CR, Allison, T and McCarthy, G (2005) Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an fMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex 15, 18661876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, KE, Wagner, DD, Norris, CJ and Heatherton, TF (2013) Socially excluded individuals fail to recruit medial prefrontal cortex for negative social scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8, 151157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G and Fabbri-Destro, M (2008) The mirror system and its role in social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 18, 179184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saito, T and Angles, J (2013) Hikikomori: Adolescence Without End. USA: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Saitovitch, A, Bargiacchi, A, Chabane, N, Brunelle, F, Samson, Y, Boddaert, N and Zilbovicius, M (2012) Social cognition and the superior temporal sulcus: implications in autism. Revue Neurologique 168, 762770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stahn, AC, Gunga, HC, Kohlberg, E, Gallinat, J, Dinges, DF and Kühn, S (2019). Brain changes in response to long Antarctic expeditions. New England Journal of Medicine 381, 22732275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tateno, M, Park, TW, Kato, TA, Umene-Nakano, W and Saito, T (2012) Hikikomori as a possible clinical term in psychiatry: a questionnaire survey. BMC Psychiatry 12, 169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tian, Y, Yang, L, Chen, S, Guo, D, Ding, Z, Tam, KY and Yao, D (2017) Causal interactions in resting-state networks predict perceived loneliness. PLoS ONE 12, e0177443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uquillas, F, Jacobs, HIL, Biddle, KD, Properzi, M, Hanseeuw, B, Schultz, AP, Rentz, DM, Johnson, KA, Sperling, RA and Donovan, NJ (2018) Regional tau pathology and loneliness in cognitively normal older adults. Translational Psychiatry 8, 19.Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F, Baetens, K, Mariën, P and Vandekerckhove, M (2014) Social cognition and the cerebellum: a meta-analysis of over 350 fMRI studies. Neuroimage 86, 554572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, J, Javelle, F, Klein, T, Foitschik, T, Crucian, B, Schneider, S and Abeln, V (2019) Neurophysiological, neuropsychological, and cognitive effects of 30 days of isolation. Experimental Brain Research 237, 15631573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, NML, Shao, R, Wu, J, Tao, J, Chen, L and Lee, TMC (2019) Cerebellar neural markers of susceptibility to social isolation and positive affective processing. Brain Structure and Function 224, 33393351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yi, Y, Li, LMW, Xiao, Y, Ma, J, Fan, L and Dai, Z (2018) Brain activity mediates the relation between emotional but not instrumental support and trait loneliness. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 13, 9951002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. A flow diagram of the articles screening and selection process.

Figure 1

Table 1. Neuroimaging studies exploring the neurobiological correlates of objective social isolation and perceived loneliness

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Overview of the brain regions more consistently found associated with social isolation and loneliness. Only results that were reported by two or more studies are shown.