Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:54:58.979Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Blind spots in stigma research? Broadening our perspective on mental illness stigma by exploring ‘what matters most’ in modern Western societies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 March 2021

G. Schomerus*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Leipzig Medical Center, Leipzig, Germany
M. C. Angermeyer
Affiliation:
Center for Public Mental Health, Gösing am Wagram, Austria
*
Author for correspondence: G. Schomerus, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims

The theory of ‘what matters most’ (WMM) has been developed to understand differences in mental illness stigma between cultures, postulating that stigma becomes most pervasive in situations that matter most in a specific cultural context. The rise of populism in Western societies demonstrates that also within one cultural context, different values ‘matter most’ to different groups. We expand the WMM framework to explore the spectrum of stigma manifestations within Western societies, relating it to both conservative/authoritarian and liberal/modern values. From our findings, we will develop hypotheses on how further research into value orientations and stigma might address potential blind spots in stigma research.

Methods

Based on a narrative review of the literature on mental illness stigma and value orientations, we apply the WMM framework to cultural mechanisms of stigma within modern Western societies.

Results

There are several studies showing an association between traditional, authoritarian, conservative values with stronger mental illness stigma, while studies examining the stigma within liberal, modern value orientations are scarce. We hypothesise on situations where encountering a person with mental illness could threaten liberal values and thus might provoke stigma among persons with such value orientations. For example, living with a person with mental illness could be seen as consuming energy and time, thereby jeopardising ‘self-actualisation’, the modern value of realising one's own full potential. As a result, a person highly valuing self-actualisation might try to avoid contact with persons with mental illness. Instances of potential ‘liberal stigma’ also include structural stigma or self-stigma, when, e.g. changing assumptions of what is considered ‘normal’ increase perceptions of being fundamentally different when experiencing mental illness.

Conclusions

‘WMM’ appears to be a useful framework to direct research to potential blind spots within the field of stigma research. Looking at instances where liberal values conflict with dealing with a person with mental illness could provide a more comprehensive understanding of stigma experiences among persons with mental illness. However, for measuring stigma, tapping into liberal variations of mental illness stigma is methodologically challenging. Qualitative work could be the first step to elicit potential stigma experiences based on conflicts with liberal values.

Type
Special Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

‘What matters most’ (WMM) – understanding cultural differences in stigma

In their seminal article on ‘Culture and stigma: Adding moral experience to stigma theory’, Yang et al. (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007) hypothesise that ‘Stigma … threatens the loss or diminution of what is most at stake’. Building on Kleinman's (Reference Kleinman2006) concept of ‘moral experience’, the authors emphasise the role of engagements in everyday social life that revolve around the preservation of fundamental values that ‘matter most’ to ordinary people. For remaining a fully recognised member of a local cultural group it is mandatory to participate in these activities. By diminishing the capacity to do this, stigma undermines a person's standing within the local group and, thus, endangers their personhood. ‘WMM, as Yang et al. later term their theory (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Chen, Sia, Lam, Lam, Ngo, Kleinman and Good2014b), shapes both how stigma is experienced and the way stigma is enacted: For those who stigmatise, the necessity to use stigma to protect personal values or interactions might be greatest in situations that ‘matter most’. And for those stigmatised, stigma is particularly harmful by affecting the culturally most relevant interactions and situations. Yang et al. describe stigma as a ‘highly pragmatic, even tactical response to perceived threats, real dangers and fear of the unknown’ (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007, p. 1528). ‘WMM’ gives a motive to stigma: people stigmatise persons with, for example mental illness in order to protect or advance those cultural values that are most important to them.

Stigma differs between cultures, and according to Yang et al., these differences can be understood by identifying ‘WMM’ to persons within a shared cultural context, and contrasting it with ‘WMM’ in a different culture. So far, the ‘WMM’ theory has mostly informed research characterising stigma in non-Western cultures, often comparing it to Western culture or to stigma among persons of Western European descent. For example, a recent study characterising the stigma of human immunodeficiency virus among women in Botswana (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Ho-Foster, Becker, Misra, Rampa, Poku, Entaile, Goodman and Blank2021) identified themes related to being viewed as a ‘good woman’. Two representative population surveys compared the stigma towards a person with schizophrenia in Tunisia and Germany (Angermeyer et al., Reference Angermeyer, Carta, Matschinger, Millier, Refai, Schomerus and Toumi2016), finding that the desire for social distance was greater in more distant relationships in Germany, while it was greater for close, family-related relationships in Tunisia, where family loyalty and obligations take precedence over loyalty to friends or demands of a job. Another study found ‘threat to family lineage’ is particularly relevant with regard to mental illness stigma among Chinese–Americans compared to European–Americans (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Purdie-Vaughns, Kotabe, Link, Saw, Wong and Phelan2013). A systematic review using the ‘WMM’ perspective to identify culturally salient aspects of mental illness stigma found ‘cultural ideals of the everyday activities that define personhood’ to be different between Asian groups (‘preserving ones lineage’), African–American groups (‘establishing trust among religious institutions due to institutional discrimination’) and Latino-American groups (‘fighting hard to overcome problems and taking advantage of immigration opportunities’). These essential cultural interactions were found to shape culturally salient stigma manifestations (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Thornicroft, Alvarado, Vega and Link2014a).

‘WMM’ within a shared cultural context

Meaningful differences of ‘WMM’ to a group of persons, however, are not restricted to differences between cultures. Within Western societies, the rise of populism and an increasing polarisation of societies have demonstrated that quite different value orientations and cultural ideals are prevalent even within one cultural context. We argue that conceptualising these intra-cultural differences will help to broaden the scope of stigma research in a meaningful way. Applying the ‘WMM’ approach to groups that are distinguished by their values and ideals within the Western culture will probably enable identification of situations where stigmatisation of persons with mental illness occurs, that have so far not been fully acknowledged.

Several authors have pointed out that the apparent cleavages in Western societies are not primarily situated along a socio-economic axis between the rich and the poor. Instead, they see deepening cultural divisions, and a definition of ‘left’ and ‘right’ in cultural rather than economic terms. Norris and Inglehart, for example observe that traditional values of a once-dominant cultural majority have been threatened by a spread of cosmopolitan liberal orientations by growing minorities (Norris and Inglehart, Reference Norris and Inglehart2019, p. 87–88). As a result, they see a deepening rift between authoritarian and libertarian cultural orientations. Goodhart (Reference Goodhart2017) contrasts ‘somewheres’, persons that are firmly connected to a specific community and are committed to traditional values, with ‘anywheres’, persons not being rooted in local communities, and entertaining liberal values, Merkel (Reference Merkel, Harfst, Kubbe and Poguntke2017) puts communitarians against cosmopolitans. These authors all refer to a cluster of traditional, national, authoritarian and culturally conservative values as opposed to modern, liberal, cosmopolitan values as defining a growing polarisation in Western societies. To capture this polarity of value orientations, we will refer to conservative or authoritarian v. liberal, cosmopolitan or modern values throughout this paper. This cultural division becomes even more visible as adherents of both world views tend to concentrate in different locations: While people endorsing modern values preferably live in big cities (where they tend to gravitate towards trendy districts), those endorsing traditional values prevail in small towns and rural hinterlands. Apart from this spatial segregation, and this seems to become ever more important, social media facilitate the creation of virtual communities which allow people to develop a sense of connectedness and where they can communicate with others who share the same values. As real communities, virtual communities can also function as ‘local worlds’ in which people are eager to maintain their social standing by enacting ‘WMM’.

Aims of the study

We aim to determine whether different value orientations have been examined in relation to mental illness stigma. In particular, we are interested in whether stigma has been examined with regard to both liberal and conservative value orientations. Extending the ‘WMM’ theoretical framework to different value orientations within Western culture, we will develop hypotheses on how further research into value orientations and stigma might address potential blind spots of previous stigma research.

Methods

For this conceptual article, we conducted a review of the literature related to value orientations and mental illness stigma, based on both databases (Pubmed, Web of Knowledge, Google Scholar) and bibliographies of identified articles. We used both general search terms like ‘value orientations’, and specific terms like conservative, liberal, authoritarian, progressive etc. Using an iterative approach, we further extended our search to specific values and situations that emerged as being conceptually relevant when applying ‘WMM’ to conservative and liberal value orientations. Since our aim is not to provide an exhaustive account of the literature related to values and mental illness stigma, but to expand an existing theoretical framework to identify areas that have potentially been understudied, our results will be presented as a narrative (rather than systematic) review.

Results

Between the two opposing value orientations of conservative/authoritarian values and liberal/progressive values, the stigma of mental illness has primarily been examined with regard to its association with the former. Using scales like the right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) scale (Altemeyer, Reference Altemeyer1996) or the authoritarianism–conservatism–traditionalism (ACT) scale (Duckitt and Sibley, Reference Duckitt and Sibley2010), several studies found more conservative/authoritarian values to be related to mental illness stigma (Duckitt and Sibley, Reference Duckitt and Sibley2007; Kvaale and Haslam, Reference Kvaale and Haslam2016). Persons endorsing authoritarian values showed stronger endorsement of negative stereotypes (DeLuca and Yanos, Reference DeLuca and Yanos2016), and a preference for harsher punishment of a hypothetical offender with mental illness (Fodor et al., Reference Fodor, Wick, Hartsen and Preve2008). An experimental study found persons with higher RWA-scores having stronger negative attitudes towards a person with medically controlled schizophrenia at the workplace than persons with lower RWA-scores (Fodor, Reference Fodor2006). RWA has also been shown to be related to negative attitudes towards mental health services (Furr et al., Reference Furr, Usui and Hines-Martin2003).

Closely related to value orientations are political attitudes (Rempala et al., Reference Rempala, Okdie and Garvey2016), with self-reported conservatism being associated with higher RWA scores (DeLuca et al., Reference DeLuca, Vaccaro, Seda and Yanos2018), but also with adherence to tradition, resistance to change and justification of inequality (Jost et al., Reference Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski and Sulloway2003). The social dominance orientation (SDO) scale (Pratto et al., Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994) has been developed to quantify a political preference for hierarchical relations between groups, putting one's own in-group in a superior position towards seemingly inferior out-groups (Pratto et al., Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994). SDO has repeatedly been shown to be associated with more stigma towards persons with mental illness (Duckitt and Sibley, Reference Duckitt and Sibley2007; Bizer et al., Reference Bizer, Hart and Jekogian2012; Kvaale and Haslam, Reference Kvaale and Haslam2016), for example with stronger dangerousness beliefs (Lampropoulos and Apostolidis, Reference Lampropoulos and Apostolidis2018) or a stronger desire for a social distance towards persons with schizophrenia (Phelan and Basow, Reference Phelan and Basow2007).

A study in Sweden demonstrated both conservative political ideology and support for conservative political parties to be associated with stronger stigmatising attitudes towards persons with depression (Löve et al., Reference Löve, Bertisson, Matinsson, Wängnerud and Hensing2019). An online study in the United States found persons with liberal political ideology attributing less responsibility to a person with depression, showing more empathy and scoring lower on a depression stigma scale (Thibodeau et al., Reference Thibodeau, Fein, Goodbody and Flusberg2015). But not all studies found political orientation related to stigma: A study among the general public in Germany found right-wing extremism only marginally related to social distance towards persons with mental illness (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Angermeyer and Brähler2005).

Only few studies have examined associations of stigma with a broader spectrum of different value orientations. Angermeyer and Matschinger examined a large representative sample of the German public and found respondents who endorsed more traditional values having a greater desire for social distance, while those endorsing more liberal (equality, social justice, tolerance) and more ‘modern’ (self-realisation, hedonism, post-materialism) values were more accepting of persons with mental illness (Angermeyer and Matschinger, Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger1997). Similarly, using the Schwartz Value Inventory (1992), Norman et al. found greater self-transcendence being associated with a lower desire for social distance from a person with mental illness, while self-enhancement or conservatism values were related to more social distance (Norman et al., Reference Norman, Sorrentino, Windell and Manchanda2008). Using the same value inventory, Skinner et al. (Reference Skinner, Feather, Freeman and Roche2007) found nurses with high self-transcendence values expressing more, and with high conservatism values expressing less positive affective responses towards patients with stigmatised conditions like drug-use. Traditional values have also been shown to be associated with more negative attitudes towards persons with alcohol use disorders, for example regarding resource allocation within health care services (Schomerus et al., Reference Schomerus, Matschinger and Angermeyer2006).

Previous studies thus paint a fairly consistent picture: traditional, authoritarian, conservative values are associated with more, and liberal/modern values are associated with less stigma. Some authors even propose a general prejudice factor, encompassing negative attitudes towards several minorities, being related to traditional, conservative, authoritarian values (Duckitt and Sibley, Reference Duckitt and Sibley2007).

Traditional values and stigma

Looking at specific traditional values, the stigma literature is rich in findings suggestive of a close relationship with mental illness stigma. These inter-relations seem clearly shaped by ‘WMM’ for people holding these values. According to the motivated social cognition perspective, the endorsement of conservative values is in part determined by the way people perceive their social world (Jost et al., Reference Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski and Sulloway2003). Persons who perceive the world to be threatening and dangerous are more inclined to espouse conservative ideologies (e.g. Van Leeuwen and Park, Reference Van Leeuwen and Park2009, Shook et al., Reference Shook, Ford and Boggs2017). A view of the world as a dangerous place fosters the desire for security. Frequently endorsed stereotypes for schizophrenia include being dangerous and unpredictable (Pescosolido et al., Reference Pescosolido, Manago and Monahan2019), and fear has been shown to be the emotion central to the stigma of schizophrenia (Angermeyer et al., Reference Angermeyer, Holzinger and Matschinger2010). The desire for security is thus threatened by these perceived attributes of someone with schizophrenia. Perceptions of dangerousness are accompanied by a strong perceived need for coercive treatment for this group of patients (Pescosolido et al., Reference Pescosolido, Manago and Monahan2019). In fact, Watson et al. could show that conservatives are more likely to attribute bad character to a person with mental illness, which in turn was associated with perceptions of dangerousness and stronger support of legal coercion (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Corrigan and Angell2005). Similar to fear, the stereotype of unpredictability (as well as that of lack of self-control) may also reflect a perceived threat to order and structure, which have been shown to be related to political conservatism, directly (Webster and Stewart, Reference Webster and Stewart1973) and indirectly through the endorsement of right-wing authoritarianism (Altemeyer, Reference Altemeyer1998; Peterson and Lane, Reference Peterson and Lane2001).

Another example is the belief that the world is a generally just place, where all people normally get what they deserve (Lerner, Reference Lerner1980), a belief which shows strong links to conservatism (Furnham and Procter, Reference Furnham and Procter1989). In consequence of the view that good things happen to good people and bad things to bad ones, sick people are at risk of being judged as responsible for their illness. In fact, just world beliefs tended to be associated with higher attributions of blame to persons with mental illness in a general population sample (Rüsch et al., Reference Rüsch, Todd, Bodenhausen and Corrigan2010), supporting the hypothesis that stigma serves as a strategy to protect one's personal values: if people with mental illness are responsible for their illness, there is no need to question one's belief in a just world.

As a third example, values rooted in Protestant theology (Luther, Calvin) might increase mental illness stigma. Protestant ethic, a term coined by the German sociologist Max Weber (Reference Weber1904/1905), emphasises the importance of hard work and self-discipline for success in life. According to this doctrine, those who are successful deserve it because they have worked hard and are morally superior, whereas those who lack success deserve it because they are self-indulgent and morally flawed. Beliefs in Protestant ethic, which is related to higher levels of conservatism and authoritarianism (Feather, Reference Feather1984), have been found to be associated with more perceived dangerousness, more negative stereotypes and more implicit guilt attributed to a person with mental illness (Rüsch et al., Reference Rüsch, Todd, Bodenhausen and Corrigan2010).

Hence, it seems plausible that a conservative mindset, if it is rooted in values like those described, is associated with more stigma towards persons with mental illness.

A blind spot in stigma research?

From the perspective of most academic scholars (including the authors), the finding that stigma seems to be a phenomenon associated with conservative values is reassuring, since it locates stigma within a group of ‘others’, those with traditional, conservative and authoritarian values, and not within the realm of liberalism, tolerance and cosmopolitanism (Inbar and Lammers, Reference Inbar and Lammers2012). However, when taking a ‘WMM’ perspective, looking at persons with different value orientations within Western culture reveals a potential blind spot in previous, value related stigma research. If stigma impairs the most meaningful interactions in a certain cultural context, and is most apparent in situations that affect the individuals' most important values, the question arises whether stigma in situations and values relevant to persons with liberal values has been examined with similar rigour. We argue that there is a lack of stigma research capturing situations that are important to persons with liberal value orientations, and that hence potential discrimination of persons with mental illness in such situations might have been overlooked.

Potential manifestations of stigma within a liberal cosmopolitan culture

The assumption that liberal values may foster stigma might, at first glance, appear counterintuitive. In fact, typical liberal values like inclusion of marginalised groups or valuing diversity are more likely to work against stigma than to encourage it. However, there are some liberal values which may have the opposite effect. In the following, we discuss some examples of how liberal values may intensify various forms of stigma (public stigma, structural stigma, self-stigma).

First, liberal values could decrease tolerance in situations that so far have not been evaluated as part of traditional stigma measures. A ‘modern’ value is striving for self-actualisation (Goldstein, Reference Goldstein1940), or to realise one's own full potential. If living or dealing with someone with mental illness is perceived as an impediment to one's personal choices, or as consuming energy and time that would be needed to achieve self-actualisation, a person might try to avoid contact with persons with mental illness. Related, the value of hedonism might imply that ‘negative’ contacts should be avoided in order to increase one's personal happiness. People with mental illness could be regarded as a threat to one's personal happiness and individual wellbeing. As a result, people who highly value positivity might avoid closer contact with persons with mental illness. Or, if they have a relative or friend with mental illness, they might prefer professional mental health care over informal care within a family or group of friends.

Another example is mobility , which is a central value for ‘anywheres’, members of the urban middle class. Leaving one's parents' city and moving to new, attractive places in order to get an education and to take on new job opportunities is a central part of a successful cosmopolitan biography (Reckwitz, Reference Reckwitz2019, p. 92). Being close to someone with mental illness, however, might conflict with these personal goals. Presumably, persons who highly value mobility might endorse the stereotype of persons with mental illness being dependent on others, and fear that a close relationship to a person with mental illness might impede their own mobility. As a consequence, they could be more reluctant to engage in closer relationships with someone with mental illness.

There are other modern values or virtues like creativity, self-reliance, independence, all potentially being threatened by mental illness or by living with a person with mental illness, and potentially causing social withdrawal. The poet with lived experience Bill McKnight has concisely captured this motive for discrimination in his poem ‘Comfort zone’: ‘I don't want/To get involved with you – /You who are weak and upset. / Because you might upset me.’ (McKnight, Reference McKnight2012).

An area where a high emphasis on achievement or self-actualisation could foster separation is schooling. If providing one's children with optimal, competitive starting conditions or providing them with a space of undisturbed self-actualisation, are of great personal value, dealing with potentially difficult, challenging classmates with mental health problems threatens this value. In Germany, in spite of a generally well-functioning public school system, private schooling is on the rise, predominantly driven by school choices of middle-class parents who seek the best learning environment for their offspring (Koppetsch, Reference Koppetsch2019). Avoidance of classmates with difficulties due to their social backgrounds, or, often related, due to mental health problems could be a hidden motive here.

Second, liberal values could lead to structural discrimination of persons with mental illness. For instance, a consequence of the aforementioned trend towards segregation in schooling could be that young person with mental illness will be disadvantaged as regards access to higher education and their professional prospects. Another well-documented example is the not-in-my-backyard (NIMBY) syndrome (Wexler, Reference Wexler MN1996). Here, personal prestige and economic success are highly esteemed, so housing for persons with mental illness in one's neighbourhood is perceived as threatening the prestige and material value of one's property. A study in Germany found men with higher education being more strongly opposed to implementing community psychiatry services in their neighbourhood (Angermeyer and Matschinger, Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger1991). Of course, valuing a property is not restricted to persons with liberal values and does not even constitute a strictly liberal value. Rather, it seems to ‘matter more’ also to liberal residents than the tolerance that might on other occasions be stated as an important value to them. The NIMBY syndrome amounts to structural discrimination of persons in need of supported housing because they end up in less-privileged neighbourhoods with all their negative mental health consequences.

Third, modern values could increase both public stigma and self-stigma by changing assumptions of what is considered to be normal. If being flexible, productive, positively minded is the norm, being normal implies an even greater difference to having a mental illness. Baer et al. demonstrated in a qualitative discourse analysis using both interviews with people with mental illness and media reports, that the media carry an evolving norm of happy individuals characterised by being energised, motivated and powerful (Baer et al., Reference Baer, Sikorski, Luppa, Riedel-Heller and Schomerus2016). This modern, individualistic norm puts persons with a mental illness like depression in even stronger opposition to being normal, thus increasing shame and self-stigma (Hahm et al., Reference Hahm, Speerforck, Fleischer, Grabe, Beutel and Schomerus2020; Rechenberg et al., Reference Rechenberg, Nowikow and Schomerus2020), but also increasing public notions of otherness, and potentially devaluation and discrimination. This would be an indirect, but powerful way of how modern liberal values could increase stigma.

Conclusions

Examining what matters most within Western culture and its relation to mental illness stigma

There is clear evidence that traditional, conservative values are associated with mental illness stigma in many forms, from negative stereotypes, stronger negative emotional reactions to stronger discrimination of persons with mental illness. However, the ‘WMM’ approach offers the opportunity to look for potential blind spots in stigma research, namely the question of whether there are instances where liberal values, too, could lead to stigmatisation of persons with mental illness. The challenge lies in measuring facets of the stigma associated with liberal values.

One way to look at ‘WMM’ with regard to mental illness stigma would be to create measures that tap on situations that are sensitive also to the fears of liberal respondents, where liberals may perceive their values to be in jeopardy through persons with mental illness. There are experimental studies in social psychology showing that liberal respondents, for example show levels of intolerance similar to conservative respondents, if both are asked about curtailing the rights of groups that are ideologically dissimilar to them (Brandt et al., Reference Brandt, Reyna, Chambers, Crawford and Wetherell2014), like environmentalists to conservative people or anti-abortionists to liberal people. When intolerance towards persons with mental illness would be the focus of such studies, operationalising and varying situations where closer contact with a person with mental illness would threaten either liberal or conservative values would be necessary.

However, as we have discussed with regard to the NIMBY-syndrome, there might be particular difficulties with eliciting relevant attitudes from educated, liberal respondents, since attitudes stated in an interview might differ from attitudes and behaviour in real-world situations. Using the ‘WMM’ perspective, one could even argue that in an interview situation, self-affirmation as being tolerant and liberal could be what matters most – while in real-life situations other values like advancing the prospects of one's children could be more important. Competing values might also simultaneously influence how we act in certain situations: the desire to achieve self-actualisation might compete with the desire to support a close person with mental illness, leading to mixed outcomes like, for example ‘reluctant help’ or other subtle forms of stigma that are difficult to measure, but nevertheless consequential for someone with mental illness. Measuring liberal variations of mental illness stigma thus remains a challenge. Implicit measures as well as experimental designs allowing for gradual behavioural responses could yield novel insights. But probably, starting with qualitative research eliciting when and how people experience being stigmatised in a context determined by liberal value orientations should be a first step that may provide a way to more fully understand the impact of stigma on persons with mental illness in western societies.

Implications for anti-stigma interventions

Most anti-stigma campaigns related to mental illness stigma have so far addressed the general public, or specific stakeholders, but have not differentiated between political or culturally defined subgroups of the population. However, as we have argued in this paper, in polarising societies, where a cultural divide between groups committed to traditional values seems from groups favouring liberal values is growing, these values define ‘WMM’ in the cultural context where they are most salient. If core lived values appear threatened through people with mental illness, stigma results (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Kleinman, Link, Phelan, Lee and Good2007). Anti-stigma interventions should therefore focus on what is fundamentally at stake in a given local setting. Rather than being prescribed by psychiatric experts, who run the risk of turning a blind eye to potential negative effects of their own (often liberal) values, interventions should be based on the local stigma experiences of people with mental illness – quite in line with Corrigan's demand that stigma needs to be changed locally, both in a geographical and cultural sense (Corrigan, Reference Corrigan2011). Applying ‘WMM’ might improve our understanding and conceptualisation of stigma experiences in culturally defined subgroups, and it might clarify which situations need to be addressed in order to empower people with mental illness to recover their personhood.

Data

Not applicable.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial support

This work was supported by the Fritz-Thyssen-Stiftung (Az. 10.18.2.009SO).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Altemeyer, B (1996) The Authoritarian Specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Altemeyer, B (1998) The other “authoritarian personality”. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 30, 4792. Academic Press: San Diego.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, MC and Matschinger, H (1991) Einstellung der evölkerung der BRD zu psychiatrischen Patienten und zu psychiatrischen Einrichtungen. Forum Gesundheitswesen 1, 824.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, MC and Matschinger, H (1997) Social distance towards the mentally ill: results of representative surveys in the Federal Republic of Germany. Psychological Medicine 27, 131141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, MC, Holzinger, A and Matschinger, H (2010) Emotional reactions to people with mental illness. Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale 19, 2632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, MC, Carta, MG, Matschinger, H, Millier, A, Refai, T, Schomerus, G and Toumi, M (2016) Cultural differences in stigma surrounding schizophrenia: comparison between Central Europe and North Africa. British Journal of Psychiatry 208, 389397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baer, N, Sikorski, C, Luppa, M, Riedel-Heller, SG and Schomerus, G (2016) Das Stigma Depression – eine Interaktion zwischen öffentlichem Diskurs und Erfahrungsberichten Betroffener. Psychiatrische Praxis 43, e1e8.Google Scholar
Beck, M, Angermeyer, MC and Brähler, E (2005) Gibt es einen Zusammenhang zwischen Rechtsextremismus und sozialer Distanz gegenüber psychisch Kranken? Psychiatrische Praxis 32, 6872.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bizer, GY, Hart, J and Jekogian, AM (2012) Belief in just world and social dominance orientation: evidence for a mediational pathway predicting negative attitudes and discrimination against individuals with mental illness. Personality and Individual Differences 52, 428432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, MJ, Reyna, C, Chambers, JR, Crawford, JT and Wetherell, G (2014) The ideological-conflict hypothesis: intolerance among both liberals and conservatives. Current Directions in Psychological Science 23, 2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corrigan, PW (2011) Strategic stigma change (SSC): five principles for social marketing campaigns to reduce stigma. Psychiatric Services 62, 824826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLuca, JS and Yanos, PT (2016) Managing the terror of a dangerous world: political attitudes as predictors of mental health stigma. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 62, 2130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLuca, JS, Vaccaro, J, Seda, J and Yanos, PT (2018) Political attitudes as predictors of the multiple dimensions of mental health stigma. International Journal of Social Psychiatry 64, 459469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duckitt, J and Sibley, CG (2007) Right wing authoritarianism, social dominance orientation and the dimensions of general prejudice. European Journal of Personality 21, 113130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckitt, J and Sibley, CG (2010) Right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation differentially moderate intergroup effects on prejudice. European Journal of Personality 24, 583601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feather, NT (1984) Protestant ethic, conservatism, and values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46, 11321141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, E (2006) Right-wing authoritarianism and managerial assessment of a schizophrenic candidate. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 36, 953978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, EM, Wick, DP, Hartsen, KM and Preve, RM (2008) Right-wing authoritarianism in relation to proposed judicial action, electromyographic response, and affective attitudes toward a schizophrenic mother. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 38, 215233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A and Procter, E (1989) Belief in a just world: review and critique of the individual difference literature. British Journal of Social Psychology 28, 365384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furr, LA, Usui, W and Hines-Martin, V (2003) Authoritarianism and attitudes toward mental health services. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 73, 411418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, K (1940) Human Nature in the Light of Psychopathology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodhart, D (2017) The Road to Somewhere. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Hahm, S, Speerforck, S, Fleischer, T, Grabe, HJ, Beutel, M and Schomerus, G (2020) [The effects of gender, education, and income on anticipated shame regarding mental illness – results of a German population study]. Psychiatrische Praxis 47, 142147.Google Scholar
Inbar, Y and Lammers, J (2012) Political diversity in social and personality psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7, 496503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jost, JT, Glaser, J, Kruglanski, AW and Sulloway, FJ (2003) Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin 129, 339375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinman, A (2006) Culture, moral experience and medicine. The Mount Sinai Journal of Medicine 73, 834839.Google Scholar
Koppetsch, C (2019) Die Gesellschaft des Zorns. Bielefeld: Transcript Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kvaale, EP and Haslam, N (2016) Motivational orientations and psychiatric stigma: social motives influence how causal explanations relate to stigmatizing attitudes. Personality and Individual Differences 89, 111116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lampropoulos, D and Apostolidis, T (2018) Social dominance orientation and discrimination against people with schizophrenia: evidence of medicalization and dangerousness beliefs as legitimizing myths. The Spanish Journal of Psychology 21, e37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, MJ (1980) The Belief in A Just World: A Fundamental Delusion. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Löve, J, Bertisson, M, Matinsson, J, Wängnerud, L and Hensing, G (2019) Political ideology and stigmatizing attitudes toward depression: the Swedish case. International Journal of Health Policy and Management 8, 365374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKnight, B (2012) Loud Silence. Lulu.com.Google Scholar
Merkel, W (2017) Kosmopolitismus versus Kommunitarismus. Ein neuer Konflikt in der Demokratie. In Harfst, P, Kubbe, I and Poguntke, T (eds), Parties, Governments and Elites: The Comparative Study of Democracy. Wiesbaden: Springer, pp. 923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, RMG, Sorrentino, R, Windell, D and Manchanda, R (2008) Are personal values of importance in the stigmatization of people with mental illness? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 53, 848856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, P and Inglehart, R (2019) Cultural Backlash. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pescosolido, BA, Manago, B and Monahan, J (2019) Evolving public views on the likelihood of violence from people with mental illness: stigma and its consequences. Health Affairs 38, 17351743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, BE and Lane, MD (2001) Implications of authoritarianism for young adulthood: longitudinal analysis of college experiences and future goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27, 678690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelan, JE and Basow, SA (2007) College students’ attitudes toward mental illness: an examination of the stigma process. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 37, 28772902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pratto, F, Sidanius, J, Stallworth, LM and Malle, BF (1994) Social dominance orientation: a personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, 741763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rechenberg, T, Nowikow, J and Schomerus, G (2020) [“Nowadays, we wouldn't admit being depressed – instead, we tend to call it burnout” – reporting on burnout in German print media]. Psychiatrische Praxis 47, 426432.Google Scholar
Reckwitz, A (2019) Das Ende der Illusionen. Politik, Ökonomie und Kultur in der Spätmoderne. Berlin: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Rempala, DM, Okdie, BM and Garvey, KJ (2016) Articulating ideology: how liberals and conservatives justify political affiliations using morality-based explanations. Motivation and Emotion 40, 703719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rüsch, N, Todd, AR, Bodenhausen, GV and Corrigan, PW (2010) Biogenetic models of psychopathology, implicit guilt, and mental illness stigma. Psychiatry Research 179, 328332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schomerus, G, Matschinger, H and Angermeyer, MC (2006) Preferences of the public regarding cutbacks in expenditure for patient care: are there indications of discrimination against those with mental disorders? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 41, 369377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, NJ, Ford, CG and Boggs, ST (2017) Dangerous worldview: a mediator of the relation between disgust sensitivity and social conservatism. Personality and Individual Differences 119, 252261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, N, Feather, NT, Freeman, T and Roche, A (2007) Stigma and discrimination in health-care provision to drug users: the role of values, affect, and deservingness judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 37, 163186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thibodeau, PH, Fein, MJ, Goodbody, ES and Flusberg, SJ (2015) The depression schema: how label, features, and causal explanations affect lay conceptions of depression. Frontiers in Psychiatry 6, 728.Google ScholarPubMed
Van Leeuwen, F and Park, JH (2009) Perceptions of social dangers, moral foundations, and political orientation. Personality and Individual Differences 47, 169173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, AC, Corrigan, PW and Angell, B (2005) What motivates public support for legally mandated mental health treatment? Social Work Research 29, 8794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, M (1904/1905) Die protestantische Ethik und der Geist des Kapitalismus. Archiv für Sozialwissenschaften und Sozialpolitik 20 & 21.Google Scholar
Webster, AC and Stewart, RAC (1973) Theological Conservatism. Academic Press: New York, pp. 129147.Google Scholar
Wexler MN, (1996) A sociological framing of the NIMBY (not-in-my-backyard) syndrome. International Review of Modern Sociology 26, 91110.Google Scholar
Yang, LH, Kleinman, A, Link, BG, Phelan, JC, Lee, S and Good, B (2007) Culture and stigma: adding moral experience to stigma theory. Social Science & Medicine 64, 15241535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, LH, Purdie-Vaughns, V, Kotabe, H, Link, BG, Saw, A, Wong, G and Phelan, JC (2013) Culture, threat, and mental illness stigma: identifying culture-specific threat among Chinese-American groups. Social Science & Medicine 88, 5667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, LH, Thornicroft, G, Alvarado, R, Vega, E and Link, BG (2014a) Recent advances in cross-cultural measurement in psychiatric epidemiology: utilizing ‘what matters most’ to identify culture-specific aspects of stigma. International Journal of Epidemiology 43, 494510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, LH, Chen, F, Sia, KJ, Lam, J, Lam, K, Ngo, H, Kleinman, A and Good, B (2014b) “What matters most”: a cultural mechanism moderating structural vulnerability and moral experience of mental illness stigma. Social Science & Medicine 103, 8493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, LH, Ho-Foster, AR, Becker, TD, Misra, S, Rampa, S, Poku, OB, Entaile, P, Goodman, M and Blank, MB (2021) Psychometric validation of a scale to assess culturally-salient aspects of HIV stigma among women living with HIV in Botswana: engaging “what matters most” to resist stigma. AIDS and Behavior 25, 459474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar