Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T13:25:06.063Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Taming English modals

How a Construction Grammar approach helps to understand modal verbs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2018

Extract

In the present paper, I pursue a Construction Grammar (CxG) characterization of English modal auxiliaries (e.g., can-could, must, have (got) to, should, ought to, need to, will-would) that seeks to add to established lexical approaches. It is argued that Construction Grammar (e.g., Goldberg, 1995, 2006; Tomasello, 2003) can successfully account for underlying modality patterns, the understanding of which can lead to distinct gains for both linguistics and second language acquisition research. To that end, some of the tenets of CxG are invoked.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alonso–Ovalle, L. & Menéndez–Benito, P. 2015. ‘Epistemic indefinites: An overview.’ In Alonso–Ovalle, L. & Menéndez–Benito, P. (eds.), Epistemic Indefinites: Exploring Modality Beyond the Verbal Domain. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp.127.Google Scholar
Bergs, A. 2010. ‘Expressions of futurity in contemporary English: A Construction Grammar perspective.’ English Language and Linguistics, 14(2), 217–38.Google Scholar
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S. & Finegan, E. 1999. Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, Essex: Pearson Education Ltd.Google Scholar
Borer, H. 2012. ‘In the event of a nominal.’ In Everaert, M., Marelj, M. & Siloni, T. (eds.), The Theta System: Argument Structure at the Interface. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 104–50.Google Scholar
Borer, H. 2013. Structuring Sense: Volume III: Taking Form. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cappelle, B. & Depraetere, I. 2016. ‘Short-circuited interpretations of modal verb constructions: Some evidence from The Simpsons.’ Constructions and Frames, 8(1), 739.Google Scholar
Depraetere, I. & Reed, S. 2011. ‘Towards a more explicit taxonomy of root possibility.’ English Language and Linguistics, 15(1), 129.Google Scholar
Egan, A. & Weatherson, B. (eds.). 2011. ‘Introduction: Epistemic modals and epistemic modality.’ In Epistemic Modality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elugardo, R. 2013. ‘Sub-sentential speech acts, reflexive content, and pragmatic enrichment.’ In Goldstein, L. (ed.), Brevity. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 91106.Google Scholar
Enfield, N. J. 2017. ‘Elements of agency.’ In Enfield, N. J. & Kockelman, P. (eds.), Distributed Agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 48.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. 1995. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. 2006. Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kockelman, P. 2012. Agent, Person, Subject, Self: A Theory of Ontology, Interaction, and Infrastructure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. 2013. ‘Modals: Striving for control.’ In Marín–Arrese, J. I., Carretero, M., Hita, J. A. & van der Auwera, J. (eds.), English Modality: Core, Periphery and Evidentiality. Berlin/Boston: DeGruyter Mouton, pp. 356.Google Scholar
Mani, I. & Pustejovsky, J. 2012. ‘Introduction.’ In Mani, I. & Pustejovsky, J. (eds.), Interpreting Motion: Grounded Representations for Spatial Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 128.Google Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G. & Svartvik, J. 1985. A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. 1990. From Etymology to Pragmatics: Metaphorical and Cultural Aspects of Semantic Structure. (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 54). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 1988. ‘Force dynamics in language and cognition.’ Cognitive Science, 12, 49100.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a Language: A Usage-based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Torres–Martínez, S. 2014. ‘Introducing conversational grammar in EFL: A case for hedging strings: Bringing insights from corpus linguistics and construction grammar into the English language classroom.’ English Today, 30(2), 2432.Google Scholar
Torres–Martínez, S. 2015. ‘A constructionist approach to the teaching of phrasal verbs.’ English Today, 31(3), 4658.Google Scholar
Torres–Martínez, S. 2016. ‘Working out multiword verbs within an Applied Cognitive Construction Grammar framework.’ European Journal of Applied Linguistics, 4(2), 132.Google Scholar
Torres–Martínez, S. 2017. ‘Applied Cognitive Construction Grammar: A usage-based approach to the teaching of phrasal verbs (and other constructions).’ European Journal of Applied Linguistics, 5(1), 138.Google Scholar
Torres–Martínez, S. 2018. ‘Constructions as triads of form, function, and agency: An Agentive Cognitive Construction Grammar study of English modals.’ Cognitive Semantics, 4(1), 138.Google Scholar
Tyler, A. 2012. Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Learning: Theoretical Basics and Experimental Evidence. Oxon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Yu, C. & Ballard, D. 2010. ‘The role of the body in infant language learning.’ In Mix, K. S., Smith, L. B. & Gasser, M. (eds.), The Spatial Foundations of Language and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 208–34.Google Scholar