Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T04:29:25.246Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The structural non-integration of wh-clefts1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2015

WOJCIECH GUZ*
Affiliation:
English Department, John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Al. Racławickie 14, 20-950 Lublin, [email protected]

Abstract

Weinert & Miller (1996) suggest that English wh-clefts are a heterogeneous class in that they can have varied degrees of structural integration. Many such constructions depart structurally from the canonical wh-cleft which consists of a wh-clause, the copula and a focus constituent, and in which all the three elements are brought together into a fully integrated utterance. In the types of wh-clefts displaying looser structure, their lack of syntactic integration has so far been related to such linguistic features as (a) omission of the copula, (b) non-canonical copular complementation, e.g. independent main clauses instead of standard infinitival phrases appearing in the focus constituent, (c) lack of a clearly identifiable copular complement, (d) the focusing effect of the wh-clause extending over several clauses (Weinert & Miller 1996; Koops & Ross-Hagebaum 2008; Hopper & Thompson 2008; Callies 2012). Although the disintegrating effect of these features has been observed, the extent of the phenomenon in modern English has not been properly established and other non-integration features have not been investigated. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to use corpus data to identify and examine such non-integration features and to investigate the extent – expressed quantitatively – to which these features are found in wh-clefts with the verb do in the wh-clause. The article also points out the formulaic status of those wh-clauses which become disconnected from their focus phrases.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1

I would like to thank two anonymous reviewers of ELL for their most valuable comments on the first draft of this article. Thanks are also due to Piotr Pęzik for the opportunity to access the BNC audio files.

References

Biber, Douglas, Johansson, Stig, Leech, Geoffrey, Conrad, Susan & Finegan, Edward. 1999. Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Harlow: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul. 2001. Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International 5 (9/10), 341–5.Google Scholar
Callies, Marcus. 2012. The grammaticalization and pragmaticalization of cleft constructions in present-day English. In Hoffmann, Sebastian, Rayson, Paul & Leech, Geoffrey (eds.), Corpus linguistics: Looking back – moving forward, 521.Amsterdam: Rodopi.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, Ronald & McCarthy, Michael. 2006. Cambridge grammar of English CD-ROM: A comprehensive guide to spoken and written English usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Collins, Peter. 1991. Cleft and pseudo-cleft constructions in English. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Collins, Peter. 2006. It-clefts and wh-clefts: Prosody and pragmatics. Journal of Pragmatics 38 (10), 1706–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, Mark. 2004–. BYU-BNC. (Based on the British National Corpus from Oxford University Press). Available online at http://corpus.byu.edu/bnc/.Google Scholar
Davies, Mark. 2008–. The Corpus of Contemporary American English: 450 million words, 1990–present. Available online at http://corpus.byu.edu/coca/.Google Scholar
Hopper, J. Paul & Thompson, Sandra A.. 2008. Projectibility and clause combining in interaction. In Laury, Ritva (ed.), Crosslinguistic studies of clause combining: The multifunctionality of conjunctions, 99124. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddleston, Rodney & Pullum, Geoffrey. 2002. The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendal, Tyler. 2013. Speech rate, pause and sociolinguistic variation: Studies in corpus sociophonetics. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koops, Christian & Ross-Hagebaum, Sebastian. 2008. Information structure and discourse function of amalgam wh-clefts. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 461–72.Google Scholar
Lambrecht, Knud. 2001. A framework for the analysis of cleft constructions. Linguistics 39 (3), 463516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Jim. 2011. A critical introduction to syntax. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Miller, Jim & Weinert, Regina. 1998. Spontaneous spoken language: Syntax and discourse. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukherjee, Joybrato. 2000. Speech is silver, but silence is golden: some remarks on the function(s) of pauses. Anglia 118 (4), 571–84.Google Scholar
O'Keeffe, Anne, McCarthy, Michael & Carter, Ronald. 2007. From corpus to classroom: Language use and language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pęzik, Piotr. 2015–. Spokes – a search and exploration service for conversational corpus data. Available online at: http://pelcra.clarin-pl.eu/SpokesBNCGoogle Scholar
Quirk, Randolph, Greenbaum, Sidney, Leech, Geoffrey & Svartvik, Jan. 1985. A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Ross, Haj. 2000. The frozenness of pseudoclefts – towards an inequality-based syntax. In Okrent, Arika & Boyle, John P. (eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, 385426. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Weinert, Regina & Miller, Jim. 1996. Cleft constructions in spoken language. Journal of Pragmatics 25 (2), 173206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar