Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T11:38:21.776Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A construction grammar analysis of the transitive be perfect in present-day Canadian English

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 January 2015

YURI YERASTOV*
Affiliation:
Department of English, Fort Hays State University, 600 Park Street, Hays, KS [email protected]

Abstract

This article offers a syntactic analysis of the construction [be done NP], e.g. I am done dinner, I am finished my homework, as found in Canadian English and some US dialects. After situating this construction in the context of a productive transitive be perfect in Scots/English dialects, [be done NP] will be distinguished from a set of its conceptual and structural relatives, and ultimately be shown not to be reducible to a surface realization of another underlying structure. From the perspective of syntactic theory, the article problematizes the parsimony of the mainstream generative approach (most recently in MacFadden & Alexiadou 2010) in accounting for the facts of [be done NP] on strictly compositional grounds, as well as the mainstream view of lexical items as projecting theta grids and subcategorization frames (as e.g. in Grimshaw 1979; Emonds 2000). Following Fillmore et al. (1988), Goldberg (1995, 2005) and others, what will be suggested instead is a construction grammar approach to [be done NP], under which a construction holistically licenses its argument structure. Along these lines [be done NP] will be characterized as an abstract construction with some fixed material.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atwood, E. Bagby. 1953. A survey of verb forms in the Eastern United Stated. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, Mark. 1988. Theta theory and the syntax of applicatives in Chichewa. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 6 (3), 353–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Brinton, Laurel J. & Traugott, Elizabeth Closs. 2005. Lexicalization and language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burzio, Luigi. 1986. Italian syntax: A government–binding approach. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1985. Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2001. Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2006. From usage to grammar: the mind's response to repetition. Language 82 (4), 711–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague and Paris: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on government and binding: The Pisa lectures. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Comrie, Bernard. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter & Jackendoff, Ray. 2005. Simpler syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Embick, David. 2004. On the structure of resultative participles in English. Linguistic Inquiry 35 (3), 355–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emonds, Joseph E. 2000. Lexicon and grammar: The English syntacticon. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J., Kay, Paul & O’Connor, Mary Katherine. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let a lone. Language 64, 501–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, Elaine. 2007. Aspect in Bungi: Expanded progressives and be perfects. In Radišić, Milica (ed.), Proceedings of the 2007 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. (Available online at http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2007/Gold.pdf, accessed 1 May 2014)Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2003. Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science 7 (5), 219–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, Adele E. 2005. Argument realization: The role of constructions, lexical semantics and discourse. In Östman, Jan-Ola & Fried, Mirjan (eds.), Construction grammars: Cognitive grounding and theoretical extension, 1744. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grimshaw, Jane. 1979. Complement selection and the lexicon. Linguistic Inquiry 10 (2), 279326.Google Scholar
Hinnell, Jennifer. 2012. A construction analysis of [be done X] in Canadian English. MA thesis, Simon Fraser University.Google Scholar
Hoekstra, Teun. 1984. Transitivity: Grammatical relations in government binding theory. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 1997. Twistin’ the night away. Language 73, 534–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jespersen, Otto. 1924. The philosophy of grammar. London: George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Kay, Paul & Fillmore, Charles. 1999. Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The What's X doing Y? construction. Language 75 (1), 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kortmann, Bernd & Lunkenheimer, Kerstin (eds.) 2013. The electronic world atlas of varieties of English. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. (Available online at http://ewave-atlas.org, accessed 1 May 2014)Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 2005. A dynamic usage-based model. In Barlow, Michael and Kemmer, Suzanne (eds.), Usage based models of language, 163. Stanford, CA: CSLI.Google Scholar
Lightfoot, David. 1991. How to set parameters. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McFadden, Thomas & Alexiadou, Artemis. 2010. Perfects, resultatives, and auxiliaries in Earlier English. Linguistic Inquiry 41 (3), 389425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millar, Robert. 2007. Northern and Insular Scots. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mustanoja, Tauno F. 1960. A Middle English syntax. Helsinki: Société néophilologique.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. 1997. The origin of the be-perfect with transitives in the Shetland dialect. Scottish Language 16, 8896.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, D. 1978. Impersonal passives and the unaccusative hypothesis. Proceedings of the Fourth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistic Society. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistic Society.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeper, Thomas & Williams, Edwin. 1987. Parameter setting. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, K. Aaron. 2001. The role of frequency in the specialization of the English anterior. In Bybee, Joan (ed.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 361–82. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, K. Aaron. 2007. Language use and auxiliary selection in the perfect. In Aranovich, Raúl (ed.), Split auxiliary systems: A cross-linguistic perspective, 255–70. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sorace, Antonella. 2000. Gradients in auxiliary selection with intransitive verbs. Language 76, 859–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Zeno. 1957. Verbs and times. The Philosophical Review 66, 143–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfram, Walt. 1996. Delineation of and description in dialectology: The case of perfective I’m in Lumbee English. American Speech 71 (1), 526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerastov, Yuri. 2010. I am done dinner: When synchrony meets diachrony. PhD dissertation, University of Calgary.Google Scholar
Yerastov, Yuri. 2012. Transitive be perfect: An experimental study of Canadian English. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 57 (3), 427–57.Google Scholar