Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T13:39:50.264Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Auditory temporal processing, phonological awareness, and oral language ability in prereaders: Can we identify children at risk for reading disability more accurately?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

Steve M. Heath*
Affiliation:
The University of Western Australia
John H. Hogben
Affiliation:
The University of Western Australia
*
Department of Psychology, The University of Western Australia, Nedlands WA 6907, Ph: 9380 3259, Fax: 9380 3643, E-mail: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

A longitudinal study was designed to investigate the possibility of improving current accuracy in prediction of reading disability, using phonological awareness (PA), oral language, and auditory temporal processing (ATP) as predictors. Preschoolers (n = 106) were tested on PA, and two groups were selected from the upper and lower quartiles of the PA distribution for initial testing as prereaders on ATP and oral language, and later testing on reading and oral language at the end of years 1, 2 , and 3. Oral language markedly improved levels of prediction previously achieved using PA alone. However, although ATP is related to PA and oral language in prereaders, it contributed little to prediction of reading achievement beyond that afforded by measures of PA and oral language. Options for improving the levels of prediction achieved here by increasing the sensitivity of our measure of AJP are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: Author.Google Scholar
Ball, E.W., & Blachman, B.A. (1988). Phoneme segmentation training: Effect on reading readiness. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 208225.Google Scholar
Bishop, D.V.M. (1991). Phonological abilities and reading disabilities. Mind and Language, 6, 93122.Google Scholar
Bishop, D.V.M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 10271050.Google Scholar
Bishop, D.V.M., Bishop, S.J., Bright, P., James, C., Delaney, T., & Tallal, P. (1999). Different origin of auditory deficit and weak phonological short-term memory in children with specific language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 155168.Google Scholar
Bishop, D.V.M., Carlyon, R.P., Deeks, J.M., & Bishop, S.J. (1999) Auditory temporal processing impairment: Neither necessary nor sufficient for causing language impairment in children. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 12951310.Google Scholar
Bowey, J.A., Cain, M.T., & Ryan, S.M. (1992). A reading level design study of phonological skills underlying fourth-grade children’s word reading difficulties. Child Development, 63, 9991011.Google Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P.E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of backward reading. Nature, 271, 746747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P.E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read-a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.Google Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P.E. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexies’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874886.Google Scholar
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1995). Oral and written language scales: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc.Google Scholar
Catts, H.W. (1991). Early identification of dyslexia: Evidence from a follow-up study of speech-impaired children. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 163177.Google Scholar
Catts, H.W. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948958.Google Scholar
Catts, H.W. (1996). Defining dyslexia as a developmental language disorder: An expanded view. Topics of Language Disorders, 16, 1429.Google Scholar
Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, S.M., & McCrosky, R.L. (1980). Auditory fusion in children. Child Development, 51, 7580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodd, B., Oerlemans, M., Holm, A., & McCormick, M. (1996). Queensland University Inventory of Literacy. St. Lucia, Qld.: Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology, University of Queensland.Google Scholar
Donahue, M. (1986). Linguistic and communicative development in learning-disabled children. In Ceci, S. (Ed.). Handbook of cognitive, social and neurological aspects of learning disabilities (263–291). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dunn, L.M., & Dunn, L.M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test – Revised: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Elliott, C.D., Murray, D.J., & Pearson, L.S. (1983). British Ability Scales: Manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Farmer, M.E., & Klein, R. (1993). Auditory and visual temporal processing in dyslexic and normal readers. In Tallal, P.Galaburda, A.M.Llinas, R.R. & von Euler, C. (Eds.), Temporal information processing in the nervous system: Special reference to dyslexia and dysphasia (pp. 339341). New York: The New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Gillon, G., & Dodd, B. (1993). The phonological, syntactic and semantic skills of children with specific reading disability. Australian Journal of Human Communication Disorders, 21(1), 86102.Google Scholar
Hari, R., & Kiesila, P. (1996). Deficits of temporal auditory processing in dyslexic adults. Neuroscience Letters, 205, 138140.Google Scholar
Heath, S.M., & Hogben, J.H. (2000). Further evidence of auditory temporal processing and oral language deficits in children with reading disability. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Heath, S.M., Hogben, J.H., & Clark, C.D. (1999). Auditory temporal and phonological processing in disabled readers with and without oral language delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 637647.Google Scholar
Hurford, D.P., Schauf, J.D., Bunce, L., Blaich, T., & Moore, K. (1994). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 371382.Google Scholar
Irwin, R.J., Ball, K.R., Stillman, J.A., & Rosser, J. (1985). The development of auditory temporal acuity in children. Child Development, 56, 614620.Google Scholar
Jensen, J.K., Neff, D.L., & Callaghan, B.P. (1987). Frequency, intensity and duration discrimination in young children. Asha, 29, 88.Google Scholar
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437447.Google Scholar
Juel, C., Griffith, P.L., & Gough, P.B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243255.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A.G. (1992). Response to historical perspective: A developmental language perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 4852.Google Scholar
Lundberg, I., Frost, J., Petersen, O.-P. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in pre-school children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263284.Google Scholar
McArthur, G.M., Hogben, J.H., Edwards, V.T., Heath, S.M., & Mengler, E.D. (2000). On the “specifics” of specific reading disability and specific language impairment. In press, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCroskey, R.L., & Kidder, H.C. (1980). Auditory fusion among learning disabled, reading disabled, and normal children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 13(2), 6976.Google Scholar
MacDonald, W.G., & Cornwall, A. (1995). The relationship between phonological awareness and reading and spelling achievement eleven years later. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 523527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Majsterek, D.J., & Ellenwood, A.E. (1995). Phonological awareness and beginning reading: Evaluation of a school-based screening procedure. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 449456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, V.A. (1993). Phoneme awareness and future reading ability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 259269.Google Scholar
Merzenich, M.M., Jenkins, W.M., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S.L., & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrongiello, B.A., Kulig, J.W., & Clifton, R.K. (1984). Developmental changes in auditory temporal perception. Child Development, 55, 461471.Google Scholar
Mueller, P.U. (1993). Some reflection on psychophysical measurement with dyslexic children. In Wright, S.F. & Groner, R. (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 207214). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, M.D. (1988). Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (rev.): Manual. Hawthorn: Australian Council for Educational Research.Google Scholar
Neilson, R. (1995, May). The Sutherland Phonological Awareness Test. Paper presented at the Australian Association for Speech and Hearing Annual Conference, Brisbane.Google Scholar
Pratt, A.C., & Brady, S. (1992). Relation of phonological awareness to reading disability in children and adults. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 319323.Google Scholar
Reed, M.A. (1989). Speech perception and the discrimination of brief auditory cues in reading disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 270292.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Yule, W. (1975). The concept of specific reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 181197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H.S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 17281743.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W. (1987). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (rev.): Manual. San Antonio, CA: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Semel, E., Wiig, E.H., & Secord, W. (1995). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (3rd ed.).: Manual San Antonio, CA: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Share, D.L., Jorm, A.E., MacLean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 13091324.Google Scholar
Snowling, M.J. (1991). Developmental reading disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 4977.Google Scholar
Snowling, M.J., & Nation, K.A. (1997). Language, phonology and learning to read. In Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention (pp 153166). London: Whurr Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Stackhouse, J., & Wells, B. (1997). How do speech and language problems affect literacy development? In Snowling, M. & Hulme, C. (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention (pp 182211). London: Whurr Publishers Ltd.Google Scholar
Standards Association of Australia. (1983). Australian standards on audiometers, (Number 2586). Sydney.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360406.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K.E. (1994). Does dyslexia exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 579595.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K.E., Cunningham, A.E. & Cramer, B.B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175190.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K.E., Cunningham, A.E., & Freeman, D.J. (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278303.Google Scholar
Swank, L.K., & Catts, H.W. (1994). Phonological awareness and written word decoding. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 914.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (1976). Rapid auditory processing in normal and disordered language development. Journal of Speech and Hearing, 3, 561571.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language, 9, 182198.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., Miller, S.L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S.S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W.M., & Merzenich, M.M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 8184.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Developmental aphasia: Impaired rate of nonverbal processing as a function of sensory modality. Neuropsychologic, 11, 389398.Google Scholar
Taylor, M.M., & Creelman, C.D. (1967). PEST: Efficient estimates on probability functions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 41, 782787.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J.B., & Quinn, M.A. (1983). The contribution of perceptual learning to performance on the repetition task. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 369372.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J.K., & Bryant, B.R. (1994). Test of Phonological Awareness. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Tukey, J.W. (1977). Exploratory data analysis. London: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Wagner, R.K., & Torgesen, J.K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212.Google Scholar
Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K., & Rashotte, C.A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abililities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 7387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, P.E. (1994). Linguistic factors in reading disability: A model for assessing children who are without overt language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 30, 259281.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler Pre-School And Primary Scale Of Intelligence – Revised: Manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wightman, F., Allen, P., Dolan, T., Kistler, D., & Jamieson, D. (1989). Temporal resolution in children. Child Development, 60, 611624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R.W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests (rev.): Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service Inc.Google Scholar
Wright, B.A., Lombardino, L.J., King, W.M., Puranik, C.S., Leonard, C.M., & Merzenich, M.M. (1997). Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolution in language impaired children. Nature, 387, 176177.Google Scholar
Yopp, H. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159177.Google Scholar