Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T05:05:27.153Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Assessing cognitive inhibition in emotional and neutral contexts in children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 April 2020

Eliana V. Zamora*
Affiliation:
Institute of Basic, Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National Council for Scientific and Technical Investigations (CONICET), National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
Santiago Vernucci
Affiliation:
Institute of Basic, Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National Council for Scientific and Technical Investigations (CONICET), National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
Macarena del Valle
Affiliation:
Institute of Basic, Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National Council for Scientific and Technical Investigations (CONICET), National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
Isabel Introzzi
Affiliation:
Institute of Basic, Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National Council for Scientific and Technical Investigations (CONICET), National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
María M. Richard’s
Affiliation:
Institute of Basic, Applied Psychology and Technology (IPSIBAT), National Council for Scientific and Technical Investigations (CONICET), National University of Mar del Plata, Mar del Plata, Argentina
*
Author for correspondence: Eliana Vanesa Zamora, Email: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

Different studies indicate that emotions can interfere with the efficacy of inhibitory control. However, understanding this impact requires considering that inhibition is not a unitary construct. Cognitive inhibition is the process responsible for attenuating and resisting the interference of thoughts, representations, and memories that are irrelevant to the task at hand. Due to the relevance of emotional stimuli for survival, different studies have indicated that the performance of cognitive inhibition can vary depending on the context, that is, whether in neutral or emotionally salient contexts. During the interval between 8 and 12 years old, the importance of this skill is rooted in the need for controlling reactions and stimuli that could be disruptive for learning. In this study, 395 children aged 8–12 years performed a 1-back visual task with emotional and neutral stimuli in order to assess cognitive inhibition in contexts with high and low emotional salience. The results support the validity of the task for the evaluation of cognitive inhibition. However, no significant differences were found, depending on the context, as expected. It is emphasised that these results constitute an approach to the problem of emotional content interference in children that considers the multidimensional approach of inhibition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© Australian Psychological Society Ltd 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychological Association. (2010). Ethical principles of psychologists and Code of Conduct. Washington, DC: Author. http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/principles.pdfGoogle Scholar
Arán-Filippetti, V. (2011). Funciones ejecutivas en niños escolarizados: efectos de la edad y del estrato socioeconómico [Executive functions in school children: Effects of age and socioeconomic status]. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, 29, 98–113. Retrived from https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=3699974Google Scholar
Arán-Filippetti, V., & López, M.B. (2017). Estructura latente de las funciones ejecutivas en adolescentes: Invarianza factorial en función del sexo [Latent structure of executive functions in adolescents: Factorial invariance according to sex]. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, 35, 615629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnsten, A.F., & Rubia, K. (2012). Neurobiological circuits regulating attention, cognitive control, motivation, and emotion: Disruptions in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 356367.10.1016/j.jaac.2012.01.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Augusti, E.M., & Melinder, A. (2013). The effect of neutral and negative colour photographs on children’s item directed forgetting. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 378391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banich, M.T., & Depue, B.E. (2015). Recent advances in understanding neural systems that support inhibitory control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 1722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bannerman, R.L., Temminck, E.V., & Sahraie, A. (2012). Emotional stimuli capture spatial attention but do not modulate spatial memory. Vision Research, 65, 1220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brenner, E. (2000). Mood induction in children: Methodological issues and clinical implications. Review of General Psychology, 4, 264283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, S.J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 307316.10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campoy-Menéndez, G., García-Sevilla, J., Egea-Caparrós, A., Saurín-Riquelme, L.,& Martínez-Sánchez, F. (1997). Influencia del nivel de intensidad afectiva en el procesamiento de estímulos emocionales en una tarea Stroop [Influence of the level of affective intensity in the processing of emotional stimuli in a Stroop task]. Revista Electrónica de Motivación y Emoción, 4(7). http://reme.uji.es/articulos/amartf5551202101/texto.html.Google Scholar
Carretié, L. (2014). Exogenous (automatic) attention to emotional stimuli: A review. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 12281258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carretié, L., López-Martín, S., & Albert, J. (2010). Papel de la corteza prefrontal ventromedial en la respuesta a eventos emocionalmente negativos [Role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the response to emotionally negative events]. Revista de Neurología, 50, 245252.10.33588/rn.5004.2009292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B.J., Thomas, K.M., Welsh, T.F., Livnat, R., & Eccard, C.H. (2000). Cognitive and behavioral probes of developmental landmarks for use in functional neuroimaging. In Ernst, M. & Rumsey, J.M. (Eds.), Functional neuroimaging in child psychiatry (pp. 155168). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas. (2006). Lineamientos para el comportamiento ético en las ciencias sociales y humanidades. Resolución 2857/06. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Ministerio de Educación, Ciencia y Tecnología. –http://www.conicet.gov.ar/wp-content/uploads/RD-20061211–2857.pdfGoogle Scholar
Conway, A.R., Kane, M.J., & Engle, R.W. (2003). Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 547552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Houwer, J., & Tibboel, H. (2010). Stop what you are not doing! Emotional pictures interfere with the task not to respond. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 699703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dempster, F.N. (1993). Resistance to interference: Developmental changes in a basic processing dimension. In Howe, M.L. & Pasnak, R. (Eds.), Emerging themes in cognitive development (Vol. 1: Foundations; pp. 327). New York: Springer–Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (2016). Why improving and assessing executive functions early in life is critical. In Griffin, J.A., McCardle, P. & Freund, L.S. (Eds.), Executive function in preschool-age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment, and translational research (pp. 1143). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolcos, F., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 20722079.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5042-05.2006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, M.W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M.G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falquez, R., Lang, S., Dinu-Biringer, R., Nees, F., Arens, E., Kotchoubey, B., … Barnow, S. (2015). On the relationship between negative affective priming and prefrontal cognitive control mechanisms. Cognition & Emotion, 30, 225–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farbiash, T., & Berger, A. (2016). Brain and behavioral inhibitory control of kindergartners facing negative emotions. Developmental Science, 19, 741756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N.P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N.P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Pacios, J., Del Río, D., Villalobos, D., Ruiz-Vargas, J.M., & Maestú, F. (2015). Emotional interference-based forgetting in short-term memory. Cognitive inhibition of pleasant but not unpleasant biologically relevant distractors. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 582.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. (2003). Statistics for Psychology Using SPSS. Mexico: Pearson EducationGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B.J., & Baddeley, A.D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 174180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Vohs, K.D., & Baumeister, R.F. (2012). What people desire, feel conflicted about, and try to resist in everyday life. Psychological Science, 23, 582588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, T.R. (2015). Validez [Validity]. In Hogan, T.R. (Ed.), Pruebas psicológicas: Una introducción práctica (2nd ed., pp. 199258). México, DF: Manual Moderno.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A.B. (2011) Four Factor Index of Social Status. Yale Journal of Sociology, 8, 252Google Scholar
Hung, Y., Gaillard, S.L., Yarmak, P., & Arsalidou, M. (2018). Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: Voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Human Brain Mapping, 39, 40654082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huston, A.C., & Ripke, M.N. (2006). Middle childhood: Contexts of development. In Huston, A.C., & Ripke, M.N. (Eds.). Developmental contexts in middle childhood (pp. 122). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (2002). Developmental change in proactive interference. Child Development, 73, 17031714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalanthroff, E., Cohen, N., & Henik, A. (2013). Stop feeling: inhibition of emotional interference following stop-signal trials. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M.J., Meier, M.E., Smeekens, B.A., Gross, G.M., Chun, C.A., Silvia, P.J., & Kwapil, T.R. (2016). Individual differences in the executive control of attention, memory, and thought, and their associations with schizotypy. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 145, 10171048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P., & Kotz, S.A. (2010). Modulation of early conflict processing: N200 responses to emotional words in a flanker task. Neuropsychologia, 48, 36613664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kar, B. R., Vijay, N., & Mishra, S. (2013). Development of cognitive and affective control networks and decision making. Progress in Brain Research, 202, 347368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E.A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion, 3, 378393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S., Nordling, J., Yoon, J., Boldt, L., & Kochanska, G. (2013). Effortful control in ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ tasks differentially predicts children’s behavior problems and academic performance. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 4356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, R.M., Christie, J.J., & Ivanoff, J. (2004). Graphical and other methods for representing the speed and accuracy of performance. Presented at the 45th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Ladouceur, C.D., Dahl, R.E., Williamson, D.E., Birmaher, B., Ryan, N.D., & Casey, B.J. (2005). Altered emotional processing in pediatric anxiety, depression, and comorbid anxiety-depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 165177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., White, L.K., McDermott, J.M., & Fox, N.A. (2012). Neural activation underlying cognitive control in the context of neutral and affectively charged pictures in children. Brain Cognition, 79, 181187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langner, R., Leiberg, S., Hoffstaedter, F., & Eickhoff, S.B. (2018). Towards a human self-regulation system: Common and distinct neural signatures of emotional and behavioural control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 400410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levens, S.M., & Phelps, E.A. (2008). Emotion processing effects on interference resolution in working memory. Emotion, 8, 267280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindström, B.R., & Bohlin, G. (2012) Threat-relevance impairs executive functions: Negative impact on working memory and response inhibition. Emotion, 12, 384–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loosli, S.V., Rahm, B., Unterrainer, J.M., Weiller, C., & Kaller, C.P. (2014). Developmental change in proactive interference across the life span: Evidence from two working memory tasks. Developmental Psychology, 50, 10601072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, Y., Jaquess, K.J., Hatfield, B.D., Zhou, C., & Li, H. (2017). Valence and arousal of emotional stimuli impact cognitive-motor performance in an oddball task. Biological Psychology, 125, 105114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, C.M., Dodd, M.D., Sheard, E.D., Wilson, D.E., & Bibi, U. (2003). In opposition to inhibition. In Ross, B.H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory: Vol. 43 (pp. 163214). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Marchewka, A., Żurawski, Ł., Jednoróg, K., & Grabowska, A. (2014). The Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS): Introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 596610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, F.P., & Sharma, D. (2004). Reversing the emotional Stroop effect reveals that it is not what it seems: The role of fast and slow components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 30, 382392.Google Scholar
Meule, A. (2017). Reporting and interpreting working memory performance in n-back tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueller, S.C., Cromheeke, S., Siugzdaite, R., & Boehler, C.N. (2017). Evidence for the triadic model of adolescent brain development: Cognitive load and task-relevance of emotion differentially affect adolescents and adults. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 26, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullane, J.C., Corkum, P.V., Klein, R.M., & McLaughlin, E. (2009). Interference control in children with and without ADHD: A systematic review of Flanker and Simon task performance. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 321342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J.T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nigg, J.T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 361383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowicka, A., Marchewka, A., Jednorog, K., Tacikowski, P., & Brechmann, A. (2011). Forgetting of emotional information is hard: An fMRI study of directed forgetting. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 539549.10.1093/cercor/bhq117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberauer, K. (2009). Design for a working memory. In Ross, B.H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 51. (pp. 45100). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, A.M., McMillan, K.M., Laird, A.R., & Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 4659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascual, L., Galperín, C., & Bornstein, M.H. (1993). La medición del nivel socioeconómico y la psicología evolutiva: el caso argentino [The measurement of socioeconomic status and evolutionary psychology: The Argentine case]. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 27, 5974.Google Scholar
Panayiotou, M., & Humphrey, N. (2017). Mental health difficulties and academic attainment: Evidence for gender-specific developmental cascades in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 523538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, J.W. (2007). PsychoPy-Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162, 813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, E., & Welsh, M.C. (2014). The development of hot and cool executive functions in childhood and adolescence: Are we getting warmer?. In Goldstein, S. & Naglieri, J.A. (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 4565). New York, NY: Springer.10.1007/978-1-4614-8106-5_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petrican, R., & Grady, C.L. (2019). The intrinsic neural architecture of inhibitory control: The role of development and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia, 127, 93105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prencipe, A., Kesek, A., Cohen, J., Lamm, C., Lewis, M.D., & Zelazo, P.D. (2011). Development of hot and cool executive function during the transition to adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 621637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebetez, M.M.L., Rochat, L., Billieux, J., Gay, P., & Van der Linden, M. (2015). Do emotional stimuli interfere with two distinct components of inhibition? Cognition & Emotion, 29, 559567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redick, T.S., & Lindsey, D.R. (2013). Complex span and n-back measures of working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 11021113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ridderinkhof, K.R., & van der Stelt, O. (2000). Attention and selection in the growing child: Views derived from developmental psychophysiology. Biological Psychology, 54, 55106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sautú, R. (1989). Teoría y técnica en la medición del status ocupacional: Escalas objetivas de Prestigio (Working Document). Buenos Aires, Argentina: UBA Institute of Social Sciences.Google Scholar
Shilling, V.M., Chetwynd, A., & Rabbitt, P.M.A. (2002). Individual inconsistency across measures of inhibition: An investigation of the construct validity of inhibition in older adults. Neuropsychologia, 40, 605619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U. (2003). Affect measurement in experience sampling research. Journal of Happiness Studies, 4, 79106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, K.P., Fan, J., Magidina, O., Marks, D.J., Hahn, B., & Halperin, J.M. (2007). Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 151160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvers, J.A., McRae, K., Gabreli, J.D.E., & Gross, J.J. (2012). Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Emotion, 12, 12351247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E.E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 16571661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, S., Zilverstand, A., Song, H., Uquillas, F.D.O., Wang, Y., Xie, C., … Zou, Z. (2017). The influence of emotional interference on cognitive control: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using the emotional Stroop task. Scientific Reports, 7, 19.Google ScholarPubMed
Szmalec, A., Verbruggen, F., Vandierendonck, A., & Kemps, E. (2011). Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 137151.Google ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Tiego, J., Testa, R., Bellgrove, M.A., Pantelis, C., & Whittle, S. (2018). A hierarchical model of inhibitory control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbruggen, F., & De Houwer, J. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 391403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, A.M., & Lipp, O.V. (2008). Visual search for emotional faces in children. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 13061326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J.M.G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M., Kupersmidt, J., Voegler-Lee, M., & Bryant, D. (2011). Contributions of hot and cool self-regulation to preschool disruptive behavior and academic achievement. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 162180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Medical Association. (2013). Declaration of Helsinki — Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. https://www.wma.net/policies-post/wma-declaration-of-helsinki-ethical-principles-for-medical-research-involving-human-subjects/Google Scholar
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamora, E., Richards, M., Introzzi, I., & Aydmune, Y. (2016). Aportes empíricos sobre la validez de contenido del Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS) en niños. Resultados preliminares. PSIENCIA. Latin-American Journal of Psychological Science, 9. doi:10.5872/psiencia/9.103Google Scholar
Zelazo, P.D., & Cunningham, W.A. (2007). Executive Function: Mechanisms Underlying Emotion Regulation. In Gross, J.J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 135158). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P.D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 1137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P.D., Qu, L., & Kesek, A.C. (2010). Hot executive function: Emotion and the development of cognitive control. In Calkins, S.D. & Bell, M.A. (Eds.), Human brain development. Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 97111). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar