Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T11:36:27.198Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The onset of linguistic understanding

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

Claire M. Fletcher-Flinn*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland
Catherine J. McCormack
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland
*
Department of Psychology, The University of Auckland, Private Bag 92019, Auckland, New Zealand, E-mail: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

It is generally accepted that word comprehension emerges between 8 to 10 months of age and that speech production occurs several months later. In contrast, sign production appears earlier than its counterpart in speech and, by inference, suggests an earlier onset for comprehension. Most studies examining comprehension have tended to use older infants and have reported measures that may overestimate when words first are understood.This study examined the comprehension abilities of 33 infants between the ages of 5 and 11 months, using a parental diary-keeping method.The results support an early onset of comprehension within the context of common social routines and converge with a growing body of data on the cognitive precocity of infants.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, E. (1993). Comprehension and production in early language development. Monographs for the Society for Research in Early Child Development, 58, 222242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, M. (1995). Early lexical development. In Fletcher, P. and MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 362393). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation — Harcourt Brace & Co.Google Scholar
Benedict, H. E. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 6, 183200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonvillian, J. D., Orlansky, M. D., & Novack, L. L. (1983). Developmental milestones: Sign language acquisition and motor development. Child Development, 54, 14351445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E., & Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Language, 11, 345359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C. (1983). Communication to language: Deaf children’s and hearing children’s development compared. Sign Language Studies, 39,113143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behaviour. Language, 35, 2658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Hecht, B. F. (1983). Comprehension, production, and language acquisition. Annual Review of Psychology, 28, 406413.Google Scholar
Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eimas, P. D.Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., & Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thai, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, v–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R.M., & Hirsch-Pasek, K. (1995). Reinterpreting children’s sentence comprehension: Toward a new framework. In Fletcher, P. & MacWhinney, B. (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 430461). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Harris, M., Yeeles, C., Chasin, J., & Oakley, Y. (1995). Symmetries and asymmetries in early lexical comprehension and production. Journal of Child Language, 22, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M., (1991). Language comprehension: A new look at some old themes. In Krasnegor, N., Rumbaugh, D., Schiefelbusch, R., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (Eds.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development (pp. 301320). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L. (Ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 331368). New York: Halstead.Google Scholar
Kearney, K., & Kearney, C. (1995). The accidental genius. Murfreesboro, TN: Woodshed Press, (p. 23).Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representation system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehler, J., & Dupoux, E. (1994). What infants know: The new cognitive science of early development. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J. W., & Schultz, M. C. (1995). Early language development in full-term and premature infants. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Prinz, M., & Prinz, E.A. (1979). Simultaneous acquisition of ASL and spoken English (in a hearing child of a deaf mother and hearing father). Sign Language Studies, 25, 283296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznick, J. S., & Goldfield, B. A. (1992). Rapid change in lexical development in comprehension and production. Developmental Psychology, 28, 406413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S.L., Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed