Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T04:28:03.379Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A new look at the potential of the brain to change as a result of early experience: The possible role of glutamate in early learning and retardation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

Amy N. Johnston*
Affiliation:
The University of New England
*
C/- Dept. of Physiology, University of New England, Armidale NSW 2531
Get access

Abstract

Lateralized brain function was originally thought to be unique to humans, but it has now been demonstrated to occur in many animal species, including primates, rats, dogs, and birds. Memory consolidation and retrieval in chicks is lateralized in neuroanatomical areas used and also in the times these areas are active. As the general patterns of memory encoding, elucidated in chicks, might also occur in humans, chicks are used as a model system for studying learning and memory. In addition, the detailed neurochemical investigation of imprinting has shown that the neurotransmitter glutamate plays an important role in the consolidation of early learning. Glutamate is also known to be important in neural plasticity, and a close link is now seen between early memory formation and the neural plastic events of early brain development. Behavioural testing using chicks has shown that glutamate intake can cause retardation of visual discrimination learning. This is of some concern given the common use of glutamate as a food additive. Clearly the role and indeed the presence of glutamate in the brain are finely balanced. This information is leading us to a greater understanding of the brain and its dynamic interaction with its environment. It also reinforces the increasing concerns regarding neural interactions with environmental roxins.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Angrist, B., Sathananthan, G., Wilk, S., & Gershon, S. (1974). Amphetamine psychosis: Behavioural and biochemical aspects. Journal of Psychological Research, 11, 1324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, M. W., & Horn, G. (1990). Are specific proteins implicated in the learning process of imprinting? Developmental Brain Research, 52, 294297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, D. W. (1992). Bench to bedside: The glutamate connection. Science, 258,241243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cotman, C. W., & Iversen, L. L. (1987). Excitatory amino acids in the brain - focus on NMDA receptors. Trends in Neuroscience, 10, 263265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1992). Neurology of memory. Current Opinion in Neurology and Neurosurgery, 5, 8387.Google ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, M. E., Richdale, A. L., & Ng, K. T. (1987). Effects of excess intracranial amino acids on memory: A behavioural survey. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews, 11, 331339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giuditta, A., Capano, C. P., D’Onofrio, G., Toniatti, C., Menna, T., & Hyden, H. (1986). Synthesis of rat brain DNA during acquisition of an appetitive task. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behaviour, 25,651658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, G. (1985). Memory, imprinting and the brain: An enquiry into mechanisms. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, G. (1990). Neural bases of recognition memory investigated through an analysis of imprinting. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B., 329,133142.Google ScholarPubMed
Horn, G., McCabe, B. J., & Cipolla-Neto, J. (1983). Imprinting in the domestic chick: The role of each side of the hyperstriatum ventrale in acquisition and retention. Experimental Brain Research, 53,9198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horn, G., Bradley, P., & McCabe, B. J. (1985). Changes in the structure of synapses associated with learning. Journal of Neuroscience,5,31613168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, A. N., Rogers, L. J., & Johnston, G. A.R. (1993). Glutamate and imprinting memory: The role of glutamate receptors in the encoding of imprinting memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 54,137143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, A. N., & Rogers, L. J. (1992). Glutamate receptor binding and memory formation in the chick. Proceedings of the Australian Neuroscience Society, 3,145.Google Scholar
Johnston, A. N., & Rogers, L. J. (1993). Glutamate mechanisms in early learning: Hemispheric differences. Proceedings of the Australian Neuroscience Society, 4,105.Google Scholar
Knudsen, G., Pettigrew, K. D., Patlak, C. S., Hertz, M. M., & Paulson, O. B. (1990). Asymmetrical transport of amino acids across the blood-brain barrier in humans. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 10,698706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lajtha, A. (1975). Alterations in the level and distribution of cerebral amino acids. In Tower, D. B. (Eds.), The nervous system. Vol. 1. The basic neurosciences. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Levi, G., & Morisi, G. (1971). Free amino acids and related compounds in chick brain during development. Brain Research, 26,131140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maragos, W. F., Greenamyre, J. T., Penney, J. B., & Young, A. B. (1987). Glutamate dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: An hypothesis. Trends in Neuroscience, 10,6568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, B.J., & Horn, G. (1988). Learning and memory: Regional changes in N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in the chick brain after imprinting. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 85,28492853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, K. D., Keller, J. B., & Stryker, M. P. (1989). Ocular dominance column development: Analysis and stimulation. Science, 241, 605615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, R. G. M., Anderson, E., Lynch, G. S., & Baudry, M. (1986). Selective impairment of learning and blockadeof long-term potentiation byan N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5. Nature, 319,774776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olney, J. W. (1969). Brain lesions, obesity and other disturbances in mice treated with monosodium glutamate. Science, 164,119121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perkins, A. T., & Teyler, T. J. (1988). A critical period for long-term potentiation in the developing rat visual cortex. Brain Research, 439,222229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, P. J., & Johnston, G. A. R. (1981). Glutamate: Transmitter in the CNS. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Robinson, M. B., & Coyle, J. T. (1987). Glutamate and related acidic excitatory neurotransmitters: From basic science to clinical application. Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology, 1,446455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, L. J. (1987). Free glutamate and aspartate in the diet: Effects on the development of brain and behaviour. Recent advances in animal nutrition in Australia. Annidale: UNE Press.Google Scholar
Rose, S. P. R. (1991). How chicks make memories: The cellular cascade from c-fos to dendritic remodelling. Trends in Neuroscience, 14,390397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salzen, E. A., Parker, D. M., & Williamson, A. J. (1976). A forebrain lesion preventing imprinting in domestic chicks. Experimental Brain Research, 24,145157.Google Scholar
Schmidt, W. J., Bubser, M., & Hauber, W. (1990). Excitatory amino acids and Parkinson’s disease. Trends in Neuroscience, 13,46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, G. R., Zorumski, C. F., Price, M. T., & Olney, J. W. (1990). Domoic acid: A dementia-inducing excito toxic food poison with kanic acid receptor specificity. Experimental Neurology, 110,127138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, M. G., Bourne, R. C., & Steele, R. J. (1992). Quantitative autoradiographic demonstration of changes in binding to NMDA-sensitive [3H]glutamate and [3H]MK801, but not [3HJAMPA receptors in the chick forebrain 30 min after passive avoidance training. European Journal of Neuroscience, 4,936943.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welker, E., Rao, S. B., Dörfl, J., Melzer, P. & Van der Loos, H. (1992). Plasticity in the barrel cortex of adult mouse: Effects of chronic stimulation upon deoxyglucose uptake in the behaving animal. The Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 153170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed