Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T07:09:50.776Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Controllability, information processing, and learning motivation: Moderation of working memory by perceptions of control

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

David A. Yeigh*
Affiliation:
Southern Cross University
*
School of Education, Southern Cross University, P.O. Box 157, LISMORE NSW 2480, Phone: 61 2 6620 3659, E-mail: [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

This study investigated the effects of perceived controllobility on information processing within the attributional model of learning (Weiner, 1985, 1986). Attributional style was used to identify trait patterns of controllability for 37 university student. Task-relevant feedback was then manipulated to test for differences in working memory function between participants with high versus low levels of trait controllobility. Trait controllability occurred differently for hi-trait and lo-trait types. Results supported the hypothesis that it exerts a moderating effect on the way task-relevant feedback is processed. This selective encoding of information appeared to involve limitations inherent to the working memory system that affect processing efficiency, marking an important consideration for the way in which information is presented during the learning process.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasedale, J.D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 4974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, C.A. (1991). How people think about causes: Examination of the typical phenomenal organisation of attributions for success and failure. Social Cognition, 9, 295329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C.A., & Rigor, A.L. (1991). A controllability attributional model of problems in living: Dimensional and situational interactions in the prediction of depression and loneliness. Social Cognition, 9, 149181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, R.C., & Shiffrin, R.M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K.W. & Spence, J.T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 276311). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G. (1977). Recency re-examined. In Domic, S. (Ed.), Attention and performance 6 (pp. 646667). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodner, E., & Mikulincer, M. (1998). Uncontrollable failure, learned helplessness, and paranoid-like responses: The role of attentional focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 10101023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J.D., & Siegel, J.M. (1988). Attributions for negative life events and depression: The role of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 316322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, P.G., Hamilton, D.L., & Ostrom, T.M. (1994). Social cognition: Impact on social psychology (pp. 78102). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fiske, S.T., & Taylor, S.E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd Ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D.T., Fiske, S.T., & Lindzey, G. (1988). The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gravetter, F.J., & Wallnau, L.B. (1996). Statistics for the behavioral sciences (4th ed.). Minneapolis: West PublishingGoogle Scholar
Hastie, R. (1984). Causes and effects of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 4456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M.K., Hashtroudi, S. & Lindsay, S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, E.E., & Nisbett, R.E. (1972). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Just, M.A., & Carpenter, P.A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Lefcourt, H.M. (1982). Locus of control: Current trends in theory research (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Markus, H. & Zajonc, R.B. (1985). The cognitive perspective in social psychology. In Lindsey, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 137230). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Martin, D.S. (1995). The causal attributions of young car drivers. In Kenny, D.T. & Job, R.F.S. (Eds.), Australia’s Adolescents: A health psychology perspective (pp. 139143). Annidale, NSW: University of New England Press.Google Scholar
McInerney, D.M., & McInerney, V. (2002). Educational psychology: Constructing learning (3rd ed.). Sydney: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Martinez, J.C. (1994). Perceived control and feedback in judgement and memory. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 374381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, C.P., Kane, M.J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 3554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P.M., & Seiffert, A.E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105, 203229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moeller, J., & Koeller, O. (1999). Spontaneous cognitions following academic test results. Journal of Experimental Education, 67, 150164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nesdale, A.R. (1983). Effects of person and situation expectations on explanation seeking and causal attributions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 9199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ostell, A., & Divers, P. (1987). Attributional style, unemployment, and mental health. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 60, 333337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pashler, H. (1997). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Passolunghi, M.C., Cornoldi, C., & De Liberto, S. (1999). Working memory and intrusions of irrelevant information in a group of specific poor problem solvers. Memory & Cognition, 27, 779790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & Villanova, P. (1988). An expanded attributional style questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 8789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child. London: Routledge & Keagan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1977). Epistemology and psychology of functions. Dordrecht: D. Reidal Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pintrich, P., & Schunk, D. (1996). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Merrill-Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Pittman, T.S., & D’Agostino, P.R. (1985). Motivation and attribution: The effects of control deprivation of subsequent information processing. In Harvey, J.H. & Weary, G. (Eds.), Attribution: Basic issues and applications (pp. 117142). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Porter, L. (2000). Student behaviour: Theory and practice for teachers. St. Leonards, NSW: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Reisberg, D. (1997). Cognition: Exploring the science of the mind. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Richardson, J.T.E., Engle, R.W., Hasher, L., Logie, R.H., Stoltzftis, E.R., & Zacks, R.T. (1996). Working memory and human cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rotter, J.B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (1, Whole No. 609).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seligman, M.E.P., Abramson, L.Y., Semmel, A., & VonBaeyer, C. (1979). Depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 242247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stillings, N.A., Weisler, S.E., Chase, C.H., Feinstein, M.H., Garfield, J.L., & Rissland, E.L. (1995), Cognitive science: An Introduction (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, S.E., & Brown, J.D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, M., Kaslow, N.J., Weiss, B., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Children’s attributional style questionnaire-revised: Psychometric examination. Psychological Assessment, 10, 166170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webb, W.M., Worchel, S., & Brown, E.H. (1986). The influence of control on self-attributions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 49, 260267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, B. (1974). Achievement motivation and attribution theory. Morristown, N. J.: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1980). Human motivation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985). Spontaneous causal thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 7484.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1986). Human motivation: Metaphors, theories, and research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Winter, L., Uleman, J.S., & Cunniff, C. (1985). How automatic are social judgements? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 904917.Google Scholar
Wong, P.T.P., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask “why” questions, and the heuristics of attributional search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wormian, C.B., Panciera, L., Shusterman, L., & Hibscher, J. (1976). Attributions of causality and reactions to uncontrollable outcomes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 301316.Google Scholar