Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:21:53.270Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is Overtriage Associated With Increased Mortality? The Evidence Says “Yes”

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2013

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Invited Editorial Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Disaster Medicine and Public Health, Inc. 2008

Things should be made as simple as possible, but not any simpler.

—Albert Einstein

Research in disaster medicine in general, and triage in particular, is largely descriptive and conducted after the fact because disasters do not lend themselves to study via randomized controlled trials. A useful alternative involves modeling and analysis based on parameters obtained from real disasters. Triage is particularly appropriate for this kind of analysis because it involves a relatively distinct problem space, with discrete decisions made at well-defined points in time. Defining and varying the inputs into particular triage schemes should therefore permit model predictions across a variety of scenarios. In particular, the effects of overtriage, or labeling noncritical casualties as critical, could be demonstrated relative to critical mortality, or the mortality of critical casualties with an Injury Severity Score (ISS) of >15. The results of this modeling could then be applied to planning and practice.

Examining the effect of overtriage on critical mortality, Hupert and colleagues have modeled triage as a diagnostic test that gives a result of critical or noncritical to casualties in sequence; this result then leads to prioritized resource utilization.Reference Hupert, Hollingsworth and Xiong1 The model emphasizes trauma system capability, rather than individual hospital capability. Modeling triage in this way allowed the authors to vary not only triage sensitivity but also the prevalence of critical casualties and hence to examine the effects of overtriage across a range of hypothetical casualty loads.

Using this modeling approach, Hupert et al claim to demonstrate that overtriage had minimal effect on critical mortality. Variation in the prevalence of critical casualties showed, not surprisingly, that critical mortality increased when higher overtriage resulted from adding more noncritical casualties to the case mix, and that critical mortality decreased when there were fewer critical casualties. Treatment capability had a larger effect on outcomes than triage performance.

When trying to assess the usefulness of a model, however, a key question is whether its structure approximates or reflects the clinical reality. Simulation using modeling is the imitation of a real-world process that can lead to inferences about the operating characteristics of the real system. As such, the fidelity and validity of the model are dependent upon the accuracy of the underlying assumptions and logic. The model of Hupert et al deviates from the real-life experience of trauma systems dealing with mass casualty incidents in three important respects: it assumes an orderly sequential flow of casualties, which almost never occurs in real mass casualty incidentsReference Auf der Heide2; it links the trauma bay in the emergency department with an operating room, even though only a small minority of critical casualties in a real MCI require surgeryReference Gutierrez de Ceballos, Turegano-Fuentes and Perez-Diaz3; and it uses the START triage algorithm that is mostly applicable to field situations and almost irrelevant to triage at the hospital door.

The model does, in fact, show a positive correlation between overtriage and critical mortality when the number of noncritical casualties increases. This observation is compatible with reported experience. Evidence derived from urban terrorism during the past 40 years clearly shows that overtriage is directly related to critical mortality (Fig 1). More recent urban disasters show better tolerance of overtriage, compared with earlier or rural events, but at some inevitable point, the system is overwhelmed, and the mortality of people who are critically injured rises regardless of the setting. The factors responsible for this apparent improvement in tolerating the adverse effects of overtriage are unclear. Nevertheless, the message emerging from analysis of real-life data, as opposed to theoretical models, remains that at some point, the ability to provide optimal care will be overcome by the casualty load. Noncritical casualties who are erroneously sorted as critical increase the “critical casualty” workload while competing for limited trauma-care resources, assets, and expertise with genuine critical casualties.

FIGURE 1 Relationship of overtriage rate to critical mortality rate in 10 (1969–1995) vs 4 (2001–2007) terrorist events. Note shift of curve to left.

An important aspect of any scientific discussion of triage is that the sorting process does not involve a single decision point but rather a series of sequential decisions. This aspect of triage was ignored by Hupert et al in their model. A single triage decision is unreliable, even when made by experienced physicians.Reference Ashkenazi, Kessel and Khashan4 Triage accuracy comes from repetition and reiteration of the triage process as the casualty is moved between service points in the hospital. This redundancy permits a greater tolerance of triage errors as they are caught and corrected at successive levels of care.

Is this kind of modeling study helpful? Yes. Despite some obvious limitations, the article generates a fruitful discussion surrounding a core question in disaster medicine: how does overtriage influence critical mortality? It also emphasizes more broadly the critical role that triage plays in determining outcome, and therefore, the importance of intensive training in triage among medical providers who may be confronted by the unique challenges of mass casualty care. In analyzing its flaws, we are reminded that “planning should take into consideration how people and organizations are likely to act, rather than expecting them to change their behavior to conform to the plan.”Reference Quarantelli5 In essence, the article becomes a call for more research in triage, focused on an insightful combination of theoretical modeling with evidence from real-life experience.

Authors’ Disclosures

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

References

REFERENCES

1.Hupert, N, Hollingsworth, E, Xiong, W.Is overtriage associated with increased mortality? Insights from a simulation model of mass casualty trauma care, Disaster Med Public Health Preparedness. 2007;1:S14S24.Google Scholar
2.Auf der Heide, E.The importance of evidence-based disaster planning. Ann Emerg Med. 2006;47:3449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Gutierrez de Ceballos, JP, Turegano-Fuentes, F, Perez-Diaz, Det alMarch 2004: the terrorist bomb explosions in Madrid, Spain—an analysis of the logistics, injuries sustained and clinical management of casualties treated at the closest hospital. Crit Care. 2005;9:104111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4.Ashkenazi, I, Kessel, B, Khashan, Tet alPrecision of in-hospital triage in mass-casualty incidents after terror attacks. Prehosp Disaster Med. 2006;21:2023.Google Scholar
5.Quarantelli, EL.Organization Behavior in Disasters and Implications for Disaster Planning, Report series 18. Newark, DE: Disaster Research Center, University of Delaware 1985 .Google Scholar
Figure 0

FIGURE 1 Relationship of overtriage rate to critical mortality rate in 10 (1969–1995) vs 4 (2001–2007) terrorist events. Note shift of curve to left.