Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T18:20:41.389Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Changing Trends of Excess Self-protective Behavior, and Association with Belief in Prevention Myths During the COVID-19 Epidemic in China: A Panel Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 November 2024

Sihui Peng
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, Jinan University, Guangzhou, China
Tingzhong Yang*
Affiliation:
Yongkang Women and Children’s Health Hospital, Yongkang, China/Center for Tobacco Control Research, Zhejiang University, School of Medicine, Hangzhou, China Injury Control Research Center, West Virginia University, Morgantown, USA
Ian R.H. Rockett
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, West Virginia University School of Public Health, Morgantown, West Virginia, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York, USA
*
Corresponding author: Tingzhong Yang; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This prospective observational study examined changing trends of excess self-protective behavior (EPB), and their association with perceived risk, perceived severity, and irrational beliefs about prevention during the Chinese COVID-19 epidemic. Participants were recruited for an online panel survey. There are 150 participants for the baseline and 102 for the final survey. There were 5 waves of interviews. Perceptions of risk and disease severity were measured by single items. Irrational beliefs about prevention and EPB comprised 5 common prevention misconceptions. Descriptive statistics and the CATMOD program were used for data analysis. The prevalence of participants perceiving personal risk of contracting COVID-19 and severe consequences of the disease was 18.6% and 25.5%, respectively, at baseline, and declining to 4.9% and 17.6% at final observation. The 5 selected EPB also showed a diminishing trend. Belief in COVID-19 prevention myths trended upwards. Perceived risk was positively associated with each EPB, and perceived severity with disinfection of clothes and hoarding of products. Myth adherence was positively associated with disinfection of clothes and both hand washing and sanitization. This study yields new information about EPB among the Chinese public. Policy modifications and public education interventions are essential for minimizing the adverse health effects of subscribing to irrational beliefs.

Type
Original Research
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Society for Disaster Medicine and Public Health, Inc.

Following the initial Wuhan outbreak in Hubei province, China, COVID-19 has rapidly diffused into a global pandemic. 1 Given the salience of human psychological and behavioral factors to disease prevention and mitigation, it is crucial to evaluate their role in propagating or impeding behavioral responses.Reference Bish and Michie 2 , Reference Wise, Zbozinek and Michelini 3 Many studies reported on mental and behavioral responses during an outbreak of an acute respiratory infection.Reference Li, Yang and Dou 4 Reference LV and Yang 6 People quarantined at home or at another location may have experienced boredom, anger, and loneliness, which in turn elicited a personal behavioral response.Reference Severance, Dickerson and Viscidi 7 , Reference Yang 8 COVID-19 is a new disease, and the 2019 outbreak in Wuhan and elsewhere in China may have been stressful. An epidemic of a highly lethal disease can overwhelm people emotionally and physically and induce strong mental and behavioral responses in both adults and children.Reference Lunn, Belton and Lavin 5 , Reference Cullen, Gulati and Kelly 9 , Reference Greenberg, Docherty and Gnanapragasam 10

According to the Stimulus, Cognition and Response (SCR) model, various stimuli (S) affect internal states of people through cognition (C), which in turn elicits mental and behavioral responses (R).Reference Yang 8 , Reference Mehrabian and Russell 11 COVID-19 is a potent stimulus that plausibly induces people to perceive high risk of infection with potentially severe health consequences. Health belief theory proposes such perceptions may generate a behavioral response.Reference Yang 8 Some studies found an association between strong risk and threat perceptions and excess mental and behavioral problems during outbreaks of the severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) and Ebola.Reference Dorfan and Woody 12 Reference Yang and Chu 14 A recent study revealed that individuals’ perceived severity of the COVID-19 epidemic was related to undesirable emotional and behavioral outcomes among the Chinese public.Reference Li, Yang and Dou 4 Perceived risk and severity of COVID-19 may also induce irrational beliefs about its prevention.Reference Rabalais 15 , Reference Ellis, David and Lynn 16 Rational action theory proposes that all action is fundamentally “rational” in terms of what the actor believes to be true, but in his or her practice comprises irrational as well as rational elements.Reference Jaeger, Webler and Rosa 17 Irrational beliefs are rigid, inaccurate, and illogical, but are used defensively to process external events. Unlikely to find empirical support, these beliefs are self-defeating, unconditional, and in conflict with reality.Reference Bridges and Harnish 18 Many studies found that that the more irrational the belief, the more negative the health behaviors.Reference Rabalais 15 , Reference Bridges and Harnish 18 , Reference Kaygusuz 19 Irrational beliefs commonly manifest in many social and health arenas.Reference Yang 8

Survival is a biological imperative for humans, and self-protection is the common behavioral response for confronting a mortal crisis. In sudden major crises, coping can induce excess self-protective behavior (EPB). COVID-19’s evolution on the world scene has not been paralleled by any other communicable disease since the 1918/19 Spanish Flu pandemic; a catastrophic phenomenon that killed an estimated 50-100 million people globally.Reference Bratspies 20 The dire threat to personal health and survival, posed by the COVID-19 pandemic, has quickly promoted perceptions of high risk for infection with potentially severe outcomes. In turn, these responses can be a strong stimulus for EPB. Meanwhile, EPB may be disproportionate and not recommended as an effective response to the actual threat; thus, overburdening individuals and society and sparking diversion of scarce critical resources away from places where need for assistance is most acute.Reference Cullen, Gulati and Kelly 9 In cognitive science, EPB emanates from people’s fear and distorted view of the world.Reference Yang 8 Irrational beliefs or subscription to myths about COVID-19 prevention measures could stimulate pervasive EPB. This type of behavior consumes a high degree of personal physical energy, and in the process diminishes disease immunity. Excessive protective measures can overstress health care facilities and other resources, and consequently exert a strong negative impact upon the economy and society as a whole.Reference Garfin, Silver and Holman 21 For example, panic buying of essential consumer items like toilet paper, first aid kits, bottled water, and hand sanitizer, in response to COVID-19, has led to global shortages and price gouging of consumer staples.Reference Garfin, Silver and Holman 21 This response impedes disease prevention and economic recovery. However, this issue has generated little discussion and debate,Reference Garfin, Silver and Holman 21 and no previous empirical studies have addressed EPB in relation to COVID-19 or other acute infectious respiratory diseases.

This study has three key objectives:

  1. 1. To examine levels of EPB by the Chinese amid the COVID-19 epidemic.

  2. 2. To evaluate temporal trends in EPB during that epidemic.

  3. 3. To study the association between perceived risk and severity of COVID-19, and belief in related prevention myths, respectively, and EPB.

This study may yield information important in formulating policy and health education initiatives aimed at reducing EPB, with the goal of improving the design and targeting of effective interventions for preventing and mitigating COVID-19.

Methods

Study Design

We conducted a prospective longitudinal observation study to examine temporal trends and changes in EPB, and its associations with selected perceptions and beliefs during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Participants

Participants were recruited via a survey advertisement from social media groups on WeChat and Douban, two of the most popular social media platforms in China. Inclusion criteria were membership in a common community; being in the age group 20-60 years; having access to a Smartphone; knowing the Chinese language; and being willing to participate in the panel study and provide follow-up information at the scheduled observation points. Participants were excluded if they refused to provide this information or had a medical condition that could limit or preclude their participation. Within the registration system, potential participants were screened to ascertain eligibility. Upon consent, participants received an electronic questionnaire and instructions on how to proceed. After reading the instructions, they were asked to provide an e-consent by tapping the “Confirmation and Authorization” button and were then directed to the questionnaire. A special administrative WeChat group was established to manage the follow-up data collection, using a unique QR code for each respondent. The QR code was the vehicle not only for identifying unique participants but prohibiting non-participants from taking the survey. After scanning the QR code, survey participants could enter the investigation group without further preconditions.

This panel study analyzed 5 waves of data collected over 1 month: Wave 1(5/Feb/2020), Wave 2(12/Feb/2020), Wave 3(19/Feb/2020), Wave 4 (26/Feb/2020), and Wave 5(4/March/2020). The entire observation period covered the peak and trough of the COVID-19 epidemic in China. The corresponding number of reported confirmed patients was 3887, 2015, 394, 433, and 133 in each wave, respectively. 25

Data Collection

An online survey was implemented on Wenjuanxing (www.wjx.cn), a survey service website similar to Qualtrics or SurveyMonkey, but tailored to Chinese users. Each wave of the survey had a dedicated electronic questionnaire access link. The online questionnaire link was posted to the respondent group, centrally managed in a WeChat group, and accessible every Wednesday from 10:30 a.m. to 4:30 p.m. In cases where participants missed their survey appointment, follow-up procedures were initiated by data collectors. Participants who did not respond by 11:00 a.m. the following Monday were classified as lost to follow-up. Data collectors and facilitators were third-year doctoral students in a public health program. All responses were anonymous. The questionnaire took approximately 10 minutes to complete, and the same survey protocol was used for every wave of the survey to assure homogeneity of data administration and collection. As appropriate, a token of appreciation, a total of 30 RMB was given to those participants who completed all 5questionnaires.

Measurement

In this study, basic individual demographic characteristics were tapped: age, gender, ethnicity, education level, marital status and occupation. Perceptions of risk and disease severity were respectively captured through the items “continual fear of infection by COVID-19” and “becoming infected by COVID-19 is a serious misfortune.”

This study addressed personal harboring of myths or irrational beliefs about effective COVID-19 prevention measures that were not founded upon reality and science. They reflected 5 common misconceptions that circulated during the COVID-19 epidemic in China: (1) Smokers are not susceptible to COVID-19, (2) Consuming alcohol can prevent the spread of the virus, (3) People should avoid people from Hubei province, where COVID-19 first manifested in China, in order to prevent contraction of the disease, (4) It is reasonable for employers to dismiss Hubei employees to prevent the spread of the novel virus, and (5) People who move away from an affected area should be deported back to their place of origin. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale, which ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Item scores were summed to attain a total score for belief in COVID-19 prevention myths. The higher that score, the greater the level of irrational prevention belief. The Cronbach’s α coefficient was 0·70, suggesting the questionnaire had acceptable reliability. Consistent with prior practices, a cutoff score of 15 or above signified strong acceptance or belief in prevention myths.Reference Yang 8 EPB was a common coping mechanism for confronting the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Currently, there is no protocol for determining EPB in this situation. We identified them empirically from social norms perspective.Reference Yang 8 Through an online survey, study participants stated which of 16 types of behavior, aimed at preventing COVID-19, they would categorize as EPB. Complete responses were obtained from 116 participants. Where there was 80% agreement on a type among these participants, we preliminarily categorized it as EPB. These selections were then reviewed and approved by 12 health experts, and the 5 that received universal acceptance by the participants and experts were classified as EPB. They included in this study: (1) Clothing disinfected every 1 or 2 days (Disinfecting clothes), (2) Frequent hand washing beyond the regular washing before and after meals or after work (Washing hands), (3) Frequent use of a disinfectant when washing hands at home (Sanitizing hands), (4) Hoarding masks, alcohol, and other forms of protective products (Hoarding products), and (5) Making arrangements for another or others to handle their family and occupational responsibilities if, and when, they contracted COVID-19 (Transferring responsibilities).

Data Analysis

All data were entered into a database using Microsoft Excel. They were then imported into SAS (9.3 version) for statistical analysis. Across survey waves, descriptive statistics were calculated for belief in prevention myths, perceived high risk for contracting the disease, perceived high severity of disease consequences, and EPB prevalence. The CATMOD program was used to conduct repeated measures analysis of variance to determine changing trends across the 5 observation points, and to examine the association between perceived disease risk and severity, and belief in prevention myths, respectively, with EPB using the method of weighted least squares.Reference Inc 22

Results

One hundred-and-fifty participants were recruited at baseline. The baseline was linkable and there were 3 intermediate and a final observation point, with 102 participants available for analysis throughout; 99 came from 24 provinces located across China, differentiated by region. The remaining 3 were international.

Of the study sample, 61.8% were female and 93.3% were Han Chinese. The average age of participants was 39.1 years (SD: 12.5), 43%, were never married, and 50.0% were married. The prevalence of belief in COVID-19 prevention myths was higher among males than females (adjusted OR: 0.14) and increased with age (adjusted OR: 8.25, 12.56). Married people had a lower prevalence of high perceived risk (OR: 0.36), and the middle-aged (40-49 years) had a higher prevalence of perceived disease severity than comparison groups (OR: 1.86) (Table 1). Disinfection of clothes was less prevalent among females than males (adjusted OR: 0.34), and among professionals than people in other occupational groups (adjusted OR: 0.35). Hand washing was more prevalent among females than males (adjusted OR: 3.48) and less prevalent among the married than the never married (adjusted OR: 0.43). Hoarding products was more prevalent among the middle-aged (adjusted OR: 4.32), the least educated (adjusted OR: 0.32), and the married than their respective demographic counterparts (adjusted OR: 0.13) (Table 2).

Table 1. Sample characteristics and prevalence of prevention myth belief, perceived disease risk, and severity

* P<0.05

** P<0.01

Table 2. Sample characteristics and prevalence of excessive self-protective behavior

* P<0.05

** P<0.01

All participants knew the disease had attained epidemic proportions and was highly contagious at the time of the first survey was implemented. The prevalence at baseline of the perceptions of high risk of contracting COVID-19 and disease severity was 18.6% and 25.5%, respectively, and declined to 4.9% and 17.6% by the last observation point, with a statistically significant change. The prevalence of the 5 types of EPB showed a statistically significant downwards trend across the total observation period of this panel study. Simultaneously, there was a statistically significant upwards trend in belief in COVID-19 prevention myths (Table 3).

Table 3. Time change trend in prevention myth belief, perceived disease risk and severity, and excessive self-protective behavior

* P<0.05

** P<0.01

Perceived high risk for contracting COVID-19 was positively associated with each type of selected EPB, and perception that the disease had severe consequences was positively associated with disinfection of clothes and hoarding of products. Belief in the disease prevention myths was positively associated with disinfection of clothes and both hand washing and sanitization (Table 4).

Table 4. Association of perceived disease risk and severity and prevention myth belief with excessive self-protective behavior

* P<0.05

** P<0.01

Discussion

At baseline, this study found that 18.6% and 25.5% participants, respectively, believed they were at high risk of contracting COVID-19, and that this disease seriously threatened their health. Turning to previous studies, 1 found approximately 10-30% of the general public were very worried or moderately worried about the possibility of contracting influenza during an outbreak.Reference Rubin, Potts and Michie 23 Another reported that during the period February 1 through 10, when disease cases were increasing dramatically, 15.3% of Shanghai and Wuhan residents perceived COVID-19 as a very serious disease.Reference Qian, Wu and Wu 24 Our research commenced at the peak of the epidemic, and one-fifth to one-quarter of study participants perceived the disease was very serious and their risk of contraction was high. COVID-19 is a new disease, and the epidemic may profoundly impact people mentally and behaviorally. Viewed as a stimulus, this disease can be overwhelming and elicit strong mental and behavioral responses. Many studies have found that COVID-19, now a global pandemic, generates negative mental and behavioral outcomesReference Cullen, Gulati and Kelly 9 , Reference Greenberg, Docherty and Gnanapragasam 10 that may include inappropriate health-protective and help-seeking behaviors.Reference Garfin, Silver and Holman 21

Addressing a gap in the literature, this Chinese study found changing temporal trends in perceived high risk of contracting COVID-19, perception of severe adverse consequences of the disease, belief in prevention myths about the disease, and EPB during the epidemic. Prevalence of perceived risk and severity declined over the observation period, a trend consistent with the decline in the actual risk of infection. The new reported COVID-19 patients in China across the 5 observation points numbered 3887, 2015, 394, 433 and 133, respectively. 25 As COVID-19 was reaching epidemic proportions, there were increases in the prevalence of perceptions of personal risk for contracting the disease and the severity of its health consequences. The prevalence of the five types of EPB, highlighted in this study, showed a statistically significant decreasing trend over the observation period. EPB is typically considered a behavioral overreaction in China and increases with perceived disease risk and severity. Plausibly then, high perceived risk and severity induce EPB. Contrasting with our findings about the perception data, belief in COVID-19 prevention myths manifested a statistically significant increasing trend over the observation period. The explanation may inhere in an “energy consumption” mechanism.Reference Posner and Rothbart 26 People likely functioned rationally as they mobilized all their physical and mental energy to cope with COVID-19 at the beginning of the epidemic. However, as time passed, such energy waned and rational thinking diminished as belief in prevention myths became more common. This information is useful for formulating prevention policy and educational programming.

The risk of disease or injury and the severity of outcomes are crucial themes in individual health behavior. This study provided new evidence that perceived risk of contracting COVID-19 and perception of the severity of its consequences were both positively associated with several types of EPB, findings generally compatible with those from some other studies.Reference Wise, Zbozinek and Michelini 3 , Reference Li, Yang and Dou 4 , Reference Dorfan and Woody 12 We found a negative association between belief in prevention myths and some of the constituents of EPB. Affirmation for our findings, other investigators also found a relationship between such a belief and negative health behaviour.Reference Rabalais 15 , Reference Bridges and Harnish 18

EPB transcends normal self-protective behaviors, with special significance from a disease prevention perspective. For effective prevention, it is necessary both to avoid inadequate prevention measures that increase the likelihood of a disease epidemic, and to avoid excessive activities that waste personal and social resources. These two scenarios may vary across cultures. Inadequate prevention may be a prominent problem in Western culture and excessive prevention in Eastern culture.Reference Yang 8 , Reference Christopher Perry, Bekes and Starrs 27 , Reference Berg and Lin 28 A related dimension of such cultural variance is societal “rigidity” versus “porousness.”Reference Van Bavel, Baicker and Boggio 29 “Rigid” cultures, such as those of Singapore, Japan, and China, have strict social norms and punishment for deviance, whereas “porous” cultures, such as those characterizing the U.S., Italy, and Brazil, reflect weaker social norms and greater permissiveness.Reference Gelfand, Raver and Nishii 30 “Qǐ rén yōu tiān” from ancient China is a tale about a person who worried every day the sky would collapse. This study found that in the COVID-19 epidemic there was pervasive over-prevention among members of the public. Excessive prevention consumes too much personal energy and societal resources, and hence impedes disease control and economic recovery. Government and society at large must give this issue more attention. Reforms in health policy and health education will be essential for minimizing the adverse effects of belief in prevention myths and associated deleterious behavior.

There are two study limitations. Firstly, our sample size is small. Nevertheless, the sample originated from 24 provinces covering diverse regions and a wide array of demographic characteristics. Also, sample attrition may introduce a “cluster” bias because many longitudinal studies likely over-represent some of these characteristics, such as high educational attainment. A more sophisticated design and representative sample would be necessary to resolve this problem. A second limitation in this study is the lack of a clear definition of EPB. We operationalized this concept through empirically identifying 5 constituents from a social norms perspective. Operationalization of the concept of belief in prevention myths may so be thought as an external criterion for measuring the validity of EPB.Reference Yang 8 , Reference Rabalais 15 , Reference Kaygusuz 19 Belief in prevention myths was significantly associated with 3 of the 5 types of EPB in this study. This finding enhances the validity of our measure of EPB. The concept and its operationalization require further research.

Conclusion

This study provides new information on the relationship between belief in COVID-19 prevention myths and EPB among the Chinese public, in the context of perceived risk of contracting the disease and perception of the severity of its consequences during the epidemic. As the virus spreads relentlessly around the globe, our findings could guide similar research outside China in less and more developed countries. They harbor important implications for understanding and decreasing EPB, as appropriate, during this new global pandemic.

Data availability statement

Data are available on reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Author contribution

Tingzhong Yang: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing- Original draft preparation; Sihui Peng: Investigation, Resource, Writing - Review & Editing, Data curation, Supervision; Ian R.H. Rockett: Writing - Review & Editing.

Competing interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Funding statement

This study was supported, in part, by the National Nature Science Foundation of China (Major Project 71490733).

Ethical standard

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation the Ethics Committee at the Medical Center, Zhejiang University (No.2014:1-017) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000 (5). Verbal consent was obtained from all participants for being included in the study.

All participants received an electronic questionnaire and instructions on how to proceed. After reading the instructions, they were asked to provide an e-consent by tapping the “Confirmation and Authorization” button and then directed to the questionnaire.

References

World Health Organization. Mental Health and Psychosocial Considerations During the COVID-19 Outbreak, 18 March 2020. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2020.Google Scholar
Bish, A, Michie, S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pandemic: a review. Br J Health Psychol. 2010;15(4):797824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, T, Zbozinek, TD, Michelini, G, et al. Changes in risk perception and protective behavior during the first week of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. R Soc Open Sci. 2020;7(9):200742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, JB, Yang, A, Dou, K, et al. Chinese public’s knowledge, perceived severity, and perceived controllability of COVID-19 and their associations with emotional and behavioural reactions, social participation, and precautionary behaviour: a national survey. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lunn, PD, Belton, CA, Lavin, C, et al. Using behavioral science to help fight the Coronavirus. J Behav Public Admin. 2020;3(1):115.Google Scholar
LV, S TB, Yang, T. Study on health-related behavior during and after the Sars epidemic. Chinese J Public Health. 2008;24:960961.Google Scholar
Severance, EG, Dickerson, FB, Viscidi, RP, et al. Coronavirus immunoreactivity in individuals with a recent onset of psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(1):101107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, T. Health research: Social and Behavioral Theory and Methods. Beijing: People’s Medical Publishing House; 2018.Google Scholar
Cullen, W, Gulati, G, Kelly, B. Mental health in the COVID-19 pandemic. QJM: Int J Med. 2020;113(5):311312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, N, Docherty, M, Gnanapragasam, S, et al. Managing mental health challenges faced by healthcare workers during covid-19 pandemic. BMJ. 2020;368:m1211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehrabian, A, Russell, JA. An Approach to Environmental Psychology Cambridge. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press; 1974.Google Scholar
Dorfan, NM, Woody, SR Danger appraisals as prospective predictors of disgust and avoidance of contaminants. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2011;30(2):105132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vartti, AM, Oenema, A, Schreck, M, et al. SARS knowledge, perceptions, and behaviors: a comparison between Finns and the Dutch during the SARS outbreak in 2003. Int J Behav Med. 2009;16(1):41-48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, JZ, Chu, H. Who is afraid of the Ebola outbreak? The influence of discrete emotions on risk perception. J Risk Res. 2018;21(7):834853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabalais, TL. Understanding the Relationship of Stress, Irrational Health Beliefs, and Health Behaviors Among Adults 18-45 Years of Age. Walden University; 2015.Google Scholar
Ellis, A, David, D, Lynn, SJ. Rational and irrational beliefs: a historical and conceptual perspective. In: Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Research, Theory, and Clinical Practice. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2010:322.Google Scholar
Jaeger, CC, Webler, T, Rosa, EA, et al. Risk, Uncertainty and Rational Action. British: Taylor & Francis; 2013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridges, K, Harnish, R. Irrational Beliefs. Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Hoboken, NY. In: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2010.Google Scholar
Kaygusuz, C. Irrational Beliefs and Abuse in University Students’ Romantic Relations. Eurasian J Educ Res. 2013;51:141155.Google Scholar
Bratspies, R. This great catastrophe: bungling pandemics from 1918 to today. Mich State Int Law Rev. 2022;30(2):189.Google Scholar
Garfin, DR, Silver, RC, Holman, EA. The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. 2020;39(5):355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inc, SI. Base SAS® 9.3 Procedures Guide: Statistical Procedures. New York: SAS Institute Inc.; 2011.Google Scholar
Rubin, GJ, Potts, HW, Michie, S. The impact of communications about swine flu (influenza A H1N1v) on public responses to the outbreak: results from 36 national telephone surveys in the UK. Health Technol Assess. 2010;14(34):183266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qian, M, Wu, Q, Wu, P, et al. Psychological responses, behavioral changes and public perceptions during the early phase of the COVID-19 outbreak in China: a population based cross-sectional survey. MedRxiv. 2020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Health Commission of People’s Republic of China. Epidemic Situation. 2020; Accessed April 23, 2020. http://www.nhc.gov.cn/xcs/yqfkdt/gzbd_index.shtml.Google Scholar
Posner, MI, Rothbart, MK. The Concept of Energy in Psychological Theory. In: Energetics and Human Information Processing. Springer; 1986:2340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christopher Perry, J, Bekes, V, Starrs, CJ. A systematic survey of adults’ health-protective behavior use during early COVID-19 pandemic in Canada, Germany, United Kingdom, and the United States, and vaccination hesitancy and status eight months later. Prev Med Rep. 2022;30:102013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, MB, Lin, L. Prevalence and predictors of early COVID-19 behavioral intentions in the United States. Transl Behav Med. 2020;10(4):843849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Bavel, JJ, Baicker, K, Boggio, PS, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4(5):460471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, MJ, Raver, JL, Nishii, L, et al. Differences between tight and loose cultures: a 33-nation study. Science. 2011;332(6033):11001104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics and prevalence of prevention myth belief, perceived disease risk, and severity

Figure 1

Table 2. Sample characteristics and prevalence of excessive self-protective behavior

Figure 2

Table 3. Time change trend in prevention myth belief, perceived disease risk and severity, and excessive self-protective behavior

Figure 3

Table 4. Association of perceived disease risk and severity and prevention myth belief with excessive self-protective behavior