Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T01:36:47.302Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Trajectories of child externalizing problems between ages 3 and 10 years: Contributions of children's early effortful control, theory of mind, and parenting experiences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 March 2017

Sheryl L. Olson*
Affiliation:
University of Michigan
Daniel Ewon Choe
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
Arnold J. Sameroff
Affiliation:
University of Michigan
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Sheryl L. Olson, Department of Psychology, 2270 East Hall, University of Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1043; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Preventing problem behavior requires an understanding of earlier factors that are amenable to intervention. The main goals of our prospective longitudinal study were to trace trajectories of child externalizing behavior between ages 3 and 10 years, and to identify patterns of developmentally significant child and parenting risk factors that differentiated pathways of problem behavior. Participants were 218 3-year-old boys and girls who were reassessed following the transition to kindergarten (age 5–6 years) and during the late school-age years (age 10). Mothers contributed ratings of children's externalizing behavior at all three time points. Children's self-regulation abilities and theory of mind were assessed during a laboratory visit, and parenting risk (frequent corporal punishment and low maternal warmth) was assessed using interview-based and questionnaire measures. Four developmental trajectories of externalizing behavior yielded the best balance of parsimony and fit with our longitudinal data and latent class growth analysis. Most young children followed a pathway marked by relatively low levels of symptoms that continued to decrease across the school-age years. Atypical trajectories marked chronically high, increasing, and decreasing levels of externalizing problems across early and middle childhood. Three-year-old children with low levels of effortful control were far more likely to show the chronic pattern of elevated externalizing problems than changing or low patterns. Early parental corporal punishment and maternal warmth, respectively, differentiated preschoolers who showed increasing and decreasing patterns of problem behavior compared to the majority of children. The fact that children's poor effortful regulation skills predicted chronic early onset problems reinforces the need for early childhood screening and intervention services.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

This research was supported by Grant RO1MH57489 from the National Institute of Mental Health (to S.O. and A.S.). We are very grateful to the children, parents, and teachers who generously shared their time with us and to the many individuals who helped with data collection and coding. We also thank the administrators of the University of Michigan Children's Center for their invaluable assistance.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/2–3 and 1992 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Alink, L. R. A., Mesman, J., van Zeijl, J., Stolk, M. N., Juffer, F., Koot, H., … van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2006). The early childhood aggression curve: Development of physical aggression in 10- to 50-month-old children. Child Development, 77, 954966.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. M. (1989). Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60, 946964. doi:10.2307/1131035.Google Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (2007). On tests and indices for evaluating structural models. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 825829.Google Scholar
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning. Child Development, 81, 326339.Google Scholar
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Dodge, K. A., … Vitaro, F. (2003). Developmental trajectories of childhood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site, cross-national study. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222245.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., & Dedmon, S. E. (2000). Physiological and behavioral regulation in two-year-old children with aggressive/destructive behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 103118.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. B., Spieker, S., Burchinal, M., Poe, M., & NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2006). Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood to age 9 predict academic and social functioning through age 12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 791800.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. B., Spieker, S., Vandergrift, N., Belsky, J., Burchinal, M., & NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2010). Predictors and sequelae of trajectories of physical aggression in school age boys and girls. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 133150. doi:10.1017/S0954579409990319.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology, 40, 11051122. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 10321053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333.Google Scholar
Chang, H., Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., & Sexton, H. R. (2011). Child effortful control as a mediator of parenting practices on externalizing behavior: Evidence for a sex-differentiated pathway across the transition from preschool to school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 7181. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9437-7.Google Scholar
Choe, D. E., Lane, J. D., Grabell, A. S., & Olson, S. L. (2013). Developmental precursors of young school-age children's hostile attribution bias. Developmental Psychology, 49, 22452256. doi:10.1037/a0032293.Google Scholar
Choe, D. E., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2013a). Effects of early maternal distress on parenting and the development of children's self-regulation and externalizing behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 437453. doi:10.1037/t00789-000.Google Scholar
Choe, D. E., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2013b). The interplay of externalizing problems and inductive and physical discipline during childhood. Developmental Psychology, 49, 20292039. doi:10.1037/a 003 2054.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (2006). Developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and method (2nd ed., pp. 123). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Combs-Ronto, L., Olson, S. L., Lunkenheimer, E. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (2009). Interactions between maternal parenting and children's early disruptive behavior: Bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 11511163. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9332-2.Google Scholar
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 13871388.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39, 349371.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization mediators of the relation between socioeconomic status and child conduct problems. Child Development, 65, 649665.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other peoples’ feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 13521366.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Spinrad, T., Cumberland, A., Liwe, J., Reiser, M., … Losoya, S. H. (2009). Longitudinal relations of children's effortful control, impulsivity, and negative emotionality to their externalizing, internalizing, and co-occurring behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 45, 9881008.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children's effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76, 10551071.Google Scholar
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223231.Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2013). Childhood self-control and adult outcomes: Results from a 30-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 709717. doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.008.Google Scholar
Gardner, F. E. M. (1994). The quality of joint activity between mothers and their children with behaviour problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 935948.Google Scholar
Hollingshead, A. A. (1979). Four Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat? Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in “hard-to-manage” preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 981994. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00401.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2006). Behavioral problems in two-year-olds: Links with individual differences in theory of mind, executive function, and negative parenting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 488497.Google Scholar
Hyde, L. W., Waller, R., Trentacosta, C. J., Shaw, D. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., Ganiban, J. M., … Leve, L. D. (2016). Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. American Journal of Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111381.Google Scholar
Jung, T., & Wickrama, K. A. S. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 302317. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.x.Google Scholar
Keenan, K., Boeldt, D., Chen, D., Coyne, C., Donald, R., Duax, J., … Hill, C. (2011). Predictive validity of DSM-IV oppositional defiant and conduct disorders in clinically referred preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 4755.Google Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., & Vandegeest, K. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490507. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01747.x.Google Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Keenan, K., Rathouz, P. J., D'Onofrio, B. M., Rodgers, J. L., & Waldman, I. D. (2008). Temperament and parenting during the first year of life predict future child conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 11391158. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9247-3.Google Scholar
Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated model of emotion processes and cognition in social information processing. Child Development, 71, 107118. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00124.Google Scholar
Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Revealing the relation between temperament and behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measurement confounding: Expert ratings and factor analyses. Child Development, 73, 867882.Google Scholar
Lengua, L. J., Bush, N. R., Long, A. C., Kovacs, E. A., & Trancik, A. M. (2008). Effortful control as a moderator of the relations between contextual risk factors and growth in adjustment problems. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 509528.Google Scholar
Lengua, L. J., West, S. G., & Sandler, I. N. (1998). Temperament as a predictor of symptomatology in children: Addressing contamination of measures. Child Development, 69, 164181.Google Scholar
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83, 11981202. doi:10.2307/2290157 Google Scholar
Lo, Y., Mendell, N. R., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767778. doi:10.1093/biomet/88.3.767.Google Scholar
McCart, M. R., Priester, P. E., Davies, W. H., & Azen, R. (2006). Differential effectiveness of behavioral parent-training and cognitive-behavioral therapy for antisocial youth: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 525541.Google Scholar
Mesman, J., Stoel, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., Koot, H. M., & Alink, A. R. (2009). Predicting growth curves of early childhood externalizing problems: Differential susceptibility of children with difficult temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 625636.Google Scholar
Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology, 44, 771786. doi:10:10370012-1689.44.3.771.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda. In Lahey, B. B., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency (pp. 4975). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H. L., … Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 26932698.Google Scholar
Murray, K. T., & Kochanska, G. (2002). Effortful control: Factor structure and relation to externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 503514.Google Scholar
Muthén, B. O., & Asparouhov, T. (2008). Growth mixture modeling: Analysis with non-Gaussian random effects. In Fitzmaurice, G., Davidian, M., Verbeke, G., & Molenberghs, G. (Eds.), Longitudinal data analysis (pp. 143165). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus version 7 user's guide. Los Angeles: Author.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. S., & Odgers, C. L. (2010). Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 109138. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131413.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys’ physical aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70, 11811196. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00086.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(4, Serial No. 278), 1143.Google Scholar
Odgers, C. L., Moffitt, T. E., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., … Caspi, A. (2008). Female and male antisocial trajectories: From childhood origins to adult outcomes. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 673716. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000333.Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., Sandy, J. M., & Lanthier, R. (2000). Early developmental precursors of externalizing behavior in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 119133.Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., Sandy, J. M., & Schilling, E. M. (2002). Early developmental precursors of impulsive and inattentive behavior: From infancy to middle childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 435447.Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., Lopez-Duran, N. L., Chang, H., & Lunkenheimer, E. S. (2011). Individual differences in the development of early peer aggression: Integrating contributions of self-regulation, theory of mind, and parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 253266. doi:10.1017/S09545.794/0000775.Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., Lopez, N. L., & Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of externalizing problems in young children: The role of effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 2545. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050029.Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C. R., & Lunkenheimer, E. S. (2009). Self-regulatory processes in early disruptive behavior: The preschool to school transition. In Olson, S. L. & Sameroff, A. J. (Eds.), Regulatory processes in the development of childhood behavior problems: Biological, behavioral, and social-ecological interactions. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, S. L., Tardif, T., Miller, A., Felt, B., Grabell, A., Kessler, D., … Hirabayashi, H. (2011). Executive function and parenting as predictors of externalizing problems in young children: A cross-national study of U.S., Chinese and Japanese preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 11631175.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441.Google Scholar
Power, T. (1993). Parenting Dimensions Inventory (PDI): A research manual. Unpublished manuscript, University of Houston, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, M. A. J., Smidts, D. P., Sergeant, J. A., Maasen, G. H., Posthumus, J. A., van Engeland, H., & Matthys, W. (2008). Executive functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 10971107.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament and development. In Kohnstamm, G. A., Bates, J. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 187247). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A, Hersey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at 3 to 7 years: The Children's Behavior Questionnaire. Child Development, 72, 13941408.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In Damon, W. & Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J. (2009). Transactional processes in development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Schaeffer, C. M., Petras, H., Ialongo, N., Masyn, K. E., Hubbard, S., Poduska, J., & Kellam, S. (2006). A comparison of girls’ and boys’ aggressive–disruptive behavior trajectories across elementary school: Prediction to young adult antisocial outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 500510.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., & Bell, R. Q. (1993). Developmental theories of parental contributors to antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 493518.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003). Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 39, 189200. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.2.189.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Hyde, L. W., & Brennan, L. W. (2012). Early predictors of boys’ antisocial trajectories. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 871888. doi:1017/S0954579412000429.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., & Shelleby, E. C. (2014). Early-starting conduct problems: Intersection of conduct problems and poverty. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 10, 503528.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Cohn, J. F., & Bell, R. Q. (1998). The development of early externalizing problems among children from low-income families: A transformational perspective. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 95107.Google Scholar
Shelleby, E. C., Shaw, D. S., Cheong, J., Chang, H. C., Gardner, F., Dishion, T., & Wilson, M. (2012). Behavioral control in at-risk toddlers: The influence of the Family Check-Up. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 288301.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Winter, C. C., & Patterson, G. R. (2014). Coercive family process and early-onset conduct problems from ages 2 to school entry. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 917932.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1729.Google Scholar
Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Developmental origins of disruptive behavior problems: The “original sin” hypothesis, epigenetics, and their consequences for prevention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 341367.Google Scholar
Trentacosta, C. J., Criss, M. M., Shaw, D. S., LaCourse, E., Hyde, L. W., & Dishion, T. J. (2011). Antecedents and outcomes of mother-son conflict and warmth during middle childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 82, 16761690.Google Scholar
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Larose, S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2005). Kindergarten disruptive behaviors, protective factors, and educational achievement by early adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 617629.Google Scholar
Walters, G. D., & Ruscio, J. (2013). Trajectories of youth antisocial behavior: Categories or continuua? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 653666.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1989). Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 3, 655684.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., & Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cognition & Emotion, 9, 117149.Google Scholar
Yuan, K. H., & Bentler, P. M. (2000). Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociological Methodology, 30, 165200. doi:10.1111/0081-1750.00078.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Markovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3, Serial No. 274), vii137.Google Scholar
Zhou, Q., Hofer, C., Eisenberg, N., Reiser, M., Spinrad, T., & Fabes, R. (2007). The developmental trajectories of attention focusing, attentional and behavioral persistence and externalizing problems during school-age years. Developmental Psychology, 43, 369385.Google Scholar