Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T14:17:54.021Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Sally J. Rogers
Affiliation:
University of Colorado Health Sciences Center
Bruce F. Pennington
Affiliation:
University of Denver

Abstract

Deficits specific to the syndrome of infantile autism appear in imitation, emotion sharing, theory of mind, pragmatics of communication, and symbolic play. Current competing theories of Hobson and of Baron-Cohen, Frith, and associates account for some, but not all, of these specific deficits. The present article suggests that early social capacities involving imitation, emotion sharing, and theory of mind are primarily and specifically deficient in autism. Further, these three capacities involve forming and coordinating social representations of self and other at increasingly complex levels via representational processes that extract patterns of similarity between self and other. Stern's theory of interpersonal development is offered as a continuous model for understanding the development and deficits of the autistic child and as a means for integrating competing theories about the primary deficits in autism. Finally, the article suggests a neuropsychological model of interpersonal coordination involving prefrontal cortex and executive function capacities that is consistent with the social deficits observed in autism.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldwin, J. M. (1906). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18(3), 379402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). The autistic child's theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(2), 285297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). Are autistic children behaviorists? An examination of their mental–physical and appearance–reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989c). Joint attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beisler, J. M., Tsai, C. Y., & Von, K. D. (1987). Comparisons between autistic and nonautistic children on the test for auditory comprehension of language. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(1), 95102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthenthal, B. I., & Fischer, K. W. (1978). The development of self-recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14, 4450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1989). Memory impairments and communication in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 99122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowler, D. M. (1989, December). Theory of mind in Asperger's Syndrome. Paper presented at the London Conference of the British Psychological Society, London.Google Scholar
Braverman, M., Fein, D., Lucci, D., & Waterhouse, L. (1989). Affect comprehension in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 301316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a “theory of mind”? In Lamb, M. E. & Sherrod, L. R. (Eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G., & Grover, L. (1988). The Origins of referential communication in human infancy. In Weiskrantz, L. (Ed.), Thought without language. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1988). Tactical deception of familiar individuals in baboons. In Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A. (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans (pp. 205210). Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Chugani, H. T., & Phelps, M. E. (1986). Maturational changes in cerebral function in infants determined by FDG position emission tomography. Science, 231, 840843.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1990). A historical perspective on the discipline of developmental psychopathology. In Rolf, J., Masten, A., Cicchetti, D., Nuechterlein, K., & Weintaub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 228). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, P., & Rutter, M. (1981). Autistic children's responses to structure and interpersonal demands. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(2), 201217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coleman, M., & Gillberg, C. (1985). The biology of the autistic syndromes. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8(3), 281292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1985). The frontal lobes. In Heilman, K. M. & Valenstein, E. (Eds.), Clinical neuro-psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., & Maurer, R. G. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35, 777786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (1983). Emotional disturbances associated with focal lesions of the limbic frontal lobe. In Heilman, K. M. & Valenstein, E. (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., & Fernald, M. (1987). Perspective-taking ability and its relationship to the social behavior of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 487498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, G., Hill, D., Spencer, A., Galpert, L., & Watson, L. (1990). Affective exchanges between young autistic children and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 335345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G. & Lewy, A. (1989a). Arousal, attention, and the socioemotional impairments of individuals with autism. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 4974). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Dawson, G. & Lewy, A. (1989b). Reciprocal subcortical-cortical influences in autism: The role of attentional mechanisms. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 144173). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., & McKissick, F. C. (1984). Self-recognition in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 383394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Renzi, E., Motti, F., & Nichelli, P. (1980). Imitating gestures: A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Archives Neurologia, 37, 610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., Bryson, C. Q., Pontius, W., & Kimberlin, C. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2(3), 264287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1985). Evidence for involvement of prefrontal cortex in cognitive changes during the first year of life: Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on a detour task with transparent barrier. Society of Neurosciences Abstracts (Part II), 11, 832.Google Scholar
Diamond, A., Zola-Morgan, S., & Squire, L. R. (1987). Performance of monkeys with hippocampal ablations on Piaget's AB Task. Society for Neurosciences Abstracts, 13, 206.Google Scholar
Fein, D., Pennington, B. Markowitz, P., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1986). Toward a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: Are the social defects primary? Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 25(2), 198212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, M., & Matthews, W. S. (1983). Self-recognition deficits in autism: Syndrome specific or general developmental delay? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13(3), 317324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraiberg, S. (1977). Insights from the blind: Comparative studies of blind and sighted infants. New York: New American Library.Google Scholar
Freitag, G. (1970). An experimental study of the social responsiveness of children with autistic behaviors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 9, 436453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (in press). Elective affinities in schizophrenia and childhood autism. In Bebbington, P. (Ed.), Social psychiatry: Theory, method and practice. New Brunswick, NJ: Transactions.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. (1988). Imitation in animals: History, definition, and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory. In Zentall, T. R. & Galef, B. G. Jr (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 328). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldberg, E., & Bilder, R. M. (1987). The frontal lobes and hierarchical organization of cognitive control. In Perecman, E. (Ed.), The frontal lobes revisited (pp. 159188). New York: IRBN Press.Google Scholar
Goldfarb, W. (1963). Self awareness in schizophrenic children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 8(1), 4760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Circuitry of primate prefrontal cortex and regulation of behavior by representational knowledge. In Plum, F. & Mountcastle, V. (Eds.), Handbook of physiology (vol. 5, pp. 373417). Bethesda, MD: American Psychological Society.Google Scholar
Haith, M. M. (1990). Progress in the understanding of sensory and perceptual processes in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 126.Google Scholar
Hammes, J. G. W., & Langdell, T. (1981). Precursors of symbol formation and childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(3), 331346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (1989). The autistic child's impaired conception of mental states. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 191197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L., & Muncer, A. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of beliefs and desires. Paper presented at the British Psychological Society Section Conference, Coleg Harlech.Google Scholar
Hertzig, M. E., Snow, M. E., & Sherman, M. (1989). Affect and cognition in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 195199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1986a). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 321342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1986b). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 671680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Dawson, G. (Ed.), Autism: Nature, diagnosis, and treatment (pp. 2248). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988). What's in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early social relationships: A psychobiologist's view. Child Development, 58, 633647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J., Grady, C., & Rapoport, S. (1988). The cerebral metabolic landscape in autism: Intercorrelations of regional glucose utilization. Archives of Neurology, 45, 749755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoskins, B. (1987). Conversations: Language intervention for adolescents. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. (1988). The social function of intellect. In Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A. (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans (pp. 1326). Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Jones, V., & Prior, M. (1985). Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Diabilities, 15(1), 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klinnert, M. D., Campos, J. J., Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., & Svejda, M. (1983). Emotions as behavior regulators: Social referencing in infancy. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotion, theory, research and experience (Vol. 2). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Kolb, B., & Milner, B. (1981). Performance of complex arm and facial movements after focal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 19, 505514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamb, M. E., & Campos, J. J. (1982). Development in infancy. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children's understanding of seeing, knowing, and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6(4), 325339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C. (1984). Peer relations in autism. In Morrison, F. J., Lord, C., & Keating, D. P. (Eds.), Applied developmental psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(3), 335349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, H., Rutter, M., Howlin, P., Rios, P., Le Conteur, A., Evered, C., & Folstein, S. (1989). Recognition and expression of emotional cues by autistic and normal adults. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 865877.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLean, P. D. (1978). A mind of three minds: Educating the triune brain. Seventy-seventh yearbook of the national society for the study of education (pp. 308342). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mahler, M. S. (1968). On human symbiosis and the vicissitudes of individuation (Vol. 1). New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Malatesta, C. Z., & Izard, C. E. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbol utilization. In Fox, N. A. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The psychobiology of affective development (pp. 161206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (1988). How to build a baby: On the development of an accessible representational system. Cognitive Development, 3, 113136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, M. A., & Uzgiris, I. C. (1983). Effects of model and action on imitation in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 6982.Google Scholar
McHale, S. M., Simeonsson, R., Marcus, L. M., & Olley, J. G. (1980). The social and symbolic quality of autistic children's communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10(3), 299310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. (1985). The roots of social and cognitive development: Models of man's original nature. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 130). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine month olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A. (1990). The place of language in a scientific psychology. Psychological Science, 1, 714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, S., Cutrer, P., Coplin, J. W., & Rodrigue, J. R. (1989). Do autistic children differ from retarded and normal children in Piagetian sensori-motor functioning? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 857864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989a). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989b). Second thoughts on the nature of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 213217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Lingerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(5), 657669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonverbal communication and play correlates of language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(3), 349364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nauta, W. J. H. (1971). The problem of the frontal lobe: A reinterpretation. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 8, 167187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1, 3559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationships, conceptualization and gesture imitations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(1), 4562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ornitz, E. M., & Ritvo, E. R. (1968). Perceptual inconstancy in early infantile autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 18, 7697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1990). Are there specific emotion perception deficits in young autistic children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 343361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (in press). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (in press). Asperger's syndrome: Evidence of an empirical distinction from high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in child-hood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Price, B. H., Daffner, K. R., Stowe, R. M., & Mesulam, M. M. (1989). The comportmental learning disabilities of early frontal lobe damage. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Prior, M. R. (1979). Cognitive abilities and disabilities in infantile autism: A review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7(4), 357380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, M., & Hoffman, W. (1990). Neuropsychological testing of autistic children through an exploration with frontal lobe tests. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 581590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
PrizantB., M. B., M., & Wetherby, A. M. (1987). Communicative intent: A framework for understanding social-communicative behavior in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(4), 472479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riguet, C. B., Taylor, L. D., Benaroya, S., & Klein, L. S. (1981). Symbolic play in autistic, Down's and normal children of equivalent mental age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11(4), 439448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., & DiLalla, D. L. (1990). Age of symptom onset in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 863872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., & Lewis, H. (1989). An effective day treatment model for young children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 207214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., Ozonoff, S., & Maslin-Cole, C. (1991). A comparative study of attachment behavior in young children with autism or other psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 483488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., & Puchalski, C. B. (1984). Development of symbolic play in visually impaired infants. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 3(4), 5257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumsey, J. M. (1985). Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15(1), 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuro-psychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1990). Neuro-psychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(2), 155168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, T., Sherman, M., Calamari, G., & Koch, D. (1987). Attachment in autism and other developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(4), 480484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M. (1989). The application of developmental knowledge to a clinical problem: The study of childhood autism. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 1: The emergence of a discipline (pp. 165187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Social attachments in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(1), 7481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Sherman, T., & Ungerer, J. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(5), 647656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Attachment behaviors in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14(3), 231244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smalley, S. L., & Asarnow, R. F. (1990). Brief report: Cognitive subclinical markers in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(2), 271278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, M. E., Hertzig, M. E., & Shapiro, T. (1987). Expression of emotion in young autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 836838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiker, D., & Ricks, M. (1984). Visual self-recognition in autistic children: Developmental relationships. Child Development, 55, 214225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., & Caro-Martinez, L. M. (1990). Naturalistic observations of spontaneous communication in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 437454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1983). Emotional concomitants of psychosurgery. In Heilman, K. M. & Satz, P. (Eds.), The neuropsychology of human emotion. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Sussman, S., & Sklar, J. J. (1969). The social awareness of autistic children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 39, 789806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremmer, R., Bond, S., & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 213225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M., & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down Syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children's ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 623630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tonick, I. (1981). Social relatedness in autistic children. Dissertation abstracts international, Dec. 42, (6-B), 2608.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication And Cooperation In Early Infancy: A Description Of Primary Intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Uzgiris, I. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkmar, F. R., Hoden, E. L., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Compliance, negativism, and the effects of treatment structure in autism: A naturalistic behavior study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26(6), 865877.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Rende, R. D., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Facial perception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 591598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, A. S. (1982). Intermodal Perception Of Expressive Behaviors By Human Infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 514535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welsh, M. C., & Pennington, B. F. (1988). Assessing frontal lobe functioning in children: Views from developmental psychology. Developmental Neuro-psychology, 4, 199230.Google Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A. M. (1986). Ontogeny of communicative functions in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental opmental Disorders, 16(3), 295316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherby, A. M., & Pruning, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S., & Brierley, L. (1977). Symbolic play in severely mentally retarded and in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 167178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Facial expressions of affect in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 725735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed