Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T07:49:22.565Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Temperament and the self-organization of personality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Douglas Derryberry*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Oregon State University
Marjorie A. Reed
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Oregon State University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Douglas Derryberry, Department of Psychology, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR 97331.

Abstract

This paper explores the development of cortical plasticity and cognitive representations in light of temperamental differences in basic motivational systems. Motivational systems related to reward/approach and punishment/avoidance begin to function early in life. By controlling the child's behavioral and emotional reactions, these systems provide exteroceptive and interoceptive information capable of stabilizing cortical synapses through use-dependent processes. By controlling attention, the motivational systems further contribute to synaptic stabilization through modulatory processes. As a result, children with strong reward/approach systems are likely to develop representations that emphasize potential rewards and frustrations and may become vulnerable to impulsive disorders. Children with strong punishment/avoidance systems may develop representations emphasizing punishment and relief, along with a vulnerability to anxiety disorders. These motivationally constructed representations differentiate in varied ways across domains involving the physical world, moral rules, and the self and, thus, contribute to the various forms of impulsive and anxious psychopathology.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. M. (in press). Child temperament in the U.S. and China: Similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality.Google Scholar
Alheid, G. F., & Heimer, L. (1988). New perspectives in basal forebrain organization of special relevance for neuropsychiatric disorders: The striatopallidal, amygdaloid, and corticopetal components of substantia innominata, Neuroscience, 27, 139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorpf, J. B. (1989). Shyness as a final common pathway for two different kinds of inhibition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 481492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B., & Nunner-Winkler, G. (1992). Children's moral motive strength and temperamental inhibition reduce their immoral behavior in real moral conflicts. Child Development, 63, 12231235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. New York: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., & van der Kooy, D. (1989). The tegmental pedunculopontine nucleus: A brain-stem output of the limbic system critical for the conditioned place preferences produced by morphine and amphetamine. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 34003409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Spirch, S. (1991). Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety and other disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 564577.Google ScholarPubMed
Boddy, J., Carver, A., & Rowley, K. (1986). Effects of positive and negative verbal reinforcement on performance as a function of extraversion-introversion: Some tests of Gray's theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 8188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I. (1991). Pouring new wine into old bottles: The social self as internal working model. In Gunnar, M. R. & Sroufe, L. A. (Eds.), Self processes and development: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Development (pp. 142). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Broks, P., Preston, G. C., Traub, M., Poppleton, P., Ward, C., & Stahl, S. M. (1988). Modelling dementia: Effects of scopolamine on memory and attention. Neuropsychologia, 26, 685700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, T. H., Kairiss, E. W., & Keenan, C. L. (1990). Hebbian synapses: Biophysical mechanisms and algorithms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 475511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S. (1973). Organization of early skilled action. Child Development, 44, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. In Rosenzweig, M. R. & Porter, L. W. (Eds.), Annual review of psychology (Vol. 42, pp. 459491). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
Campbell, S. B., & Werry, J. S. (1986). Attention deficit disorder (hyperactivity). In Quay, H. C. & Werry, J. S. (Eds.), Psychopathological disorders of childhood (3rd ed., pp. 111155). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cechetto, D. F., & Saper, C. B. (1990). Role of the cerebral cortex in autonomic function. In Loewy, A. D. & Spyer, K. M. (Eds.), Central regulation of autonomic functions (pp. 208223). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changeux, J. (1985). Neuronalman. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1991). Fractures in the crystal: Developmental psychopathology and the emergence of self. Developmental Review, 11, 271287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, C. R., Geffen, G. M., & Geffen, L. B. (1989). Catecholamines and the covert orienting of attention in humans. Neuropsychologia, 27, 131139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (1989). Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression. In Kendall, P. C. & Watson, D. (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 379411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Corulla, W. J. (1987). A psychometric investigation of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (revised) and its relationship to the 1.7 Impulsiveness Questionnaire. Personality and Individuality Differences, 8, 651658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curran, T., & Keele, S. W. (1993). Attentional and nonattentional forms of sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 189202.Google Scholar
Daugherty, T. K., & Quay, H. C. (1991). Response perseveration and delayed responding in childhood behavior disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 453461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 353375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A., & lacono, W. G. (1989). Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 457492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Attention and temperament: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 11281139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1991). The adaptive base of the neural hierarchy: Elementary motivational controls on network function. In Dienstbier, R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 38. Perspective on motivation (pp. 289342). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1992). Neural mechanisms of emotion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 329338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In Niedenthal, P. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), The heart's eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167196). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. (1984). The role of emotion in moral socialization. In Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. B. (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 484514). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological Review, 96, 84100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children. In Perl-mutter, M. (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 7790). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1991). Self-theories and goals: Their role in motivation personality and development. In Dienstbier, R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 198235). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M. (1989). The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., & Mogg, K. (1992). Clinical anxiety, trait anxiety, and memory bias. In Christiansen, S. (Ed.), The handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 429450). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fox, N. A. (1989). Psychophysical correlates of emotional reactivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 25, 364372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P., & Trower, P. (1990). The evolution and manifestation of social anxiety. In Crozier, W. R. (Ed.), Shyness and embarrassment: Perspectives from social psychology (pp. 144177). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, S. J., Aston-Jones, G., & Redmond, D. E. (1988). Responses of primate locus coeruleus neurons to simple an complex sensory stimuli. Brain Research Bulletin, 21, 401410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety. London: Oxford.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Gupta, S., & Shukla, A. P. (1989). Verbal operant conditioning as a function of extraversion and reinforcement. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 3944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamburger, V. (1977). The developmental history of the motor neuron. Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin, 15, 137.Google ScholarPubMed
Harpur, T. J., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. The personality of the psychopath. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Harter, S. (1988). Developmental processes in the construction of self. In Yawkey, T. D. & Johnson, J. E. (Eds.), Integrative processes and socialization: Early to middle childhood (pp. 4578). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Heimer, L., de Olmos, J., Alheid, G. F., & Zaborszky, L. (1991). “Perestroika” in the basal forebrain: Opening the border between neurology and psychiatry. In Holstege, G. (Ed.), Progress in brain research: Vol. 87. Role of the forebrain in sensation and behavior (pp. 109165). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1989). Self-discrepancy theory: What patterns of self-beliefs cause people to suffer? In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 93137). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1991). Development of self-regulatory and self-evaluative processes: Costs, benefits, and tradeoffs. In Gunnar, M. R. & Sroufe, L. A. (Eds.), Self processes and development: Vol. 23. The Minnesota Symposia on Child Development (pp. 125165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hockey, R. (1979). Stress and the cognitive components of skilled performance. In Hamilton, V. & Warburton, D. M. (Eds.), Human stress and cognition: An information processing approach (pp. 141177). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1988). Moral development. In Bornstein, M. H. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (2nd ed., pp. 497548). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Husain, S. A., & Kashani, J. H. (1992). Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1990). The influence of positive and negative affect on cognitive organization: Some implications for development. In Stein, N., Leventhal, B., & Trabasso, T. (Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 7594). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Gorgoglione, J. M. (1987). The influence of positive affect on cognitive organization. In Snow, R. & Farr, M. (Eds.), Aptitude, learning and instruction III: Affective and conative process analysis (pp. 143164). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (1993). Constraints on cortical plasticity. In Johnson, M. H. (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 703721). Oxford: Black well.Google Scholar
Kagan, J., Arcus, D., & Snidman, N. (1993). The idea of temperament: Where do we go from here? In Plomin, R. & McClearn, G. E. (Eds.), Nature, nurture and psychology (pp. 197210). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science, 240, 167173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Arcus, D. M. (1992). Initial reactions to unfamiliarity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 171173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, R. E., & Craighead, W. E. (1988). Differential effects of depression and anxiety on recall of feedback in a learning task. Behavior Therapy, 19, 437454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, H. A. (1992). Individual temperament and emerging self-perception: An interactive perspective. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 113120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1991). Socialization and temperament in the development of guilt and conscience. Child Development, 62, 13791392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G. (1993). Toward a synthesis of parental socialization and child temperament in early development of conscience. Child Development, 64, 324347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaVay, S., Wiesel, T. N., & Hubel, D. H. (1980). The development of ocular dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 191, 151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (1987). Emotion. In Plum, F. (Ed.), Handbook of physiology: Sect. 1. The nervous system: Vol. V. Higher functions of the brain, Part I (pp. 419459). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 1520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, M., Ward, J. C., & Clark, D. M. (1983). Neuroticism and the recall of positive and negative personality information. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 495503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matheny, A. P. (1989). Children's behavioral inhibition over age and across situations. Journal of Personality, 57, 215235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 28, 455468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathews, A., Mogg, K., May, J., & Eysenck, M. (1989). Implicit and explicit memory bias in anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 236240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayo, P. R. (1983). Personality traits and the retrieval of positive and negative memories. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 465471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merzenich, M. M., Recanzone, G., Jenkins, W. M., Allard, T. T., & Nudo, R. J. (1988). Cortical representational plasticity. In Rakic, P. & Singer, W. (Eds.), Neurobiology of neocortex (pp. 4167). New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Mikulincer, M., Kedem, P., & Paz, D. (1990). Anxiety and categorization—1. The structure and boundaries of mental categories. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 805814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishkin, M., & Appenzeller, T. (1987). The anatomy of memory. Scientific American, 9, 8090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P. (1987). Reaction to punishment in extraverts and psychopaths: Implications for the impulsive behavior of disinhibited individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 464480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. P., Widom, C. S., & Nathan, S. (1985). Passive-avoidance in syndromes of disinhibition: Psychopathy and extraversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 13161327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohman, A., & Soares, J. J. F. (1993). On the automatic nature of phobic fear: Conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 121132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 407467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1986a). The anatomy of emotions. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience: Vol. 3. Biological foundations of emotions (pp. 91124). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1986b). The psychobiology of prosocial behaviors: Separation distress, play, and altruism. In Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., & lannotti, R. (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins (pp. 1957). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1992). A critical role for “affective neuroscience” in resolving what is basic about basic emotions. Psychological Review, 99, 554560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, C. M., Kosson, D. S., & Newman, J. P. (1987). Reaction to punishment, reflectivity, and passive avoidance learning in extraverts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 565575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearce, J. M. (1987). An introduction to animal cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pliszka, S. R. (1992). Comorbidity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and overanxious disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 191203.Google ScholarPubMed
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review ofNeuroscience, 13, 2542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provost, S. C., & Woodward, R. (1991). Effects of nicotine gum on repeated administration of the Stroop text. Psychopharmacology, 104, 536540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, M. A., & Derryberry, D. (1993). Temperament and attention to positive and negative trait information. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Robbins, T. W., Cador, M., Taylor, J. R., & Everitt, B. J. (1989). Limbic-striatal interactions in reward-related processes. Neuroscience and-Biobehavioral Reviews, 13, 155162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (1987). Information representation, processing, and storage in the brain: Analysis at the single neuron level. In Changeux, J. P. & Konishi, M. (Eds.), The neural and molecular bases of learning (pp. 503540). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1990). Functions of neuronal networks in the hippocampus and of backprojections in the cerebral cortex in memory. In McGaugh, J. L., Weinberger, N. M., & Lynch, G. (Eds.), Brain organization and memory: Cells, systems, and circuits (pp. 184210). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1989a). Behavioral approach and inhibition. In Reznick, J. S. (Eds.), Perspectives on behavioral inhibition (pp. 139158). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1989b). Temperament and development. In Kohnstamm, G. A., Bates, J. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 187247). New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994). Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 2139.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. L. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. I, pp. 3786). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (in press). A psychobiological approach to the development of temperament. In Bates, J. E. & Wachs, T. D. (Eds.), Temperament: Individual differences in biology and behavior. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., Hershey, K., & Derryberry, D. (1993). Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at 7 years. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424435.Google Scholar
Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In Jones, M. R. (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 142). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Selden, N. R. W., Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1990). Enhanced behavioral conditioning to context and impaired behavioral and neuroendocrine responses to conditioned stimuli following ceruleocortical noradrenergic lesions: Support for an attentional hypothesis of central noradrenergic function. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 531539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, W. (1990). Role of acetylcholine in use-dependent plasticity of the visual cortex. In Steriade, M. & Biesold, D. (Eds.), Brain cholinergic systems (pp. 314336). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1990). An organizational perspective on the self. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (Eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 281308). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In Tuma, A. H. & Maser, J. D. (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1991). Growth and education of the cerebral hemispheres. In Trevarthen, C. (Ed.), Brain circuits and the function of mind: Essays in honor of Roger W. Sperry. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M. (1992). Developing emotions and cortical networks. In Gunnar, M. & Nelson, C. A. (Eds.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology: Vol.24. Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 75128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., & Derryberry, D. (1992). Motivated attention: Anxiety and the frontal executive mechanisms. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 5, 233252.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Review, 91, 185215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, D. M., Magnuson, D. J., & Gray, T. S. (1992). Organization of amygdaloid projections to brainstem dopaminergic, noradrenergic, and adrenergic cell groups in the rat. Brain Research Bulletin, 28, 447454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, J. F., Newman, J. P., & Bachorowski, J. (1991). Failures of response modulation: Impulsive behavior in anxious and impulsive individuals. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 2344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinberger, N. M., Ashe, J. H., Metherate, R., McKenna, T. M., Diamond, D. M., Bakin, J. S., Lennartz, R. C., & Cassady, J. M. (1990). Neural adaptive information processing: A preliminary model of receptive-field plasticity in auditory cortex during Pavlovian conditioning. In Gabriel, M. & Moore, J. (Eds.), Learning and computational neuroscience: Foundations of adaptive networks (pp. 91138). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Woolf, N. J. (1991). Cholinergic systems in mammalian brain and spinal cord. Progress in Neurobiology, 37, 475524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Kochanska, G. (1990). The origins of guilt. In Thompson, R. (Ed.), The 36th Annual Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Socio-emotional development (pp. 183258). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1991). Psychobiology of personality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar