Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T03:13:53.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Taking stock to move forward: Where the field of developmental psychopathology might be heading

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 February 2024

Peter Fonagy*
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Patrick Luyten
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Elizabeth Allison
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Chloe Campbell
Affiliation:
Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Peter Fonagy; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In this paper, dedicated to Dante Cicchetti’s contributions and enduring influence, we explore the prospective directions of developmental psychopathology. Our focus centers on key domains where Cicchetti’s significant achievements have continually shaped our evolving thinking about psychological development. These domains include (a) the concepts of equifinality and multifinality, along with the challenges in predicting developmental trajectories, (b) the imperative to integrate wider sociocultural viewpoints into developmental psychopathology frameworks, (c) the interplay of genetic and environmental influences in developmental courses, (d) the significance of mental state language, and (e) the progress, or its absence, in the development of prevention and intervention tactics for children, adolescents, and their caregivers. While many of our forecasts regarding the future of developmental psychopathology may not materialize, we maintain optimistic that the essential ideas presented will influence the research agenda in this field and contribute to its growth over the next fifty years.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

When the musician Bob Dylan was awarded a Nobel Prize in Literature in 2016, another legendary musician, Leonard Cohen, remarked that awarding Dylan the Nobel Prize in Literature was akin to “pinning a medal on Mount Everest for being the highest mountain.” In a similar vein, dedicating a special issue of Development and Psychopathology to the work and legacy of Dante Cicchetti is highly fitting considering his profound impact on the field. Dante’s contributions to the field of developmental psychopathology are extensive and varied. They range from his ground-breaking work on equifinality and multifinality in developmental paths (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996), his trailblazing studies on the impacts of adversity, especially child maltreatment (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2005; Russotti et al., Reference Russotti, Warmingham, Duprey, Handley, Manly, Rogosch and Cicchetti2021), the significance of attachment (Doyle & Cicchetti, Reference Doyle and Cicchetti2017; Raby et al., Reference Raby, Cicchetti, Carlson, Cutuli, Englund and Egeland2012) and resilience (Denckla et al., Reference Denckla, Cicchetti, Kubzansky, Seedat, Teicher, Williams and Koenen2020), to the crucial role of peer and romantic relationships in development (Handley et al., Reference Handley, Russotti, Rogosch and Cicchetti2019), and his highly innovative work on intervention and prevention (Cicchetti & Gunnar, Reference Cicchetti and Gunnar2008; DeGarmo et al., Reference DeGarmo, Gewirtz, Li, Tavalire and Cicchetti2023; Guild et al., Reference Guild, Alto, Handley, Rogosch, Cicchetti and Toth2021). His contributions cover the entire spectrum of developmental psychopathology. The blend of his extensive theoretical and conceptual interests, along with the robust interdisciplinary nature of his research, is likely the most distinctive aspect of his approach to developmental psychopathology. Dante’s long tenure as the editor of Development and Psychopathology, the leading journal in the field, and his role as editor of multiple versions of the authoritative handbook Developmental Psychopathology, have solidified his role as leader of the field.

We are profoundly privileged to have been invited to contribute to this special issue dedicated to Dante’s contributions and exploring the future of developmental psychopathology. Writing this paper has been an equally humbling experience, not only because it made us once more conscious of the immense scope of his contributions to the field but also because it reminded us of how difficult it is to predict the future. Indeed, it is very well known that humans are generally terrible at predicting the future, and we consider it extremely unlikely that we will be the exception to that rule. Having been involved in longitudinal research for several decades now ourselves, we know all too well how difficult it is to predict future developmental trajectories of people, let alone that we would be able to predict the developmental trajectory of an entire research field. In writing this paper, we were therefore reminded of the story of Robert Metcalfe, a professor at the University of Texas at Austin, who has been involved in the development of the internet from the 1970s onward. In 1995, Metcalfe famously argued in a column that the internet would suffer a “catastrophic collapse” in the following year, and he allegedly promised to eat his words if it did not. Two years later, during a keynote speech, he pulled out a printed copy of his column, put it in a blender with some water, and drank the blended mixture. Conscious of the risk that we will need to eat the present paper at some point in the future, we therefore find ourselves in complete agreement with Winston Churchill, who once noted that predictions are extremely difficult, especially when they concern the future. However, we hope that at least some of our predictions will materialize and, consistent with our dedication to empirical science, we look forward to seeing which ones are borne out and which ones turn out to be mere wishful thinking.

Our analysis of developmental psychopathology’s future is structured around five critical areas where Dante has been exceptionally influential. These areas are: (a) the concepts of equifinality and multifinality, and the complexities of predicting developmental paths, (b) the urgency of incorporating broader sociocultural perspectives into developmental psychopathology models, (c) the dynamic interaction between genetic factors and environmental conditions in shaping developmental trajectories, (d) the importance of mental state language, and (e) the progress, or lack thereof, in advancing prevention and intervention methods for children, teenagers, and their caregivers.

Multifinality, equifinality, and the elusive complexity of developmental trajectories revisited

Research over the past decades has clearly demonstrated the intricate interplay of psychological, biological, and sociocultural factors in psychological developmental processes in both typical and atypical development (Handley et al., Reference Handley, Russotti, Rogosch and Cicchetti2019; Jolicoeur-Martineau et al., Reference Jolicoeur-Martineau, Belsky, Szekely, Widaman, Pluess, Greenwood and Wazana2020; Kochanska & An, Reference Kochanska and Anin press; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021; McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022; Pollak, Reference Pollak2015; Russotti et al., Reference Russotti, Warmingham, Duprey, Handley, Manly, Rogosch and Cicchetti2021; Verhage et al., Reference Verhage, Fearon, Schuengel, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans‐Kranenburg, Madigan, Roisman, Oosterman, Behrens, Wong, Mangelsdorf, Priddis and Brisch2018; Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). In our opinion, two major sets of findings have emerged in this context, which have considerably changed our understanding of the nature of psychological development. The first set of findings concerns the major role of early adversity in determining both physical and mental health (Reference Smith and Pollak2021b; Hogg et al., Reference Hogg, Gardoki-Souto, Valiente-Gómez, Rosa, Fortea, Radua, Amann and Moreno-Alcázar2023; Madigan et al., Reference Madigan, Deneault, Racine, Park, Thiemann, Zhu, Dimitropoulos, Williamson, Fearon, Cénat, McDonald, Devereux and Neville2023; McCrory et al., Reference McCrory, De Brito and Viding2012, Reference McCrory, Foulkes and Viding2022; Pollak, Reference Pollak2015, Smith & Pollak, Reference Smith and Pollak2021a). The second relates to findings concerning the ubiquitous nature of resilience, and thus plasticity and change, particularly in critical periods in psychological development (Bonanno & Diminich, Reference Bonanno and Diminich2013; Denckla et al., Reference Denckla, Cicchetti, Kubzansky, Seedat, Teicher, Williams and Koenen2020; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Lucke, Nelson and Stallworthy2021). In both of these areas, Cicchetti and his colleagues have made key contributions (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2005; Denckla et al., Reference Denckla, Cicchetti, Kubzansky, Seedat, Teicher, Williams and Koenen2020; Russotti et al., Reference Russotti, Warmingham, Duprey, Handley, Manly, Rogosch and Cicchetti2021).

Early developmental theorists cautioned against overly narrow and simplistic explanations of psychological development. For instance, Anna Freud (Reference Freud1981) emphasized the importance of simultaneously considering different developmental lines and their intricate interactions; Erik Erikson (Reference Erikson1959) highlighted the interaction among psychological and cultural factors in his epigenetic theory of human development across the lifespan; and Bronfenbrennerʼs (Bronfenbrenner, 1977; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Morris, Damon and Lerner2007) bio-ecological approach focused on complex interactions among psychosocial and biological factors in psychological development. Consistent with the views of these pioneers, it has become increasingly clear that predicting developmental outcomes is difficult (Smith & Pollak, Reference Smith and Pollak2021a) not least because of the role of chance events and stochastic processes in explaining psychological development (Fraley & Roberts, Reference Fraley and Roberts2005). This confronts us with a problem that was described more than a century ago by Sigmund Freud when he discussed the issue of predicting later development from childhood to later adulthood:

“So long as we trace the development from its final outcome backwards, the chain of events appears continuous, and insight which is completely satisfactory or even exhaustive. But if we proceed the reverse way (…) then we no longer get the impression of an inevitable sequence of events which could not have been otherwise determined. We notice at once that there might have been another result, and that we might have been just as well able to understand and explain the latter. The synthesis is thus not so satisfactory as the analysis; in other words, from a knowledge of the premises we could not have foretold the nature of the result. (…).” (Freud, Reference Freud and Strachey1920, pp. 167–168)

Freud’s views foreshowed Dante Cicchetti and Fred Rogosch’s (Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch1996) notions of equifinality and multifinality which revolutionized our thinking concerning psychological development. Equifinality captures how a single developmental outcome may be influenced by multiple risk or protective factors, while multifinality implies that one particular risk or protective factor may result in multiple outcomes. Although both notions have been massively helpful in developmental psychopathology, and have greatly fostered a move toward more transdiagnostic and transtheoretical approaches in developmental psychopathology and beyond (discussed in more detail below), we are now – perhaps more than ever – confronted with the elusive problem of prediction in developmental psychopathology. This is the case particularly because, with some exceptions, most associations between developmental factors and later outcomes are typically at best small to modest (Fearon et al., Reference Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van Ijzendoorn, Lapsley and Roisman2010; Groh et al., Reference Groh, Roisman, van Ijzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Fearon2012; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell and Fonagy2021; Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). This issue poses challenges not just in research but also in the realms of prevention and intervention. For example, it is crucial to discern between the typical developmental upheaval characteristic of adolescence and the signs of emerging personality disorder (Fonagy, Speranza, et al., Reference Fonagy, Speranza, Luyten, Kaess, Hessels and Bohus2015; Hutsebaut et al., Reference Hutsebaut, Debbané and Sharp2020). Similarly, there is a question about whether we should persist in focusing on a limited set of presumed specific vulnerability factors in psychosocial interventions designed for particular psychological disorders. This approach may be less effective if these specific factors account for only a small portion of the variance in developmental outcomes, especially considering the substantial evidence for the role of broader transdiagnostic factors involved in equifinality and multifinality (Leichsenring et al., Reference Leichsenring, Steinert, Rabung and Ioannidis2022; Norton & Paulus, Reference Norton and Paulus2016; Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, Kuppens, Ng, Vaughn-Coaxum, Ugueto, Eckshtain and Corteselli2019).

How we can we improve our ability to predict developmental outcomes in the future and use this knowledge to improve our intervention strategies? One potential avenue seems to be provided by recently emerging hierarchical models of psychopathology and personality development. Research on a general psychopathology factor and a bifactor model of psychopathology is likely to be greatly helpful in elucidating both equifinality and multifinality in developmental outcomes in the future. The bifactor model basically argues that a single general psychopathology factor or “p” factor, akin to a general (“g”) factor in research on intelligence, explains the covariance between higher-order factors of psychopathology such as internalizing, externalizing, and thought disorder features and specific symptom domains (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel, Meier, Ramrakha, Shalev, Poulton and Moffitt2014; Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018; Choate et al., Reference Choate, Bornovalova, Hipwell, Chung and Stepp2023; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Moore, Kaczkurkin and Zald2021; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Atkinson, Davis, Riley and Oltmanns2020). The p factor is thus assumed to represent a general predisposition to mental disorder; it follows from this that the p factor should correlate with known risk factors for psychopathology. Consistent with these assumptions, higher p factor scores are associated with known vulnerability factors for psychopathology, including early adversity, markers of socioeconomic disadvantage, and disruptions in brain function and brain structure (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018; Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Fonagy, Deighton, Belsky, Vostanis and Wolpert2015). Moreover, the p factor overlaps with genomic and neural “p factors”, that is, genetic factors and impairments in neural functioning that are implicated in a wide variety of psychological disorders and problems (Sprooten et al., Reference Sprooten, Franke and Greven2022). Crucially, research findings suggest that the p factor explains more of the variance in developmental outcomes than specific vulnerability factors do (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018; Patalay et al., Reference Patalay, Fonagy, Deighton, Belsky, Vostanis and Wolpert2015), and there is emerging research that a general psychopathology factor also predicts treatment response in psychosocial interventions over and above lower-bandwidth factors (Constantinou et al., Reference Constantinou, Goodyer, Eisler, Butler, Kraam, Scott, Pilling, Simes, Ellison, Allison and Fonagy2019; Fiorini et al., Reference Fiorini, Saunders, Fonagy and Midgley2022). Furthermore, the p factor is also likely to be involved in active person–environment transactions, in that individuals with high p factor levels may unconsciously create, in part, their own stressful environment (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Atkinson, Davis, Riley and Oltmanns2020).

The relevance of a bifactor model of psychopathology for developmental psychopathology is further increased if we consider the p factor as indicative of problems with the capacity for social learning and epistemic trust in particular (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Campbell, Constantinou, Higgitt, Allison and Luyten2022; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell and Fonagy2021). Epistemic trust refers to an evolutionarily prewired capacity to identify knowledge that is conveyed by others as significant, personally relevant, and generalizable to other contexts. It is thought to facilitate resilience through a health-generating (salutogenic) process facilitated by social learning.

Evolutionary research suggests that the development of epistemic trust is a species-specific ability that allows the kinds of sophisticated and complex forms of communication and collaboration that are typical of humans (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2010), particularly in combination with another species-specific ability, namely, the capacity to mentalize. From a developmental perspective, the combination of the emerging capacity for mentalizing and epistemic trust seems to open in children what we have termed an “epistemic superhighway” allowing the developing child to take in social information conveyed by informants who are considered by the child to be trustworthy.

The development of both mentalizing and epistemic trust is complex, in that epistemic vigilance is the default mode of functioning in children as, from age 3 onward, they begin to develop the capacity for selective trust in knowledge conveyed by others (Coan & Sbarra, Reference Coan and Sbarra2015; Fonagy & Luyten, Reference Fonagy and Luyten2018; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Luyten, Allison and Campbell2017; Gergely, Reference Gergely, Metcalfe and Terrace2013; Konner, Reference Konner2010; Sperber et al., Reference Sperber, Clement, Heintz, Mascaro, Mercier, Origgi and Wilson2010; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2010). The early caregiving environment and, later on, the sociocultural environment more generally, appears to play a crucial role in overcoming epistemic vigilance in children and in allowing them to develop an adaptive epistemic stance toward cultural knowledge (Allen, Reference Allen2021; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Luyten, Allison and Campbell2017). In this regard, it is as yet not clear what impact the current epistemic crisis, in which children and young people in particular are being subjected to a torrent of socio-digital information whose trustworthiness is often unclear, will have on children’s and young people’s psychological development.

Again, hierarchical models of psychopathology and personality (Bleidorn et al., Reference Bleidorn, Hopwood, Back, Denissen, Hennecke, Jokela, Kandler, Lucas, Luhmann, Orth, Roberts, Wagner, Wrzus and Zimmermann2020; Hopwood et al., Reference Hopwood, Wright and Bleidorn2022; Widiger et al., Reference Widiger, Sellbom, Chmielewski, Clark, DeYoung, Kotov, Krueger, Lynam, Miller, Mullins-Sweatt, Samuel, South, Tackett, Thomas, Watson and Wright2018) provide a potentially productive approach in this context, as they specify both general personality-related factors (such as a general p factor) and more specific personality features that might be involved in person–environment correlations and interactions, leading to more fine-grained predictions about developmental outcomes. For instance, employing Waddington’s (Reference Waddington1957) metaphor of an epigenetic landscape, the personality or temperamental dimensions identified in multivariate studies could steer children in certain directions within the epigenetic landscape, affecting their capacity for epistemic trust (Luyten & Fonagy, Reference Luyten and Fonagy2022). Specific genetic predispositions, as manifested in low levels of temperamental factors such as effortful control and executive functioning, might negatively influence the development of epistemic trust. High levels of neuroticism, a temperamental factor that also has a high genetic component, might impact the development of secure attachment relationships in infants and thus their ability to trust others as sources of social information and thus hinder salutogenic processes. Similarly, the presence or absence of responsive caregivers or mentors, influenced by active relationship-recruiting capacities and/or chance, could alter individuals’ epistemic stance – and thus their capacity for social learning – over time. Broader aspects of the sociocultural climate in which children are raised, like the presence or absence of social capital (i.e., shared values or resources that allow individuals to work together in a group) within a given (sub)cultural context, may partly shape a developing child’s perception of others (Smith & Pollak, Reference Smith and Pollak2021b; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Harms, Green, Wilson and Pollak2023) and, ultimately, their capacity for epistemic trust. These perspectives align with contemporary views of personality and personality pathology, suggesting that personality fundamentally serves as a subjective intrapsychic system for fulfilling adult life tasks, highlighting the role of individuals as agentic actors in interpreting and managing the self. From this perspective, specific personality traits may influence the expression and manifestations of personality and personality pathology (Sharp & Wall, Reference Sharp and Wall2021).

Artificial Intelligence (AI) is extremely likely to facilitate research in this area. For example, epistemic trust, epistemic mistrust, and epistemic credulity (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Tanzer, Saunders, Booker, Allison, Li, O’Dowda, Luyten and Fonagy2021) could be viewed as variables within a Bayesian framework, with the p factor reflecting a general impairment in the capacity to revise hypotheses (i.e., beliefs about the world, and especially about others) in the light of current experience, resulting in an apparent rigidity manifested as impairments in the ability for (social) learning. Hence, internal working models of the self and others, as traditionally conceived within many developmental psychopathology approaches, may contain not only an expectancy parameter but also an expected outcome and learning parameter from this perspective. More specific personality traits, such as neuroticism, openness to experience, and agreeableness, could enhance or inhibit the updating of social information, much like certain caregiving practices or broader sociocultural environments might. With the growing accessibility and capabilities of AI and computational psychiatry methods more broadly, the future is likely to witness increasingly sophisticated modeling of developmental trajectories along these suggested lines.

Incorporating communities and culture in developmental psychopathology approaches

Developmental psychopathology approaches have become increasingly more contextual in the past few decades. This has also come with an increasing realization that, historically, most research in psychology has been based on individuals in “WEIRD” (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic) countries, who constitute only slightly more than 10% of the global population (Henrich, Reference Henrich2020). Likewise, less than 10% of studies investigating gene–environment interactions are based on non-Western samples (Leighton et al., Reference Leighton, Botto, Silva, Jimenez and Luyten2017). Cicchetti has in this context consistently emphasized the importance of the broader social environment in child development (Beeghly & Cicchetti, Reference Beeghly and Cicchetti1994; Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2005).

Our own perspectives on normal and disrupted development have similarly evolved over the past decade. Initially, we placed significant emphasis on the role of dyadic attachment processes and the development of mentalizing abilities within close attachment relationships as key factors in explaining developmental paths. However, our focus has since expanded to a broader, evolutionarily-informed, social-communicative approach. This more comprehensive perspective includes the influence of family, peers, and sociocultural factors at large in the development of a psychological self (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Campbell, Constantinou, Higgitt, Allison and Luyten2022; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy2020).

Two strands of research have contributed to this shift in our thinking. First, based on evolutionary-based research, in particular the work of Tomasello and colleagues (O’Madagain & Tomasello, Reference O’Madagain and Tomasello2019; Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2020a, Reference Tomasello2020b), we have increasingly come to realize that the role of impairments in the capacity for mentalizing far exceeds the importance we ascribed to them in understanding psychopathology. There is increasing evidence that mentalizing is a central human capacity, rooted in joint attention and shared intentionality, that plays a pivotal role in both normal and disrupted psychological development, in that it enables the complex forms of communication and collaboration, and thus (social) learning, that are typical of humans. Second, the work of Gergely and Csibra on their theory of natural pedagogy (Csibra & Gergely, Reference Csibra and Gergely2009; Gergely, Reference Gergely, Metcalfe and Terrace2013), Konner’s views of childhood as reflecting a process of enculturation made possible by the evolution of a cultural acquisition device (Konner, Reference Konner2010), Tomasello’s cultural intelligence hypothesis (Tomasello, Reference Tomasello2010), social baseline theory (Coan & Sbarra, Reference Coan and Sbarra2015), and Sperber and colleagues’ groundbreaking work on epistemic vigilance in humans (Sperber et al., Reference Sperber, Clement, Heintz, Mascaro, Mercier, Origgi and Wilson2010) drew our attention to the potential role of epistemic trust in explaining the fast transmission of social information that is typical of most human learning processes. We now assume that epistemic trust is likely to play a key role in resilience through a salutogenic process that involves deriving benefit from relevant information accessible through the social environment (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Tanzer, Saunders, Booker, Allison, Li, O’Dowda, Luyten and Fonagy2021; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Campbell, Constantinou, Higgitt, Allison and Luyten2022).

This led to a substantial shift in our understanding of the nature of several key developmental processes. Research on attachment and personality provides a good illustration of this change in our views (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell and Fonagy2021). Although we still believe that research findings are consistent with the assumption that a secure attachment context fosters a broad array of psychological processes, ranging from cognitive development to the acquirement of socioemotional skills and capacities, we have also come to realize that the importance of (early) attachment relationships for children partly lies in learning to recognize who is trustworthy, authoritative, and knowledgeable (Corriveau et al., Reference Corriveau, Harris, Meins, Fernyhough, Arnott, Elliott, Liddle, Hearn, Vittorini and de Rosnay2009; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Campbell, Constantinou, Higgitt, Allison and Luyten2022). Furthermore, we now consider secure attachment an important, but not the only – and in some cultural contexts not even necessary – context for social communication to promote the most effective way to function in a given sociocultural environment. Broader social contextual factors (e.g., peers, neighborhood climate, the social capital in a given cultural environment, and, last but not least in the digital era, social media) are likely to influence the development of attachment and epistemic trust over time. From this perspective, we no longer consider attachment or personality styles as something that reside within the individual, but as something that defines the relationship an individual has with others and their social contexts more generally. Put otherwise, attachment and personality styles are seen as communicative strategies underpinning social learning to ensure adaptation to ever-changing social situations, however “maladaptive” or “rigid” this adaptation strategy may seem. Yet, the rigidity or maladaptiveness we now posit is in the eye of the beholder. For instance, the often pervasive mistrust that is typical of individuals with high levels of dismissive and disorganized attachment (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Tanzer, Saunders, Booker, Allison, Li, O’Dowda, Luyten and Fonagy2021) should be considered a reasonable and understandable attempt to adapt to the unavailability and/or untrustworthiness of others, often as part of a “risky environment” (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2005). This view opens up interesting perspectives for intervention, as the “rigidity” or “hard-to-reach” nature of the individual is not situated primarily in the individual but in the relationship between the individual and their context, and thus may be apparent only as long as the mechanisms responsible for the obstruction to epistemic trust are in place.

Further, as noted, dyadic attachment processes and emotional sensitivity are unlikely to be the exclusive route for generating epistemic trust and salutogenesis. For instance, alloparenting, which involves caregiving by multiple caregivers without specific division of tasks based on location or time, is widely prevalent in numerous non-Western countries (Dagan & Sagi-Schwartz, Reference Dagan and Sagi-Schwartz2021; Hawks & Meehan, Reference Hawks, Meehan, Keller and Otto2014; Hrdy, Reference Hrdy2016). This practice may even be an adaptation to challenging environments, reflecting a response to adversity (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Ringen, Duda and Jaeggi2020). Caregiver–child interactions involving play and conversation tend to be less frequent in many non-Western cultures, and often do not conform to the “interactional script” commonly observed in Western countries. Viewing the child as a “social apprentice,” it is crucial for the child to experience moments of recognition and a sense of joint intentionality that provide clear guidance on how to behave within their community. A focus on attachment, emotions, and psychological states typifies a Western “script” in this context, but there are numerous other possible scripts (Mesman et al., Reference Norton and Paulus2016, Reference Mesman, Minter, Angnged, Cisse, Salali and Migliano2018). The significant benefit of a secure attachment relationship, when viewed in the context of groups rather than individuals, is that it facilitates the child’s orientation toward cultural learning opportunities from their environment (Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Campbell, Constantinou, Higgitt, Allison and Luyten2022).

Finally, there may be important cultural differences in terms of which patterns of behavior are considered to reflect normal personality variations and at various levels of the p factor. For example, there are important cultural differences in attitudes toward more introvert, schizoid personality features, which seem to be valued more in many non-Western countries than in many Western cultures. At higher levels of p factor, psychotic thoughts and beliefs may be considered to be less pathological in some cultures than in others (Paniagua, Reference Paniagua, Cuéllar and Paniagua2000).

The interplay of genes and environment

Developmental psychopathology research has greatly expanded our knowledge of key neurobiological systems, such as those involved in stress, reward, and social cognition, and their role in psychological development. Similarly, the insights from both behavioral and molecular genetic studies have significantly altered our understanding of the role of genetic factors in developmental processes. This is particularly true since the advent of research on gene–environment correlations, interactions, and epigenetics. In this field, Cicchetti and colleagues have contributed several seminal studies (Handley et al., Reference Handley, Russotti, Ross, Toth and Cicchetti2023; Raby et al., Reference Raby, Cicchetti, Carlson, Cutuli, Englund and Egeland2012).

However, a persistent issue has been the difficulty in replicating findings, especially in the realms of gene–environment correlations, interactions, and epigenetics (Bleys et al., Reference Bleys, Luyten, Soenens and Claes2018; Golds et al., Reference Golds, MacBeth and de Kruiff2020; Leighton et al., Reference Leighton, Botto, Silva, Jimenez and Luyten2017; Martins et al., Reference Martins, Yusupov, Binder, Brückl and Czamara2022; Picardi et al., Reference Picardi, Giuliani and Gigantesco2020; Smeeth et al., Reference Smeeth, Beck, Karam and Pluess2021). This challenge may be partly due to the identification of a general genetic factor involved in many developmental outcomes throughout the lifespan, leading to a lack of specificity in genetic factors in psychological development. This raises complex questions about equifinality and multifinality. Moreover, although the heritability of many developmental constructs appears substantial, in the domain of gene–environment interaction, epigenetics, and genome-wide association studies, typically only small effect sizes have been observed. Most studies have also relied on post-hoc analyses of existing data sets. Consequently, there is a pressing need for preregistered, large-scale, prospective studies using more diverse samples. Much current research may rely on what could eventually be considered “shallow” phenotyping, based on a limited set of developmental factors and a variable-centered approach. Therefore, “deep phenotyping” or “deep ecophenotyping,” which adopts a person-centered perspective and develops phenotypes or ecophenotypes based on individual characteristics across various domains (e.g., family, friendship and romantic relationships, academic achievement, community engagement), may be a more effective approach in developmental genetics. Furthermore, advancements in AI and experimental psychopathology are increasingly enabling the development of more sophisticated typologies of individuals, which could have significant implications for psychosocial interventions (Schiele et al., Reference Schiele, Gottschalk and Domschke2020).

Mentalizing, mind-mindedness, and mental-state language

Thirty years ago, Beeghly and Cicchetti (Reference Beeghly and Cicchetti1994) demonstrated that toddlers who experienced maltreatment and had insecure attachment exhibited the most compromised internal-state lexicon compared to other children. Cicchetti and colleagues have always maintained that psychological development follows a series of qualitative reorganizations within and between biological and psychological systems, involving differentiation and subsequent hierarchical integration of psychological capacities (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth2005; Doyle & Cicchetti, Reference Doyle and Cicchetti2017). A child’s own ability for mentalizing and epistemic trust, combined with access to a mentalizing environment, is likely to encourage this reorganization. In a similar vein, Cicchetti, alongside various colleagues, developed and validated a narrative coding system to evaluate the effective resolution of trauma. In longitudinal studies, these researchers discovered that resolving maternal trauma had a protective-enhancing impact on maternal sensitivity (Swerbenski et al., Reference Swerbenski, Sturge-Apple, Messina, Toth, Rogosch and Cicchetti2023).

These findings align with a growing body of research indicating that parental reflective functioning, and specifically caregivers’ ability to reflect on their own traumatic experiences, plays a protective role in the dynamics between parental trauma and child outcomes (Berthelot et al., Reference Berthelot, Savard, Lemieux, Garon-Bissonnette, Ensink and Godbout2022; Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Cohen, Pettit, Normandin, Target, Fonagy and Ensink2019; Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Begin, Normandin and Fonagy2017).

Various lines of research have substantiated the significance of parental mentalizing or reflective functioning – a caregiver’s ability to reflect on their own internal mental experiences as well as those of their child – in forecasting developmental outcomes (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Nijssens, Fonagy and Mayes2017; Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2008; Slade, Reference Slade2005; Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). Findings from diverse fields, each with unique emphases and assessment methods, converge on this point. Meins (Reference Meins2013) and colleagues’ research has focused on the capacity for parental mind-mindedness, while Oppenheim et al. (Reference Oppenheim, Koren-Karie and Sagi2001) proposed the construct of parental insightfulness, and Slade (Reference Slade2005) coined the notion of parental reflective functioning. Cicchetti and colleagues’ studies in this context focused on the role of mental-state language or internal-state lexicon (Beeghly & Cicchetti, Reference Beeghly and Cicchetti1994; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Ng, Lisle, Koenig, Sannes, Rogosch and Cicchetti2023).

Parental mentalizing represents a relationship-specific manifestation of a more general capacity for mentalizing. It is thought to foster the development of secure attachment, the child’s own capacity for reflective functioning, and, as noted, a climate conducive to the development of epistemic trust, adaptive emotion regulation, and positive interpersonal functioning. Although parenting practices may vary across cultures, their essential role in the development of mentalizing seems to be a universal phenomenon. A unique human trait appears to be our tendency to create narratives or stories about our intentionality or mental state language. The belief that another understands us or shares our narrative, or at least that we are aware of, is likely to lead to the establishment of the we-mode, which facilitates learning from others. The crucial factor is the recognition of oneself in another. Of course, the self as imagined may be significantly altered by the desire to identify, the propensity for mimicry, the innate tendency for synchrony, and might not initially be an accurate representation. Nonetheless, if the narrated imagined self aligns with the image of the self in the mind of the other, an identification or match occurs, enabling social learning and the merging of minds which is manifested in synchrony, a type of co-regulation between infants and their caregivers, through which infants learn emotion management strategies from interactions with caregivers (Endevelt-Shapira & Feldman, Reference Endevelt-Shapira and Feldman2023; Feldman, Reference Feldman2017). Taken together, these findings further support the emphasis on the agentive self in recent views on personality and personality development (Sharp & Wall, Reference Sharp and Wall2021) and thus provide promising avenues for further research concerning the role of the closely associated human capacities for meaning-making, agency, and social learning in psychological development.

Research on large language models, together with another newly emerging field, that of developmental robotics (Cangelosi & Schlesinger, Reference Cangelosi and Schlesinger2018), may be able to help us better understand the role of (parental) mentalizing and mental-state language more generally in creating the feeling of being understood and the feelings of competency and agency this seems to engender. At the same time, developmental psychopathology may also be helpful in improving current large language models by examining their underlying assumptions (Frank, Reference Frank2023).

The need to improve prevention and intervention strategies

Over the past decades, there has been a significant increase in the development and empirical assessment of prevention and intervention strategies for psychological disorders and issues in children and young people. However, similar to the situation in adults (Cuijpers, Reference Cuijpers2019; Leichsenring et al., Reference Leichsenring, Steinert, Rabung and Ioannidis2022), there remains considerable room to enhance these strategies for children and young people. The finding that the effectiveness of psychological interventions for children and young people has on average not improved despite the past five decades of research is particularly concerning (Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, Kuppens, Ng, Eckshtain, Ugueto, Vaughn-Coaxum, Jensen-Doss, Hawley, Krumholz Marchette, Chu, Weersing and Fordwood2017, Reference Weisz, Kuppens, Ng, Vaughn-Coaxum, Ugueto, Eckshtain and Corteselli2019). Additionally, the implementation of evidence-based intervention strategies in routine clinical care is significantly behind scientific advancements, and many children and young people, along with their caregivers, especially those in socioeconomically challenging situations, have limited or no access to effective psychological care (Fonagy & Luyten, Reference Fonagy and Luyten2021; Kazdin & Blase, Reference Kazdin and Blase2011; Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2011; Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, Kuppens, Ng, Eckshtain, Ugueto, Vaughn-Coaxum, Jensen-Doss, Hawley, Krumholz Marchette, Chu, Weersing and Fordwood2017).

Looking ahead, there is likely to be a shift from specific school-based interventions toward more comprehensive and integrative mechanism-based interventions. In a recent extensive study involving 348 treatment-control comparisons from 263 randomized studies in young people, for example, the impact of five empirically supported principles of change was examined. The principles are simple: reducing arousal, make desirable behavior rewarding, changing distorted cognitions, skill building in problem solving, and encouraging activities that counter unhelpful behavior. The results indicated that treatments integrating all five of these principles had effects approximately twice as large as those based on fewer principles (Fitzpatrick et al., Reference Fitzpatrick, Cho, Venturo-Conerly, Ugueto, Ng and Weisz2022). These findings align with research suggesting that modular, transdiagnostic interventions could be more effective than disorder-specific approaches (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Weisz, Carvalho, Garibaldi, Bearman and Chorpita2020).

Improving social support, and increasing the ability of children, young people and their families to benefit from their social environment in particular, may be a final common pathway to positive mental health, as well as a result of processes that enhance aspects of mental health, such as self-esteem and relationship-recruiting capacities. Those who endure prolonged social support deficits and loneliness often develop epistemic mistrust and inappropriate epistemic credulity, which, instead of fostering social engagement, trigger fear-based responses to situations, leading to a further breakdown of trust in relationships and increased isolation (Cacioppo et al., Reference Cacioppo, Capitanio and Cacioppo2014; Cacioppo, Cacioppo et all., Reference Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Capitanio and Cole2015; Cacioppo, Grippo, et al., Reference Cacioppo, Grippo, London, Goossens and Cacioppo2015; Smith & Pollak, Reference Smith and Pollak2021b). A meta-analysis by Skeen and colleagues (Skeen et al., Reference Skeen, Laurenzi, Gordon, du Toit, Tomlinson, Dua, Fleischmann, Kohl, Ross, Servili, Brand, Dowdall, Lund, van der Westhuizen, Carvajal-Aguirre, Eriksson de Carvalho and Melendez-Torres2019) of the active components of universal interventions to improve adolescent mental health, identified the development of adolescents’ social skills to support improved interpersonal relationships as crucial in enhancing their mental health. These findings align with our view of the role of social support. We believe this process is bidirectional: providing skills to engage in social relationships also increases the capacity for mentalizing and discerning personally relevant social communication (i.e., epistemic trust). In several studies, interventions aimed at rebuilding such relationships, transforming them from those based on power imbalances to reciprocal ones, have enabled the establishment of trust (Bauer et al., Reference Bauer, Stevens, Purtscheller, Knapp, Fonagy, Evans-Lacko and Paulsubmitted).

These findings are consistent with our view that all effective psychotherapeutic interventions involve changes in social learning across three hypothetical communication systems (Fonagy, Luyten, et al., Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Allison2015; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy2020). Communication system 1 focuses on reducing epistemic mistrust by presenting a model of the mind that instills a sense of recognition and independence in the patient. We concur with the viewpoint that prevention and intervention strategies should maximize the use of various empirically supported principles of change, fostering feelings of significant mirroring, and thereby enhancing the patient’s sense of agency and autonomy and reducing arousal and threat. This differs from historical approaches that often rely on a limited number of these principles. The revival of agency in the patient is believed to reactivate communication system 2, promoting mentalizing and social learning through augmented epistemic trust and opening the individual’s mind to the possibility of lasting change in their understanding. Enhanced mentalizing and epistemic trust, in turn, lead to engagement with communication system 3, which involves reconnecting with the social world, allowing the patient to interact with their wider social environment in new ways. For individuals with a background of early adversity and deprivation, this treatment phase also includes interventions at the social environmental level, counteracting social thinning and aiding the patient in developing a more adaptive social context and interpersonal relationships. This theoretical model offers a transtheoretical, transdiagnostic, and developmental view of psychotherapeutic change, suggesting that similar processes and mechanisms are operative in both normal psychological development and psychological interventions.

While precision medicine approaches, which tailor treatment to the specific needs of patients, may enhance the effectiveness of therapeutic interventions within each of these three communication systems, considering the complexity of developmental trajectories discussed earlier in this paper, such approaches seem more akin to science fiction than an imminent reality. However, those of us working in this field need to continue investing in precision medicine techniques. For example, the success of attachment-based approaches, grounded in research using the Adult Attachment Interview (George et al., Reference George, Kaplan and Main1985), has been notable in predicting a broad range of developmental outcomes by analyzing only four parameters of an individual’s narrative as per Grice’s maxims (Reference Grice1989) (i.e., quantity, quality, relation, and manner).

As mentioned earlier, we must acknowledge that the effectiveness of psychosocial interventions for children and young people has not shown significant improvement in recent decades (Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, Kuppens, Ng, Vaughn-Coaxum, Ugueto, Eckshtain and Corteselli2019). Therefore, we have a pressing responsibility to enhance the efficacy of such interventions in the coming years. At the same time, we must acknowledge that psychosocial interventions might have reached their maximum effectiveness. In this context, the field needs to significantly expand prevention strategies. For example, population-attributable fractions of early adversity (the degree to which psychopathological outcomes could be reduced if early adversity were prevented) typically range from 20 to 50% (Dragioti et al., Reference Dragioti, Radua, Solmi, Arango, Oliver, Cortese, Jones, Il Shin, Correll and Fusar-Poli2022). Socioeconomic disparities have a similar impact on the prevalence of psychopathology and help-seeking behaviors (Evans-Lacko et al., Reference Evans-Lacko, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Benjet, Bruffaerts, Chiu, Florescu, de Girolamo, Gureje, Haro, He, Hu, Karam, Kawakami, Lee, Lund, Kovess-Masfety, Levinson and Thornicroft2018; Lund et al., Reference Lund, Brooke-Sumner, Baingana, Baron, Breuer, Chandra, Haushofer, Herrman, Jordans, Kieling, Medina-Mora, Morgan, Omigbodun, Tol, Patel and Saxena2018). The remarkable effectiveness of prevention strategies in reducing the prevalence of somatic diseases has not been matched by equivalent success in the realm of psychological disorders. Despite considerable advances in understanding the crucial role of secure attachment relationships and an emotionally supportive environment in child development, it is surprising that many children still grow up in environments lacking these elements. For instance, while the impacts of physical and sexual maltreatment are well recognized and declining in prevalence, the comparably serious consequences of verbal abuse receive less attention and its frequency is likely increasing (Li et al., Reference Li, Dube, Singh, Khamisa, Fiorini and Fonagy2022). It is notable that many parents and children are often more knowledgeable about their cellphones than about their own and each other’s minds.

Discussion and conclusions

In this paper, we have outlined various potential directions for the future of developmental psychopathology, while acknowledging that our perspectives are inherently shaped by our own views, interests, and sociocultural context. We recognize that many of the potential developments we have identified may not come to fruition. Nevertheless, we earnestly hope for significant improvements in both the availability and effectiveness of prevention and intervention strategies in the years ahead. As Abraham Lincoln aptly stated, “The best way to predict the future is to create it.” Developmental psychopathologists play a crucial role in this endeavor, and Dante Cicchetti’s work continues to be a guiding force. His insights will keep inspiring us to advance from developmental discoveries to a deeper understanding of mental disorders, striving towards the ultimate aim of scalable prevention.

References

Allen, J. G. (2021). Trusting in psychotherapy. American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, A., Stevens, M., Purtscheller, D., Knapp, M., & Fonagy, P., Evans-Lacko, S., & Paul, J. (2021). Mobilising social support to improve mental health for children and adolescents: A systematic review using principles of realist synthesis. PLoS One, 16(5), e0251750. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0251750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 6(1), 530. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940000585X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthelot, N., Savard, C., Lemieux, R., Garon-Bissonnette, J., Ensink, K., & Godbout, N. (2022). Development and validation of a self-report measure assessing failures in the mentalization of trauma and adverse relationships. Child Abuse & Neglect, 128, 105017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.105017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleidorn, W., Hopwood, C. J., Back, M. D., Denissen, J. J. A., Hennecke, M., Jokela, M., Kandler, C., Lucas, R. E., Luhmann, M., Orth, U., Roberts, B. W., Wagner, J., Wrzus, C., & Zimmermann, J. (2020). Longitudinal Experience-Wide Association Studies—a framework for studying personality change. European Journal of Personality, 34(3), 285300. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2247 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleys, D., Luyten, P., Soenens, B., & Claes, S. (2018). Gene-environment interactions between stress and 5-HTTLPR in depression: A meta-analytic update. Journal of Affective Disorders, 226, 339345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.050 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonanno, G. A., & Diminich, E. D. (2013). Annual Research Review: Positive adjustment to adversity—trajectories of minimal-impact resilience and emergent resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(4), 378401. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borelli, J. L., Cohen, C., Pettit, C., Normandin, L., Target, M., Fonagy, P., & Ensink, K. (2019). Maternal and child sexual abuse history: An intergenerational exploration of children’s adjustment and maternal trauma-reflective functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1062. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01062 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32(7), 513531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.7.513 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2007). The bioecological model of human development. In Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Theoretical models of human development, 1, 6th edn. WIley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0114 Google Scholar
Brown, M. P., Ng, R., Lisle, J., Koenig, M., Sannes, D., Rogosch, F., & Cicchetti, D. (2023). Mind-mindedness in a high-risk sample: Differential benefits for developmental outcomes based on child maltreatment. Developmental Psychology, 59(6), 11261135. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001506 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cole, S. W. (2015). The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 733767. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015240 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Toward a neurology of loneliness. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 14641504. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037618 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, S., Grippo, A. J., London, S., Goossens, L., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2015). Loneliness: Clinical import and interventions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 238249. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615570616 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, C., Tanzer, M., Saunders, R., Booker, T., Allison, E., Li, E., O’Dowda, C., Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2021). Development and validation of a self-report measure of epistemic trust. PLOS One, 16(4), e0250264. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250264 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cangelosi, A., & Schlesinger, M. (2018). From babies to robots: The contribution of developmental robotics to developmental psychology. Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 183188. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12282 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., Meier, M. H., Ramrakha, S., Shalev, I., Poulton, R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613497473 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2018). All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(9), 831844. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17121383 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choate, A. M., Bornovalova, M. A., Hipwell, A. E., Chung, T., & Stepp, S. D. (2023). The general psychopathology factor (p) from adolescence to adulthood: Exploring the developmental trajectories of p using a multi-method approach. Development and Psychopathology, 35(4), 17751793. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000463 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2008). Integrating biological measures into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 737743. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000357 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8(4), 597600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2005). Child maltreatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1(1), 409438. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coan, J. A., & Sbarra, D. A. (2015). Social baseline theory: The social regulation of risk and effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 8791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constantinou, M. P., Goodyer, I. M., Eisler, I., Butler, S., Kraam, A., Scott, S., Pilling, S., Simes, E., Ellison, R., Allison, E., & Fonagy, P. (2019). Changes in general and specific psychopathology factors over a psychosocial intervention. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(8), 776786. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.11.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corriveau, K. H., Harris, P. L., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Elliott, L., Liddle, B., Hearn, A., Vittorini, L., & de Rosnay, M. (2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: Longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80(3), 750761. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01295.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(4), 148153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuijpers, P. (2019). Targets and outcomes of psychotherapies for mental disorders: An overview. World Psychiatry, 18(3), 276285. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20661 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dagan, O., & Sagi-Schwartz, A. (2021). Early attachment networks to multiple caregivers: History, assessment models, and future research recommendations. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2021(180), 919. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20446 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeGarmo, D. S., Gewirtz, A. H., Li, L., Tavalire, H. F., & Cicchetti, D. (2023). The ADAPT parenting intervention benefits combat exposed fathers genetically susceptible to problem drinking. Prevention Science, 24(1), 150160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-022-01424-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, C. A., Cicchetti, D., Kubzansky, L. D., Seedat, S., Teicher, M. H., Williams, D. R., & Koenen, K. C. (2020). Psychological resilience: An update on definitions, a critical appraisal, and research recommendations. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1822064. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2020.1822064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doyle, C., & Cicchetti, D. (2017). From the cradle to the grave: The effect of adverse caregiving environments on attachment and relationships throughout the lifespan. Clinical Psychology, 24(2), 203217. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpsp.12192 Google Scholar
Dragioti, E., Radua, J., Solmi, M., Arango, C., Oliver, D., Cortese, S., Jones, P. B., Il Shin, J., Correll, C. U., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2022). Global population attributable fraction of potentially modifiable risk factors for mental disorders: A meta-umbrella systematic review. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(8), 35103519. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01586-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endevelt-Shapira, Y., & Feldman, R. (2023). Mother-infant brain-to-brain synchrony patterns reflect caregiving profiles. Biology (Basel), 12(2), 284. https://doi.org/10.3390/biology12020284 Google ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Begin, M., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Parental reflective functioning as a moderator of child internalizing difficulties in the context of child sexual abuse. Psychiatry Research, 257, 361366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Evans, S. C., Weisz, J. R., Carvalho, A. C., Garibaldi, P. M., Bearman, S. K., & Chorpita, B. F. (2020). Effects of standard and modular psychotherapies in the treatment of youth with severe irritability. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 88(3), 255268. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000456 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans-Lacko, S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bruffaerts, R., Chiu, W. T., Florescu, S., de Girolamo, G., Gureje, O., Haro, J. M., He, Y., Hu, C., Karam, E. G., Kawakami, N., Lee, S., Lund, C., Kovess-Masfety, V., Levinson, D.Thornicroft, G. (2018). Socio-economic variations in the mental health treatment gap for people with anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders: Results from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Psychological Medicine, 48(9), 15601571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003336 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81(2), 435456. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R. (2017). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(2), 8099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiorini, G., Saunders, R., Fonagy, P., & Midgley, N. (2022). Trajectories of change in general psychopathology levels among depressed adolescents in short-term psychotherapies. Psychotherapy Research, 33(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2022.2040751 Google ScholarPubMed
Fitzpatrick, O. M., Cho, E., Venturo-Conerly, K. E., Ugueto, A. M., Ng, M. Y., & Weisz, J. R. (2022). Empirically supported principles of change in youth psychotherapy: Exploring codability, frequency of use, and meta-analytic findings. Clinical Psychological Science, 11(2), 326344. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026221120230 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonagy, P., Campbell, C., Constantinou, M., Higgitt, A., Allison, E., & Luyten, P. (2022). Culture and psychopathology: An attempt at reconsidering the role of social learning. Development and Psychopathology, 34(4), 12051220. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000092 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2018). Conduct problems in youth and the RDoC approach: A developmental, evolutionary-based view. Clinical Psychology Review, 64, 5776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2021). Socioeconomic and sociocultural factors affecting access to psychotherapies: The way forward. World Psychiatry, 20(3), 315316. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20911 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., & Allison, E. (2015). Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: A new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29(5), 575609. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2015.29.5.575 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., Allison, E., & Campbell, C. (2017). What we have changed our minds about: Part 1. Borderline personality disorder as a limitation of resilience. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 4(1), 11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-017-0061-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Speranza, M., Luyten, P., Kaess, M., Hessels, C., & Bohus, M. (2015). ESCAP Expert Article: Borderline personality disorder in adolescence: An expert research review with implications for clinical practice. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(11), 13071320. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-015-0751-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B. W. (2005). Patterns of continuity: A dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological Review, 112(1), 6074. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.60 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, M. C. (2023). Baby steps in evaluating the capacities of large language models. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(8), 451452. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00211-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freud, A. (1981). The concept of developmental lines: Their diagnostic significance. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 36(1), 129136. https://doi.org/10.1080/00797308.1981.11823333 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freud, S. (1920). The psychogenesis of a case of homosexuality in a woman. In Strachey, J. (Eds.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freudl (vol. 18, pp. 145172). Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). The adult attachment interview. Department of Psychology, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. (2013). Ostensive communication and cultural learning: The natural pedagogy hypothesis. In Metcalfe, J., & Terrace, H. S. (Eds.), Agency and joint attention (pp. 139151). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golds, L., MacBeth, A., & de Kruiff, K. (2020). Disentangling genes, attachment, and environment: A systematic review of the developmental psychopathology literature on gene-environment interactions and attachment. Development and Psychopathology, 32(1), 357381. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000142 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Groh, A. M., Roisman, G. I., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Fearon, R. P. (2012). The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 83(2), 591610. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01711.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guild, D. J., Alto, M. E., Handley, E. D., Rogosch, F., Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2021). Attachment and affect between mothers with depression and their children: Longitudinal outcomes of child parent psychotherapy. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 49(5), 563577. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00681-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Handley, E. D., Russotti, J., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2019). Developmental cascades from child maltreatment to negative friend and romantic interactions in emerging adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 31(5), 16491659. https://doi.org/10.1017/s095457941900124x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Handley, E. D., Russotti, J., Ross, A. J., Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2023). A person-centered data analytic approach to dopaminergic polygenic moderation of child maltreatment exposure. Developmental Psychobiology, 65(5), e22403. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22403 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawks, S., & Meehan, C. L. (2014). Maternal and allomaternal responsiveness: The significance of cooperative caregiving in attachment theory. In Keller, H., & Otto, H. (Eds.), Different faces of attachment: Cultural variations on a universal human need (pp. 113140). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139226684.008 Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2020). The WEIRDest people in the world: How the West became psychologically peculiar and particularly prosperous. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Hogg, B., Gardoki-Souto, I., Valiente-Gómez, A., Rosa, A. R., Fortea, L., Radua, J., Amann, B. L., & Moreno-Alcázar, A. (2023). Psychological trauma as a transdiagnostic risk factor for mental disorder: An umbrella meta-analysis. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 273(2), 397410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-022-01495-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopwood, C. J., Wright, A. G. C., & Bleidorn, W. (2022). Person-environment transactions differentiate personality and psychopathology. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(1), 5563. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-021-00004-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2016). Variable postpartum responsiveness among humans and other primates with “cooperative breeding”: A comparative and evolutionary perspective. Hormones and Behavior, 77, 272283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutsebaut, J., Debbané, M., & Sharp, C. (2020). Designing a range of mentalizing interventions for young people using a clinical staging approach to borderline pathology. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 7(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-020-0121-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., Belsky, J., Szekely, E., Widaman, K. F., Pluess, M., Greenwood, C., & Wazana, A. (2020). Distinguishing differential susceptibility, diathesis-stress, and vantage sensitivity: Beyond the single gene and environment model. Development and Psychopathology, 32(1), 7383. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579418001438 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Evidence-based treatment research: Advances, limitations, and next steps. American Psychologist, 66(8), 685698. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024975 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. E., & Blase, S. L. (2011). Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice to reduce the burden of mental illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 2137. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393527 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & An, D. (in press). The parent’s and the child’s internal working models of each other moderate cascades from child difficulty to socialization outcomes: Preliminary evidence for dual moderation? Development and Psychopathology, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422001365 Google Scholar
Konner, M. (2010). The evolution of childhood. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Moore, T. M., Kaczkurkin, A. N., & Zald, D. H. (2021). Hierarchical models of psychopathology: Empirical support, implications, and remaining issues. World Psychiatry, 20(1), 5763. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20824 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leichsenring, F., Steinert, C., Rabung, S., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2022). The efficacy of psychotherapies and pharmacotherapies for mental disorders in adults: An umbrella review and meta-analytic evaluation of recent meta-analyses. World Psychiatry, 21(1), 133145. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20941 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leighton, C., Botto, A., Silva, J. R., Jimenez, J. P., & Luyten, P. (2017). Vulnerability or sensitivity to the environment? Methodological issues, trends, and recommendations in gene-environment interactions research in human behavior. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8(106), 106. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00106 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, E. T., Dube, A. T., Singh, N., Khamisa, K., Fiorini, G., & Fonagy, P. (2022). Childhood verbal abuse: A systematic review of the current evidence. Report for Verbal Abuse Foundation. Division of Psychology and Language Sciences,Google Scholar
Lund, C., Brooke-Sumner, C., Baingana, F., Baron, E. C., Breuer, E., Chandra, P., Haushofer, J., Herrman, H., Jordans, M., Kieling, C., Medina-Mora, M. E., Morgan, E., Omigbodun, O., Tol, W., Patel, V., & Saxena, S. (2018). Social determinants of mental disorders and the Sustainable Development Goals: A systematic review of reviews. Lancet Psychiatry, 5(4), 357369. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30060-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Campbell, C., Allison, E., & Fonagy, P. (2020). The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: State of the art and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 297325. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071919-015355 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Campbell, C., & Fonagy, P. (2021). Rethinking the relationship between attachment and personality disorder. Current Opinion in Psychology, 37, 109113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.11.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., & Fonagy, P. (2022). Integrating and differentiating personality and psychopathology: A psychodynamic perspective. Journal of Personality, 90(1), 7588. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12656 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Nijssens, L., Fonagy, P., & Mayes, L. C. (2017). Parental reflective functioning: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 70(1), 174199. https://doi.org/10.1080/00797308.2016.1277901 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madigan, S., Deneault, A. A., Racine, N., Park, J., Thiemann, R., Zhu, J., Dimitropoulos, G., Williamson, T., Fearon, P., Cénat, J. M., McDonald, S., Devereux, C., & Neville, R. D. (2023). Adverse childhood experiences: A meta-analysis of prevalence and moderators among half a million adults in 206 studies. World Psychiatry, 22(3), 463471. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.21122 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J. S., Ringen, E. J., Duda, P., & Jaeggi, A. V. (2020). Harsh environments promote alloparental care across human societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1933), 20200758. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.0758 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, J., Yusupov, N., Binder, E. B., Brückl, T. M., & Czamara, D. (2022). Early adversity as the prototype gene × environment interaction in mental disorders? Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 215, 173371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2022.173371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Lucke, C. M., Nelson, K. M., & Stallworthy, I. C. (2021). Resilience in development and psychopathology: Multisystem perspectives. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 521549. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-120307 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E., De Brito, S. A., & Viding, E. (2012). The link between child abuse and psychopathology: A review of neurobiological and genetic research. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 105(4), 151156. https://doi.org/10.1258/jrsm.2011.110222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrory, E., Foulkes, L., & Viding, E. (2022). Social thinning and stress generation after childhood maltreatment: A neurocognitive social transactional model of psychiatric vulnerability. Lancet Psychiatry, 9(10), 828837. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(22)00202-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E. (2013). Sensitive attunement to infantsʼ internal states: Operationalizing the construct of mind-mindedness. Attachment and Human Development, 15(5-6), 524544. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013.830388 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J., Minter, T., & Angnged, A. (2016). Received sensitivity: Adapting Ainsworth’s scale to capture sensitivity in a multiple-caregiver context. Attachment and Human Development, 18(2), 101114. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.1133681 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesman, J., Minter, T., Angnged, A., Cisse, I. A. H., Salali, G. D., & Migliano, A. B. (2018). Universality without uniformity: A culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89(3), 837850. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12795 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norton, P. J., & Paulus, D. J. (2016). Toward a unified treatment for emotional disorders: Update on the science and practice. Behavior Therapy, 47(6), 854868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015.07.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Madagain, C., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Joint attention to mental content and the social origin of reasoning. Synthese, 198(5), 40574078. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02327-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oppenheim, D., Koren-Karie, N., & Sagi, A. (2001). Mothers’ empathic understanding of their preschoolers’ internal experience: Relations with early attachment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25(1), 1626. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250042000096 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paniagua, F. A. (2000). Culture-bound syndromes, cultural variations, and psychopathology. In Cuéllar, I., & Paniagua, F. A. (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural mental health (pp. 139169). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012199370-2/50009-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patalay, P., Fonagy, P., Deighton, J., Belsky, J., Vostanis, P., & Wolpert, M. (2015). A general psychopathology factor in early adolescence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 207(1), 1522. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.114.149591 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picardi, A., Giuliani, E., & Gigantesco, A. (2020). Genes and environment in attachment. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 112, 254269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D. (2015). Developmental psychopathology: Recent advances and future challenges. World Psychiatry, 14(3), 262269. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20237 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raby, K. L., Cicchetti, D., Carlson, E. A., Cutuli, J. J., Englund, M. M., & Egeland, B. (2012). Genetic and caregiving-based contributions to infant attachment: Unique associations with distress reactivity and attachment security. Psychological Science, 23(9), 10161023. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612438265 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russotti, J., Warmingham, J. M., Duprey, E. B., Handley, E. D., Manly, J. T., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2021). Child maltreatment and the development of psychopathology: The role of developmental timing and chronicity. Child Abuse & Neglect, 120, 105215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.105215 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiele, M. A., Gottschalk, M. G., & Domschke, K. (2020). The applied implications of epigenetics in anxiety, affective and stress-related disorders - A review and synthesis on psychosocial stress, psychotherapy and prevention. Clinical Psychology Review, 77, 101830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2008). The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Social Development, 17(3), 737754. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00457.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharp, C., & Wall, K. (2021). DSM-5 level of personality functioning: Refocusing personality disorder on what it means to be human. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 313337. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-105402 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skeen, S., Laurenzi, C. A., Gordon, S. L., du Toit, S., Tomlinson, M., Dua, T., Fleischmann, A., Kohl, K., Ross, D., Servili, C., Brand, A. S., Dowdall, N., Lund, C., van der Westhuizen, C., Carvajal-Aguirre, L., Eriksson de Carvalho, C., & Melendez-Torres, G. J. (2019). Adolescent mental health program components and behavior risk reduction: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 144(2)  , e20183488. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-3488 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slade, A. (2005). Parental reflective functioning: An introduction. Attachment and Human Development, 7(3), 269281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeeth, D., Beck, S., Karam, E. G., & Pluess, M. (2021). The role of epigenetics in psychological resilience. Lancet Psychiatry, 8(7), 620629. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30515-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, G. T., Atkinson, E. A., Davis, H. A., Riley, E. N., & Oltmanns, J. R. (2020). The general factor of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 7598. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071119-115848 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2021a). Rethinking concepts and categories for understanding the neurodevelopmental effects of childhood adversity. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1), 6793. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620920725 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. E., & Pollak, S. D. (2021b). Social relationships and children’s perceptions of adversity. Child Development Perspectives, 15(4), 228234. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12427 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, D., Clement, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359393. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01394.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sprooten, E., Franke, B., & Greven, C. U. (2022). The P-factor and its genomic and neural equivalents: An integrated perspective. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(1), 3848. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01031-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swerbenski, H. G., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Messina, G., Toth, S. L., Rogosch, F., & Cicchetti, D. (2023). Maternal childhood maltreatment trauma resolution: Development of a novel narrative coding measure and implications for intergenerational parenting processes. Development and Psychopathology, 1-16, 116. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001256 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2010). Origins of human communication. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2020a). The adaptive origins of uniquely human sociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 375(1803), 20190493. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2020b). The role of roles in uniquely human cognition and sociality. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 50(1), 219. https://doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12223 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhage, M. L., Fearon, R. M. P., Schuengel, C., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., Madigan, S., Roisman, G. I., Oosterman, M., Behrens, K. Y., Wong, M. S., Mangelsdorf, S., Priddis, L. E., Brisch, K‐H., The Collaboration on Attachment Transmission Synthesis (2018). Examining ecological constraints on the intergenerational transmission of attachment via individual participant data meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(6), 20232037. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13085 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waddington, C. H. (1957). The strategy of the genes. Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., Kuppens, S., Ng, M. Y., Eckshtain, D., Ugueto, A. M., Vaughn-Coaxum, R., Jensen-Doss, A., Hawley, K. M., Krumholz Marchette, L. S., Chu, B. C., Weersing, V. R., & Fordwood, S. R. (2017). What five decades of research tells us about the effects of youth psychological therapy: A multilevel meta-analysis and implications for science and practice. American Psychologist, 72(2), 79117. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040360 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisz, J. R., Kuppens, S., Ng, M. Y., Vaughn-Coaxum, R. A., Ugueto, A. M., Eckshtain, D., & Corteselli, K. A. (2019). Are psychotherapies for young people growing stronger? Tracking trends over time for youth anxiety, depression, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and conduct problems. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(2), 216237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805436 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widiger, T. A., Sellbom, M., Chmielewski, M., Clark, L. A., DeYoung, C. G., Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Lynam, D. R., Miller, J. D., Mullins-Sweatt, S., Samuel, D. B., South, S. C., Tackett, J. L., Thomas, K. M., Watson, D., & Wright, A. G. C. (2018). Personality in a hierarchical model of psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(1), 7792. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618797105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, Y., Harms, M. B., Green, C. S., Wilson, R. C., & Pollak, S. D. (2023). Childhood unpredictability and the development of exploration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(49), e2303869120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2303869120 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. M., & Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant-parent attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 12451272. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed