Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:28:30.490Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Subjective social status and neural processing of race in Mexican American adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 September 2018

Keely A. Muscatell
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Ethan McCormick
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Eva H. Telzer*
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Eva H. Telzer or Keely A. Muscatell, Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, UNC Chapel Hill, 235 E. Cameron Ave CB 3270, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3270; E-mail: [email protected] | [email protected].

Abstract

Adolescence is a sensitive period for sociocultural development in which facets of social identity, including social status and race, become especially salient. Despite the heightened importance of both social status and race during this developmental period, no known work has examined how individual differences in social status influence perceptions of race in adolescents. Thus, in the present study, we investigated how both subjective social status and objective socioeconomic status (SES) influence neural responses to race. Twenty-three Mexican American adolescents (15 females; mean age = 17.22 years) were scanned using functional magnetic resonance imaging while they viewed Black and White faces in a standard labeling task. Adolescents rated their subjective social status in US society, while their parents responded to questions about their educational background, occupation, and economic strain (objective SES). Results demonstrated a negative association between subjective social status and neural responses in the amygdala, fusiform face area, and medial prefrontal cortex when adolescents viewed Black (relative to White) faces. In other words, adolescents with lower subjective social status showed greater activity in neural regions involved in processing salience, perceptual expertise, and thinking about the minds of others when they viewed images of Black faces, suggesting enhanced salience of race for these youth. There was no relationship between objective SES and neural responses to the faces. Moreover, instructing participants to focus on the gender or emotion expression on the face attenuated the relationship between subjective social status and neural processing of race. Together, these results demonstrate that subjective social status shapes the way the brain responds to race, which may have implications for psychopathology.

Type
Special Issue Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

We would like to acknowledge generous support from Andrew Fuligini, Adriana Galvan, and Matthew Lieberman during study conceptualization, design, and data acquisition. Funding for this study was provided, in part, by NICHD Grants R01HD057164-S and R01HD057164 (to AJ Fuligni), NSF Grants SES 1023293 (to AJ Fuligni and EHT) and BCS 1539651 (to EHT), and NIDA Grant R01DA039923 (to EHT).

References

Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychology, 19, 586592. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.19.6.586Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693716. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514Google Scholar
Alba, R., Jiménez, T. R., & Marrow, H. B. (2014). Mexican Americans as a paradigm for contemporary intra-group heterogeneity. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 37, 446466. doi:10.1080/01419870.2013.786111Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M. (2014). The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 670682. doi:10.1038/nrn3800Google Scholar
Apfelbaum, E. P., Pauker, K., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Learning (not) to talk about race: When older children underperform in social categorization. Developmental Psychology, 44, 15131518. doi:10.1037/a0012835Google Scholar
Åslund, C., Leppert, J., Starrin, B., & Nilsson, K. W. (2009). Subjective social status and shaming experiences in relation to adolescent depression. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 163, 5560. doi:10.1001/archpedi.163.1.55Google Scholar
Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242261. doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0603Google Scholar
Blair, I. V., Judd, C. M., & Fallman, J. L. (2004). The automaticity of race and afrocentric facial features in social judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 763778. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.763Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187207. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202Google Scholar
Bukach, C. M., Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. J. (2006). Beyond faces and modularity: The power of an expertise framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 159166. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.02.004Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M. (2013). A roadmap for the integration of culture into developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 13751398. doi:10.1017/S0954579413000679Google Scholar
Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Lee, R. M. (2017). Culture and biology interplay: An introduction. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23, 14. doi:10.1037/cdp0000121Google Scholar
Choudhury, S. (2009). Culturing the adolescent brain: What can neuroscience learn from anthropology? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 159167. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp030Google Scholar
Cikara, M., & Van Bavel, J. J. (2014). The neuroscience of intergroup relations: An integrative review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 245274. doi:10.1177/1745691614527464Google Scholar
Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y. M., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 179193. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.38.2.179Google Scholar
Cundiff, J. M., & Matthews, K. A. (2017). Is subjective social status a unique correlate of physical health? A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 36, 11091125. doi:10.1037/hea0000534Google Scholar
Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: Amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 5459. doi:10.1177/0963721411430832Google Scholar
Eberhardt, J. L. (2005). Imaging race. American Psychologist, 60, 181190. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.2.181Google Scholar
Freeman, J. B., Penner, A. M., Saperstein, A., Scheutz, M., & Ambady, N. (2011). Looking the part: Social status cues shape race perception. PLOS ONE, 6. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025107Google Scholar
French, S. E., Seidman, E., Allen, L., & Aber, J. L. (2006). The development of ethnic identity during adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 42, 110. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.1Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 671678. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.2003Google Scholar
Golash-Boza, T., & Darity, W. (2008). Latino racial choices: The effects of skin colour and discrimination on Latinos’ and Latinas’ racial self-identifications. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 31, 899934. doi:10.1080/01419870701568858Google Scholar
Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Chiao, J. Y., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2001). Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 845850. doi:10.1038/90565Google Scholar
Goodman, E., Adler, N. E., Kawachi, I., Frazier, A. L., Huang, B., & Colditz, G. A. (2001). Adolescents’ perceptions of social status: Development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics, 108, 18. doi:10.1542/peds.108.2.e31Google Scholar
Goodman, E., Amick, B. C., Rezendes, M. O., Levine, S., Kagan, J., Rogers, W. H., & Tarlov, A. R. (2000). Adolescents’ understanding of social class: A comparison of white upper middle class and working class youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 8083. doi:10.1016/S1054-139X(99)00116-0Google Scholar
Goodwin, S. A., Operario, D., & Fiske, S. T. (1998). Situational power and interpersonal dominance facilitate bias and inequality. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 677698. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.901998090Google Scholar
Guassi Moreira, J. F., Van Bavel, J. J., & Telzer, E. H. (2017). The neural development of “us and them.” Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 184196. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw134Google Scholar
Hariri, A. R., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2000). Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. NeuroReport, 11, 4348. doi:10.1097/00001756-200001170-00009Google Scholar
Hunter, M. (2007). The persistent problem of colorism: Skin tone, status, and inequality. Sociology Compass, 1, 237254. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9020.2007.00006.xGoogle Scholar
Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 616626. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.616Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 361, 21092128. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.1934Google Scholar
Krosch, A. R., & Amodio, D. M. (2014). Economic scarcity alters the perception of race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 90799084. doi:10.1073/pnas.1404448111Google Scholar
Kubota, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2012). The neuroscience of race. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 940948. doi:10.1038/nn.3136Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D., Eisenberger, N. I., Crockett, M. J., Tom, S. M., Pfeifer, J. H., & Way, B. M. (2007). Affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli. Psychological Science, 18, 421428. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01916.xGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A., Jarcho, J. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005). An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 720722. doi:10.1038/nn1465Google Scholar
Luttmer, E. F. P. (2005). Neighbors as negatives: Relative earnings and well-being. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 120, 9631002. doi:10.1093/qje/120.3.963Google Scholar
Manczak, E. M., Basu, D., & Chen, E. (2016). The price of perspective taking. Clinical Psychological Science, 4, 485492. doi:10.1177/2167702615595001Google Scholar
Manczak, E. M., DeLongis, A., & Chen, E. (2016). Does empathy have a cost? Diverging psychological and physiological effects within families. Health Psychology, 35, 211218. doi:10.1037/hea0000281Google Scholar
Mattan, B. D., Kubota, J. T., & Cloutier, J. (2017). How social status shapes person perception and evaluation: A social neuroscience perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 468507. doi:10.1177/1745691616677828Google Scholar
McKown, C., & Weinstein, R. S. (2003). The development and consequences of stereotype consciousness in middle childhood. Child Development, 74, 498515. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.7402012Google Scholar
McLaren, D. G., Schultz, A. P., Locascio, J. J., Sperling, R. A., & Atri, A. (2011). Repeated-measures designs overestimate between-subject effects in fMRI packages using one error term. Paper presented at the 17th Annual Meeting of Organization for Human Brain Mapping, Quebec City, Canada, June, 26–30, 2011. Retrieved from http://mrtoolsmghharvardedu/indexphp/GLM_FlexGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, J. P., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Contextual variations in implicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 455469. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.3.455Google Scholar
Muscatell, K. A., Dedovic, K., Slavich, G. M., Jarcho, M. R., Breen, E. C., Bower, J. E., … Eisenberger, N. I. (2015). Greater amygdala activity and dorsomedial prefrontal-amygdala coupling are associated with enhanced inflammatory responses to stress. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 43. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2014.06.201Google Scholar
Muscatell, K. A., Dedovic, K., Slavich, G. M., Jarcho, M. R., Breen, E. C., Bower, J. E., … Eisenberger, N. I. (2016). Neural mechanisms linking social status and inflammatory responses to social stress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 915922. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw025Google Scholar
Muscatell, K. A., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). A social neuroscience perspective on stress and health. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6, 890904. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2012.00467.xGoogle Scholar
Muscatell, K. A., Morelli, S. A., Falk, E. B., Way, B. M., Pfeifer, J. H., Galinsky, A. D., … Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). Social status modulates neural activity in the mentalizing network. NeuroImage, 60, 17711777. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.080Google Scholar
O'Brien, S. F., & Bierman, K. L. (1988). Conceptions and perceived influence of peer groups: Interviews with preadolescents and adolescents. Child Development, 59, 13601365. doi:10.2307/1130498Google Scholar
Pauker, K., Ambady, N., & Apfelbaum, E. P. (2010). Race and salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development. Child Development, 81, 17991813. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01511.xGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. K., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Hannay, J. W. (2017). Economic inequality increases risk taking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 46434648. doi:10.1073/pnas.1616453114Google Scholar
Penner, A. M., & Saperstein, A. (2008). How social status shapes race. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 1962819630. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805762105Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729738. doi:10.1162/089892900562552Google Scholar
Qu, Y., & Telzer, E. H. (2017). Developmental cultural neuroscience: Progress and prospect. In Causadias, J. M., Telzer, E. H., & Gonzales, N. (Eds.), The handbook of culture and biology (pp. 465587). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Quintana, S. M. (1994). A model of ethnic perspective-taking ability applied to Mexican-American children and youth. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18, 419448. doi:10.1016/0147-1767(94)90016-7Google Scholar
Ritterman, M. L., Fernald, L. C., Ozer, E. J., Adler, N. E., Gutierrez, J. P., & Syme, S. L. (2009). Objective and subjective social class gradients for substance use among Mexican adolescents. Social Science and Medicine, 68, 18431851. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.02.048Google Scholar
Rivas-Drake, D., Seaton, E. K., Markstrom, C., Quintana, S., Syed, M., Lee, R. M., … Sellers, R. M. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity in adolescence: Implications for psychosocial, academic, and health outcomes. Child Development, 85, 4057. doi:10.1111/cdev.12200Google Scholar
Roberts, R. E., Phinney, J. S., Masse, L. C., Chen, Y. R., Roberts, C. R., & Romero, A. (1999). The structure of ethnic identity of young adolescents from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 301322. doi:10.1177/0272431699019003001Google Scholar
Rowley, S., Kurtz-Costes, B., Mistry, R., & Faegans, L. (2007). Social status as a predictor of race and gender stereotypes in late childhood and early adolescence. Social Development, 16, 150168. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00376.xGoogle Scholar
Scroggins, W. A., Mackie, D. M., Allen, T. J., & Sherman, J. W. (2016). Reducing prejudice with labels: Shared group memberships attenuate implicit bias and expand implicit group boundaries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42, 219229. doi:10.1177/0146167215621048Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). A longitudinal daily diary study of family assistance and academic achievement among adolescents from Mexican, Chinese, and European backgrounds. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 560571. doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9391-7Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., & Garcia, H. A. V. (2009). Skin color and self-perceptions of immigrant and U.S.-born Latinas: The moderating role of racial socialization and ethnic identity. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 31, 357374. doi:10.1177/0739986309336913Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., Humphreys, K. L., Shapiro, M., & Tottenham, N. (2013). Amygdala sensitivity to race is not present in childhood but emerges over adolescence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 234244. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00311Google Scholar
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., … Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168, 242249. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006Google Scholar
Uhlmann, E., Dasgupta, N., Elgueta, A., Greenwald, A. G., & Swanson, J. (2002). Subgroup prejudice based on skin color among Hispanics in the United States and Latin America. Social Cognition, 20, 198226. doi:10.1521/soco.20.3.198.21104Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Quintana, S. M., Lee, R. M., Cross, W. E., Rivas-Drake, D., Schwartz, S. J., … Sellers, R. M. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An integrated conceptualization. Child Development, 85, 2139. doi:10.1111/cdev.12196Google Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2008). The neural substrates of in-group bias: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Psychological Science, 19, 11311139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02214.xGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2011). Modulation of the fusiform face area following minimal exposure to motivationally relevant faces: Evidence of in-group enhancement (not out-group disregard). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 33433354. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00016Google Scholar
Zerubavel, N., Bearman, P. S., Weber, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2015). Neural mechanisms tracking popularity in real-world social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1507215077. doi:10.1073/pnas.1511477112Google Scholar