Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T17:19:26.609Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between biased maternal and filial attributions and the aggressiveness of their interactions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Carol Mackinnon-Lewis*
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
Michael E. Lamb
Affiliation:
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Barry Arbuckle
Affiliation:
Miller Children's Hospital
Laila P. Baradaran
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
Brenda L. Volling
Affiliation:
The Carolina Consortium on Human Development
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Carol MacKinnon-Lewis, Family Research Center, Department of Child Development and Family Relations, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro, NC 27412.

Abstract

This study examined the relation between maternal and filial attributions and the aggressiveness of their interactions. We also examined whether or not certain setting conditions (e.g., maternal and child depression, maternal and child negative life events, marital conflict, socioeconomic status) predispose some mothers and children to make negative attributions and interact coercively. One hundred four mothers and sons (age 7–9 years) from married and divorced families participated. They completed questionnaire and interview data and were observed while participating in two gamelike tasks (e.g., Trouble, Etch-a-Sketch). Both maternal and child attributions were significantly related to their coercive interactions. The most aggressive dyads were those in which both mothers and sons perceived hostile intent in the other. The relations between attributions and coercive interactions were found to be moderated by marital conflict, and maternal education, such that the association between attributions and coercive behavior, was stronger when marital conflict was low and the mothers were better educated.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aber, L., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Socioemotional development in maltreated children: An empirical and theoretical analysis. In Fitzgerald, H., Lester, B., & Yogman, M. (Eds.). Theory and research in behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 2, pp. 147205). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amenson, C. S., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (1981). An investigation into the observed sex difference in prevalence of unipolar depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. D. (Eds.). (1990). Peer rejection in childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baron, R., & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Berndt, T., & Ladd, G. (Eds.). (1989). Peer relationships in child development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss, Vol. 3: Loss, sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brody, G. H., & Forehand, R. (1986). Maternal perceptions of child maladjustment as a function of the combined influence of child behavior and maternal depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 237240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Stoneman, Z., & Mackinnon, C. E. (1982). Role asymmetries in interactions among school-aged children, their younger siblings, and their friends. Child Development, 53, 13641370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bugental, D. B., & Shennum, W. A. (1984). Difficult children as elicitors and targets of adult communication patterns: An attributional-behavioral transactional analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49(1, Serial No. 205).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, L. (1960). The annotated Alice: Alice's adventures in wonderland and through the looking glass. New York: Clarkson N. Potter.Google Scholar
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., & Bem, D. J. (1986). Moving against the world: Life-course patterns of explosive children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 308313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, A., Phillips, S., Glasgow, R. E., & Johnson, S. M. (1983). Parental characteristics and interactional dysfunction in families with child behavior problems: A preliminary investigation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 153166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1990). The organization and coherence of socioemotional, cognitive, and representational development: Illustrations through a developmental psychopathology perspective on Down syndrome and child maltreatment. In Thompson, R. A. (Ed.), Socioemotional development (pp. 259366). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. A. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Continuities and changes in children's social status: A five-year longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 261282.Google Scholar
Dix, T. H., & Grusec, J. E. (1985). Parent attribution processes in the socialization of children. In Sigel, I. E. (Ed.), Parental belief system (pp. 202233). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dix, T. H., & Reinhold, D. P. (1991). Chronic and temporary influences on mother's attributions for children's disobedience. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 251270.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child Development, 51, 162170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (1983). Behavioral antecedents of peer social status. Child Development, 54, 13851399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1985). Facets of social interaction. In Schneider, B. H., Rubin, K. H., & Ledingham, J. E. (Eds.), Children's peer relations: Issues in assessment and intervention (pp. 322). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information processing model of social competence in children. In Perlmutter, M. (Ed.), Minnesota symposia in child psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 77125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child Development, 53, 620635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). The assessment of intention-cue detection skills in children: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 163173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., & Brown, M. M. (1986). Social competence in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elder, G., Caspi, A., & Van-Nguyen, T. (1986). Resourceful and vulnerable children: Family influence and hard times. In Silbereisen, R., Eyferth, K., & Rudinger, G. (Eds.), Developmental action in context: Integrative perspectives on youth development (pp. 167186). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elder, G. H. Jr., Liker, J. K., & Cross, C. E. (1984). Parent-child behavior in the Great Depression: Life course and intergenerational influences. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G. Jr. (Eds.), Life span development and behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 108158). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Forehand, R., Brody, G., & Smith, K. (1986). Contribution of child behavior and marital dissatisfaction to maternal perceptions of child maladjustment. Behavior Research Therapy, 24, 4348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gnepp, J., & Chilamkurti, C. (1988). Children's use of personality attributions to predict other people's emotional and behavioral reactions. Child Development, 59, 743754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gong-Guy, E., & Hammen, C. (1980). Causal perceptions of stressful events in depressed and nondepressed outpatients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 662669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hetherington, E. M. (1988). Life Events Scale for children. Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia, Charlottes ville.Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 219266). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Kovacs, M. (1981). Rating scales to assess depression in school-age children. Ada Paedopsychiatrica, 46, 305315.Google Scholar
Kovacs, M. (1985). The Children's Depression Inventory (GDI). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995999.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E., & Jacklin, C. (1980). Sex differences in aggression: A rejoinder and reprise. Child Development, 51, 964980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackinnon, C. E. (1988a) Child Attribution Measure. Unpublished manuscript, University of North Carolina, Greensboro.Google Scholar
Mackinnon, C. E. (1988b). Maternal Attribution Measure. Unpublished manuscript, University of North Carolina, Greensboro.Google Scholar
Mackinnon, C. E. (1989). An observational investigation of sibling interactions in married and divorced families. Developmental Psychology, 25, 3644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackinnon, C. E., Lamb, M. E., Belsky, J., & Baum, C. (1990). An affective-cognitive model of mother-child aggression. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Middlebrook, J. L., & Forehand, R. (1985). Maternal perception of deviance in child behavior as a function of stress and clinic versus nonclinic status of the child: An analogue study. Behavior Therapy, 16, 494502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach, Vol. 3: Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Patterson, G. R., & Dishion, T. J. (1985). The contribution of families and peers to adolescent delinquency. Criminology, 23, 6379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, B., & O'Leary, K. D. (1980). Marital discord and childhood behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 8, 287295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rholes, W. S., & Ruble, D. N. (1984). Children's understanding of dispositional characteristics of others. Child Development, 55, 550560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E. (1985). Coercion theory revisited: Toward a new theoretical perspective on the etiology of conduct disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 5, 597625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roff, M., Sells, S. B., & Golden, M. M. (1972). Social adjustment and personality development in children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., & Lollis, S. P. (1988). Origins and consequences of social withdrawal in childhood. In Belsky, J. & Nezworski, T. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental psychopathology. In Hetherington, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 4: Social and personality development (pp. 775912). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Williams, J. G., Barlow, D. G., & Agras, W. (1972). Behavioral measurement of severe depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 27, 330333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed