Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T11:57:09.574Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relations among parenting, academic performance, and psychopathology: An investigation of developmental cascades and their interplay with maternal and paternal parenting

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 February 2023

Jeong Jin Yu*
Affiliation:
Department of Educational Studies, Xi’an Jiaotong-Liverpool University, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China
*
Corresponding author: Jeong Jin Yu, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Little effort has been made to integrate developmental cascades with maternal/paternal parenting in a single investigation. The present study seeks to test cascading effects among academic and internalizing/externalizing symptoms and their associations with maternal/paternal parenting across three time points from 8 to 10 years. Data for this investigation came from a nationally representative prospective cohort study of children born in April through July of 2008 in South Korea who were followed up annually. The sample included 1,598 families (48.5% girls). Parents rated their parenting and teachers rated children’s internalizing/externalizing problems and academic performance. Structural equation modeling showed that externalizing problems were negatively related to academic performance. Academic performance was negatively related to internalizing problems and positively related to maternal/paternal authoritative parenting, which in turn led to children’s higher academic performance. Bidirectional relations were found between academic performance and externalizing problems and between paternal authoritative parenting and children’s internalizing problems. Findings suggested cascading effects and their associations with parenting were not attributable to child gender, intelligence, or socioeconomic differences. These findings lend support to adjustment erosion and academic incompetence models and underscore the need for greater attention to the role that fathering may play in children’s development and mothering.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

It is of great interest whether and how one domain of child functioning influences or spreads to other domains of child functioning over time. Such longitudinal cross-domain pathways are referred as “developmental cascades” which are best tested by analyzing longitudinal data on three or more constructs over three or more time points (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005). The current literature suggests that academic and psychopathology domains during childhood and adolescence spill over into each other (e.g., Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). Child development does not however occur in a vacuum, but rather through parenting. Bronfenbrenner (Reference Bronfenbrenner, Gauvain and Cole1994) developed a concept of mesosystem where children’s experiences in a particular microsystem, such as parenting at home, spill over to influence their behavior in another microsystem such as, teacher ratings of externalizing behavior at school, and vice versa. Thus, developmental cascades may arise from maternal/paternal parenting at home, which could shape children’s behavior and academic performance at school, which in turn affect parenting. Of particular note, while theory and research still lag behind, some researchers suggest differential influences of maternal parenting and paternal parenting on child adjustment in middle childhood or later (Collins & Russell, Reference Collins and Russell1991; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Nelson and Dix2018). Nevertheless, little effort has been made to integrate both mothers’ and fathers’ parenting processes with developmental cascades during this developmental stage in a single investigation. This study is the first to examine associations of maternal and paternal parenting processes reported by parents with children’s developmental cascades rated by teachers using a nationally representative sample of South Korean children in middle childhood.

Internalizing and externalizing behaviors and academic performance

Three hypotheses have been proposed for the mechanism of cascading processes among internalizing and externalizing problems and academic competence across time (Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). The adjustment erosion hypothesis stipulates that internalizing and/or externalizing behavioral problems impede academic achievement by interfering with cognitive functioning and classroom learning (Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). Supporting evidence demonstrated a developmental cascade progressing from externalizing symptoms to poorer academic achievement during childhood through adolescence (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; McCarty et al., Reference McCarty, Mason, Kosterman, Hawkins, Lengua and McCauley2008; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2010; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019). Whereas some studies showed a negative longitudinal link from internalizing symptoms in childhood to academic performance in adolescence (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; McCarty et al., Reference McCarty, Mason, Kosterman, Hawkins, Lengua and McCauley2008; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2010; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019), others reported null findings (Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016). There is speculation that negative cascading effects from internalizing symptoms to academic domains may be pronounced only for those who score in the clinical ranges on internalizing symptoms (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018). A meta-analysis showed small negative effects of emotional problems on academic outcomes among adolescents (Riglin et al., Reference Riglin, Petrides, Frederickson and Rice2014), which may not be extended to children in middle childhood. Indeed, some findings indicated that internalizing symptoms interfered with later academic performance only for adolescents but not for children (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018). This thus remains an area in need of further investigation.

Internalizing symptoms may counteract the development of externalizing problems possibly because of inhibition of disruptive behavior and withdrawal from deviant peers (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016). On the other hand, externalizing problems are likely to cause or exacerbate interpersonal difficulties, which in turn may increase risk for internalizing problems (Gromoske & Maguire-Jack, Reference Gromoske and Maguire-Jack2012; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). However, empirical findings regarding directional associations between internalizing and externalizing problems remain difficult to reconcile. Some studies reported no cascade from internalizing to subsequent externalizing problems (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; van Lier et al., Reference van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2012). There is likewise no evidence for reverse directionality (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; van Lier et al., Reference van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2012). Whereas some studies found positive pathways from externalizing to internalizing symptoms (Defoe et al., Reference Defoe, Farrington and Loeber2013; Gromoske & Maguire-Jack, Reference Gromoske and Maguire-Jack2012; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Brittain, McDougall and Duku2013; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016), other studies reported negative pathways (Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018). Thus, further research is deemed necessary.

The academic incompetence hypothesis, on the other hand, posits that academic performance problems can trigger or heighten internalizing and/or externalizing symptoms, given that academic achievement is such an important age-appropriate task of childhood (Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). There is some support for this stance in the literature where poor academic performance had spillover effects on internalizing (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; Maughan et al., Reference Maughan, Rowe, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber2003; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2010; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Brittain, McDougall and Duku2013; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019; van Lier et al., Reference van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2012) and externalizing (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Brittain, McDougall and Duku2013; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016; Yu & Gamble, Reference Yu and Gamble2010; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019) symptoms.

The shared risk hypothesis proposes that cascading effects might be accounted for by third variables such as intelligence, parenting, and family socioeconomic status (SES) (Masten & Curtis, Reference Masten and Curtis2000; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). While these factors are a set of potential causes implicated in academic attainment and psychopathology in childhood, it has been suggested that their influences on developmental cascades could be less evident (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; Maughan et al., Reference Maughan, Rowe, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber2003; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019). Only a few studies so far have simultaneously controlled for all these variables. The present work addresses this gap by including intelligence and family SES as covariates and testing bidirectional effects between developmental cascades and maternal/paternal parenting.

Association of parenting with internalizing/externalizing behaviors and academic performance

Authoritative parenting refers to a child-rearing style characterized by affectionate, responsive support, involvement, but firm behavioral control (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch and Darling1992). Parental cognitive stimulation can be defined as the degree to which parents actively engage in supporting their children’s learning (McGroder, Reference McGroder2000). Thus, these two concepts are entangled with each other (McGroder, Reference McGroder2000; Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch and Darling1992). The results of meta-analyses showed that authoritative or positive parenting had beneficial effects on children’s academic outcomes (Pinquart & Kauser, Reference Pinquart and Kauser2018) and internalizing (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017b) and externalizing (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017a) symptomatology. However, authoritative or positive parenting was found to be only weakly associated with subsequent internalizing and externalizing symptoms (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017a, Reference Pinquart2017b). Furthermore, the associations of internalizing or externalizing symptoms with positive dimensions of parenting were found to be bidirectional (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017a, Reference Pinquart2017b),

In the same manner, parenting and academic performance are most likely bidirectionally associated; yet, most existing research has exclusively focused on pathways from parenting to academic performance. In light of the limited evidence base, Xiong et al. (Reference Xiong, Qin, Wang and Ren2021) suggested the evocative effect of adolescent children’s academic achievement on parental involvement rather than the opposite direction. The present study represents an attempt to add further empirical evidence on bidirectional relationships between parenting and child adjustment such as academic performance and internalizing/externalizing problems. Of note, in the Northeast Asian countries where Confucian values are pervasive, parents put top priority on education as a fundamental pillar of development (Xiong et al., Reference Xiong, Qin, Wang and Ren2021; Yu, Reference Yu2010, Reference Yu2019; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019). It would therefore be worthwhile to investigate the nature of the relation of children’s academic performance with parenting.

While there still remains a dearth of research on paternal parenting and children’s academic performance and psychopathology, Zhu et al. (in press) showed that perceived support from fathers was more strongly related to adolescents’ academic engagement than support from mothers. In addition, Möller et al. (Reference Möller, Nikolić, Majdandžić and Bögels2016)’s meta-analysis reported that paternal parenting was associated more strongly with children’ anxiety symptoms than was maternal parenting. Because of the nurturing, caring, and affective aspects of maternal parenting and the instrumental, task-oriented, and disciplinary aspects of paternal parenting, some scholars suggest that maternal parenting is likely to be more pronounced until early childhood and paternal parenting tends to be more influential during middle childhood or later (Collins & Russell, Reference Collins and Russell1991; Gamble & Yu, Reference Gamble and Yu2014; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Nelson and Dix2018). This study attempts to fill in parts of the puzzle by examining the association of paternal parenting with children’s academic performance and internalizing/externalizing symptoms during middle childhood.

Parent and child gender

Developmental cascades may vary by gender in childhood. For example, Panayiotou and Humphrey (Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018) reported that the adjustment erosion hypothesis was upheld for boys, whereas the academic incompetence hypothesis was supported for girls. However, others found no such gender-specific cascading processes (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019). Just as there is limited evidence concerning if developmental cascade pathways vary significantly as a function of child gender, there is also a dearth of evidence concerning whether transactions between parenting and child adjustment over time vary by parent and child gender. The present study seeks to fill this void.

The present study

Effective interventions at preventing negative cascades and at promoting quality parenting necessitate accurate knowledge of cascading processes and their relations to parenting over time. Yet, no studies to date have investigated the longitudinal relations among the five constructs (i.e., academic performance, internalizing/externalizing problems, and maternal/paternal parenting) in a single analysis. In particular, middle childhood is a distinct developmental period marked by tremendous variations in internalizing/externalizing symptoms and academic performance (Gamble & Yu, Reference Gamble and Yu2008; Holopainen et al., Reference Holopainen, Hoang, Koch and Kofler2020; Kjeldsen et al., Reference Kjeldsen, Nes, Sanson, Ystrom and Karevold2021; Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui, Benjet, Georgiades and Swendsen2010) as well as more diversified and complicated parenting of mothers and fathers (Collins & Russell, Reference Collins and Russell1991; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Nelson and Dix2018; Yu & Ko, Reference Yu and Ko2013; Yu, Reference Yu2019), thus warranting further investigation into developmental cascades and their interactions with maternal and paternal parenting.

This study seeks to fill the gaps in the literature in several ways. First, the present investigation represents the first effort to bring together a developmental cascade model where (mal)adaptation rated by teachers in one developmental domain spreads over domains and time and its interplay with maternal and paternal parenting at home. Second, the present analysis controls for intelligence and family SES that have rarely been controlled for in a single cascade model. Third, given that prior work examining the above two types of relations in children has been predominantly conducted based on samples from Western countries, this study makes a contribution to the literature by using a nationally representative sample of South Korean children born between April and July, 2008. Finally, of the small number of studies that have tested gender differences in cascading or parenting processes, findings have proven inconclusive. Relatedly, there has been a lack of research on fathering. In an effort to address these issues, the current study explores whether maternal and paternal parenting behaviors are differentially associated with child adjustment (i.e., academic performance and internalizing/externalizing problems) and whether the cascading and maternal/paternal parenting processes vary by child gender. Taken together, the present study seeks to test cascading effects among academic and internalizing/externalizing symptoms and their reciprocity with maternal/paternal parenting across three time points from 8 to 10 years, while taking into account gender differences and controlling for potential covariates such as intelligence and family SES.

The following hypotheses are tested: (1) Externalizing problems will negatively predict academic performance, (2) Because of the inconsistent findings pertaining to the internalizing symptoms → academic performance link, no specific hypothesis will be proposed, (3) Academic performance will negatively predict both externalizing and internalizing problems, (4) No specific hypothesis will be formulated as to bidirectional relations between internalizing and externalizing problems given mixed findings, (5) Maternal and paternal parenting will be bidirectionally associated with children’s internalizing/externalizing problems and academic performance, and (6) It will be examined whether interchanges between developmental cascades and maternal/paternal parenting differ by child gender without a specific hypothesis because of limited evidence.

Method

Participants

Data for this investigation came from the Panel Study on Korean Children (PSKC), a nationally representative ongoing prospective cohort study of children born in April through July of 2008 in South Korea. The PSKC began collecting data based on multistage probability cluster sampling annually among 2,150 two-parent heterosexual families in 2008. Families were recruited through 30 hospitals with at least 500 deliveries in 2006 across six regions of the country to represent the population of South Korean two-parent families with newborns in the period from April to July 2008. The sample for the current analyses included 1,598 families (48.5% girls) who had at least one data point from 2016 to 2018 (hereafter T1, T2, and T3). The mean ages of mothers and fathers were respectively 38.8 (SD = 3.65) and 41.2 years (SD = 3.95) at baseline (child age 8 years). Almost all mothers (99.6%) and fathers (99.5%) had completed high school or equivalent. Average family income was $4,197 (SD = 1,948) per month at T1. At T1, 2, and 3, 929, 633, and 709 school teachers, respectively, participated in the survey. The vast majority were females (84.7%, 81.8%, and 73.6%) with mean ages of 41.8 (SD = 10.42), 40.0 (SD = 9.91), and 38.2 (SD = 9.18) years, respectively. Neither gender nor age of the teachers was significantly related to children’s academic performance or internalizing/externalizing problems. Of note, each school year brought new teachers who reported on one child only. Because of the anonymized public data (https://panel.kicce.re.kr/pskc/module/rawDataManage/index.do?menu_idx=56), ethical review was not required.

Percentages of missing data ranged from 0% (child gender) to 60.4% (academic performance and internalizing and externalizing problems at T2), with the average at 20.8%. Similarly, the missing rate for academic performance and internalizing and externalizing problems at T3 was 55.6%. Children with missing data on academic performance and internalizing and externalizing problems at T2 (F(1, 966) = 5.26, p < .05, Ms = 1.75 vs 1.69) or at T3 (F(1, 966) = 9.16, p < .01, Ms = 1.77 vs 1.68) had more positive fathering at T1 than those with non-missing data, with a negligible effect size (η p 2 = .01). Children with any missing data over time scored lower on paternal authoritative parenting at T3 (F(1, 966) = 13.45, p < .001, Ms = 3.66 vs 3.80) than those with complete data, with a negligible effect size (η p 2 = .01). No other significant differences were found in any other variables.

Measures

Academic performance (child age 8–10)

Teachers rated children’s academic performance with 7, 10, and 11 items (e.g., reading, writing, math) at T1, T2, and T3, respectively, on a five-point scale (1 = bottom 20% to 5 = top 20%). A sample item includes “Compared to the other students, how well does this student write?” Cronbach alphas were .96, .98, and .97 for girls and .97, .97, .98 for boys at T1, T2, and T3, respectively.

Internalizing behaviors (child age 8–10)

Teacher ratings on the emotional (five items; “often unhappy, down-hearted or tearful”) and peer (five items; “rather solitary, tends to play alone”) problems subscales of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, Reference Goodman1997) were used to index internalizing behavior problems (Di Riso et al., Reference Di Riso, Salcuni, Chessa, Raudino, Lis and Altoè2010; Dickey & Blumberg, Reference Dickey and Blumberg2004; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Lamping and Ploubidis2010). A validated Korean version of the SDQ was administered in this study (Ahn et al., Reference Ahn, Jun, Han, Noh and Goodman2003). Each SDQ item was rated from 0 (not true) to 2 (certainly true). Summed scores for each subscale can be classified as normal (0–4 for emotional and 0–3 for peer problems), borderline (5 for emotional and 4 for peer problems), and abnormal (6–10 for emotional and 5–10 for peer problems). In community samples, approximately 80% of children are classified as normal, 10% borderline, and 10% abnormal (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Cronbach’s alphas at T1, T2, and T3 were .71, .71, .64 for girls and .75, .72, .71 for boys.

Externalizing behaviors (child age 8–10)

Children’s externalizing problems were measured with teacher ratings of the Korean version SDQ’s conduct problems (five items; “often lies or cheats”) and hyperactivity subscales (five items; “constantly fidgeting or squirming”). All items were rated on a three-point scale (0 = not true, 2 = certainly true). Summed scores for each subscale can fall into three categories: normal (0–2 for conduct problems and 0–5 for hyperactivity), borderline (3 for conduct problems and 6 for hyperactivity), and abnormal (4–10 for conduct problems and 7–10 for hyperactivity). Cronbach alphas were .83, .79, and .74 for girls and .85, .85, and .86 for boys at T1, T2, and T3, respectively. The broader Internalizing/Externalizing scales are deemed appropriate for use among a community sample of children (Di Riso et al., Reference Di Riso, Salcuni, Chessa, Raudino, Lis and Altoè2010; Dickey & Blumberg, Reference Dickey and Blumberg2004; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Lamping and Ploubidis2010).

Parenting (child age 8–10)

At T1, parenting behaviors regarding cognitively stimulating activities at home were assessed with eight items adapted from the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment scale (Caldwell et al., Reference Caldwell and Bradley1984). It was translated and validated by PSKC researcher team members and has also been used in South Korean parents (e.g., Yim et al., Reference Yim, Kim, Song and Lee2022). A sample item includes “In a typical week how often do you read to your child?” Mothers and fathers rated all items on a four-point scale (1 = never to 4 = everyday). Cronbach alphas of maternal and paternal cognitive stimulation were .81 and .83 for girls and .79 and .83 for boys.

At T2 and 3, mothers and fathers completed the 27-item authoritative parental behavior from the Parenting Practices Questionnaire (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen and Hart1995), which was translated and validated by PSKC researcher team members. The Korean version of the questionnaire has been widely used (e.g., Yang & Kim, Reference Yang and Kim2021). A sample item is “I am responsive to our child’s feelings or needs.” All items were rated on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Cronbach alphas of maternal authoritative parenting at T2 and T3 were .91 and .91 for girls and .91 and .90 for boys and those of paternal authoritative parenting at T2 and T3 were .93 and .91 for girls and .92 and .92 for boys.

Intelligence (child age 8)

Multi-Factorial Intelligence Test (M-FIT; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Hyun and Choi2014) was used by trained examiners to test children’s intellectual learning abilities at T1. The test contains 20 multiple-choice questions from each subtest, totaling 120 questions: word fluency, verbal analogy, graphical representation, numerical ability, spatial perception, and reasoning ability. T scores on each of the six subtests were used. Mean T-scores ≥56 correspond to a percentile rank of 73 or higher, between 45 and 55 percentile ranks from 30 to 70, and ≤44 a percentile rank of 27 or lower.

A total of 374 children were administered the Korean version of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised (K-WPPSI-R; Park et al., Reference Park, Kwak and Park1996; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1989) at age five and the M-FIT at age eight. Verbal IQ of the K-WPPSI-R was significantly correlated with word fluency (r = .42), verbal analogy (r = .46), graphical representation (r = .37), and numerical ability (r = .38) of the M-FIT all at p < .001. Performance IQ of the K-WPPSI-R also significantly correlated with spatial perception (r = .36) and with reasoning ability (r = .34) of the M-FIT both at p < .001. Correlation coefficients of Full-Scale IQ scores on the K-WPPSI-R with the subtest scores on the M-FIT ranged from .34 to .46 (all significant at p < .001).

Family SES (child age 8)

Family SES was assessed from three components: family income, mother’s education, and father’s education.

Analytic strategy

A multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (MGCFA) was run to verify hypothesized relationships between observed variables and their underlying latent constructs (i.e., family SES and child intelligence) and to assess the measurement equivalence of the constructs across child gender. A multigroup structural equation model (MGSEM) was then conducted to examine temporal relationships between the study variables and their equivalence across gender. Missing data were addressed with multiple imputation (MI). A sensitivity analysis using a pattern-mixture approach was performed to determine robustness of results between children with and without complete data (Hedeker & Gibbons, Reference Hedeker and Gibbons1997; Little, Reference Little1993).

Model fit was evaluated by the chi-square value, the comparative fit indexes (CFI), and the root mean square error approximation (RMSEA). CFI and RMSEA values approximating .95 and .06 respectively indicate good fit (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). A nonsignificant χ 2 change and changes in CFI of less than .01 and RMSEA of less than .01 were used to test measurement equivalence and a nonsignificant χ 2 change was used to investigate structural parameter equivalence (Cheung & Rensvold, Reference Cheung and Rensvold2002; Rutkowski & Svetina, Reference Rutkowski and Svetina2014). AMOS 26 was used to test all models.

Results

Table 1 shows descriptive statistics and child-gender differences using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). While MANOVA revealed no significant gender differences in family SES, maternal/paternal parenting, or internalizing problems (i.e., emotional and peer problems), significant gender differences were found in intelligence, externalizing problems (i.e., conduct problems and hyperactivity), and academic performance. Follow-up univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAs) showed that girls had significantly higher scores on word fluency (F(1, 1491) = 48.51, p < .001, η p 2 = .03), verbal analogy (F(1, 1491) = 19.44, p < .001, η p 2 = .01), and graphical representation (F(1, 1491) = 20.86, p < .001, η p 2 = .01), but lower scores on numerical ability (F(1, 1491) = 11.59, p < .01, η p 2 = .01) than boys. Regarding externalizing problems, ANOVAs found that teachers rated boys significantly higher than girls on conduct problems (T1, F(1, 289) = 7.03, p < .01, η p 2 = .02; T2, F(1, 289) = 4.98, p < .05, η p 2 = .02; T3, F(1, 289) = 7.84, p < .01, η p 2 = .03) and hyperactivity (T1, F(1, 289) = 51.94, p < .001, η p 2 = .15; T2, F(1, 289) = 28.80, p < .001, η p 2 = .09; T3, F(1, 289) = 14.89, p < .001, η p 2 = .05) across the three time points. ANOVAs also revealed that teachers rated girls’ academic performance significantly higher than that of boys across the three time points (T1, F(1, 289) = 5.84, p < .05, η p 2 = .02; T2, F(1, 289) = 4.92, p < .05, η p 2 = .02; T3, F(1, 289) = 6.98, p < .01, η p 2 = .02).

Table 1. Means and standard deviations, and multivariate analysis of variance in the study variables

Note.

a The unit is 1,000,000 won.

b Parental educational attainment was coded 1 = middle school degree or below to 5 = post-graduate degree.

M. = Mother. F. = Father.

The vast majority of the children were classified as normal based on the SDQ across the three time points: emotional problems, 92.5%, 93.6%, and 96.7% for boys and 94.9%, 94.1%, and 95.6% for girls; peer problems, 81.3%, 85.6%, and 88.8% for boys and 86.8%, 86.6%, and 90.7% for girls; conduct problems, 76.3%, 78.2%, and 81.9% for boys and 90.1%, 89.6%, and 91.9% for girls; and hyperactivity, 77.8%, 83.1%, and 86.0% for boys and 94.7%, 96.1%, and 95.3% for girls.

Table 2 shows correlations among the study variables. While correlations were in generally expected directions, there were some gender differences and interactions between parents’ gender and children’s gender. For example, word fluency and academic performance at age eight were positively correlated with paternal authoritative parenting at age 10, while internalizing behavior was negatively correlated in boys; however, there were no such correlations in girls. While some inverse correlations were observed between intelligence and later internalizing/externalizing behavior in boys, no such clear correlations were observed in girls. Academic performance tended to be positively correlated with both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting, whereas externalizing behavior tended to be negatively correlated in boys; however, such correlations were less evident in girls.

Table 2. Bivariate correlations for the study variables

Note. Girls are displayed above the diagonal; boys are displayed below the diagonal.

a The unit is 1,000,000 won.

b Parental educational attainment was coded 1 = middle school degree or below to 5 = post-graduate degree.

M. = Mother. F. = Father.

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

An MGCFA was undertaken to assess measurement invariance of the latent constructs (i.e., family SES and M-FIT) across boys and girls and yielded a good fit to the data, χ 2(52) = 159.69, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .036, 90% CI = .030, .042. All standardized factor loadings were significant at p < .001 and above .36. Given that the same factor structure was found for each gender, configural invariance was supported. All the factor loadings were then constrained to be equivalent across groups to test for full metric invariance, χ 2(59) = 172.05, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .035, 90% CI = .029, .041. Because the fit indexes did not deteriorate, full metric invariance was retained, Δχ 2(7) = 12.36, p < .10; ΔCFI = .002; ΔRMSEA = .001. Equality constraints were subsequently placed on factor loadings and item intercepts across groups to test full scalar invariance, but the model fit was significantly worse than that of the full metric invariance model, χ 2(68) = 298.97, p < .001; CFI = .93; RMSEA = .046, 90% CI = .041, .052; Δχ 2(9) = 126.92, p < .001; ΔCFI = .036; ΔRMSEA = .011. Therefore, partial scalar invariance was achieved after freeing the intercepts of word fluency, verbal analogy, graphical representation, and numerical ability across groups, χ 2(64) = 174.99, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .033, 90% CI = .027, .039; Δχ 2(5) = 2.95, ns; ΔCFI = .001; ΔRMSEA = .002. Specifically, item intercepts of word fluency, verbal analogy, and graphical representation were higher for girls, while that of numerical ability was higher for boys. A latent mean of family SES in each group was also compared by constraining the mean of boys to be zero, χ 2(63) = 174.97, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .033, 90% CI = .028, .039. This model was not significantly different from the model with equal means of the latent factor, Δχ 2(1) = .02, ns; ΔCFI = .001; ΔRMSEA = .000, supporting latent mean invariance. Given that full scalar invariance for the M-FIT was not supported, no test for factor mean invariance was carried out (Steenkamp & Baumgartner, Reference Steenkamp and Baumgartner1998).

An MGSEM was next employed to test the structural path invariance across groups. An unrestricted model where all structural paths were allowed to vary, χ 2(342) = 813.96, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .029, 90% CI = .027, .032, was compared with a restricted model where all the paths were constrained to be equivalent across gender, χ 2(408) = 930.62, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .028, 90% CI = .026, .031. Because the difference between the two was significant, Δχ 2(66) = 116.66, p < .001, the constrained paths were released one by one until a nonsignificant difference was observed (Keith, Reference Keith2019; Little, Reference Little2013). Seven paths were found to vary by gender, χ 2(401) = 882.42, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .027, 90% CI = .025, .030 and this model was not significantly different from the fully unrestricted model, Δχ 2(59) = 68.46, ns (see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Cascades model along with maternal/paternal parenting. Unstandardized regression coefficients are shown. For simplicity, correlations between manifest variables or error terms and nonsignificant structural paths are not presented. †p < 10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Family SES was positively associated with children’s intelligence and mothers’/fathers’ cognitive stimulation. Child intelligence was negatively associated with internalizing and externalizing behavior and maternal cognitive stimulation, whereas positively associated with academic performance. Among girls, but not boys, intelligence was negatively associated with paternal cognitive stimulation.

Internalizing behavior at T1 negatively predicted externalizing behavior at T2. Externalizing behavior at T1 negatively predicted both maternal authoritative parenting at T2 and academic performance at T2. Academic performance at T1 positively predicted both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting at T2 and negatively predicted internalizing behavior at T2. Among boys, but not girls, academic performance at T1 positively predicted externalizing behavior at T2. Paternal cognitive stimulation at T1 positively predicted maternal authoritative parenting at T2.

Internalizing behavior at T2 positively predicted academic performance at T3, but negatively predicted paternal authoritative parenting at T3. Academic performance at T2 negatively predicted both internalizing and externalizing behavior at T3 and externalizing behavior at T2 negatively predicted academic performance at T3. Maternal authoritative parenting at T2 positively predicted academic performance at T3. Among girls, but not boys, maternal authoritative parenting at T2 positively predicted internalizing behavior at T3. Paternal authoritative parenting at T2 positively predicted both academic performance and maternal authoritative parenting at T3 and negatively predicted internalizing behavior at T3. Across-time stability effects were found for the same or similar variables (i.e., cognitive stimulation to authoritative parenting), with boys showing more stable patterns of externalizing behavior from T1 to T2 and academic performance from T1 to T3.

A sensitivity analysis was conducted to determine whether the results remained the same across complete and incomplete data groups. The results of the MGCFA indicated that full configural (χ 2(52) = 178.87, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .039, 90% CI = .033, .045), metric (χ 2(59) = 183.05, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .036, 90% CI = .030, .042; Δχ 2(7) = 4.17, ns; ΔCFI = .000; ΔRMSEA = .003), and scalar invariance (χ 2(68) = 195.81, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .034, 90% CI = .029, .040; Δχ 2(9) = 12.76, ns; ΔCFI = .001; ΔRMSEA = .002) was established across the groups. Support was also found for the latent mean invariance of family SES (χ 2(67) = 194.68, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .035, 90% CI = .029, .040; Δχ 2(1) = 1.13, ns; ΔCFI = .000; ΔRMSEA = .001) and M-FIT (χ 2(67) = 195.69, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA = .035, 90% CI = .029, .040; Δχ 2(1) = 1.13, ns; ΔCFI = .000; ΔRMSEA = .001) across the groups. The MGSEM results showed that the difference between an unrestricted (χ 2(342) = 814.96, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .029, 90% CI = .027, .032) and a restricted models (χ 2(412) = 880.81, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .027, 90% CI = .024, .029) was not significant, either (Δχ 2(70) = 65.84, ns). Thus, all the structural path coefficients were found to be equal across the groups. Overall, the sensitivity analysis suggested that the main analysis remained largely unaffected by missing data. The only difference was that externalizing behavior at T1 significantly negatively predicted paternal authoritative parenting at T2 in the sensitivity analysis, whereas this path was marginally significant in the original analysis.

Discussion

This study represents a first attempt at investigating the association of developmental cascades across internalizing/externalizing symptoms and academic performance rated by teachers with maternal/paternal parenting reported by parents from 8 to 10 years. The current work addressed this void by using the longitudinal data drawn from a nationally representative sample of children in South Korea and an MGSEM, while also including relevant covariates such as intelligence and family SES. The findings supported both adjustment erosion (particularly pathways from externalizing problems to academic performance) and academic incompetence (particularly pathways from academic performance to internalizing problems) hypotheses. Teacher ratings of children’s academic performance were prospectively related to higher maternal and paternal authoritative parenting, which in turn led to children’s higher academic performance. Children’s externalizing problems rated by teachers subsequently negatively predicted maternal authoritative parenting rated by mothers. In addition, children’s internalizing problems reported by teachers and paternal authoritative parenting rated by fathers were negatively and reciprocally related to each other, all highlighting the interdependent mesosystemic influences (Bronfenbrenner, Reference Bronfenbrenner, Gauvain and Cole1994). Thus, the present study sheds light not only on developmental cascades but also their linkages with maternal and paternal parenting during this developmental period in the single analysis. Of note, the association of cascading effects with parenting in the model were largely invariant across child gender.

Externalizing problems were found to be consistently inversely related to subsequent academic performance over time, supporting the first hypothesis. These findings thus suggest that elementary children with externalizing problems are at increased risk for exhibiting poor academic performance or those with less externalizing problems are likely to achieve greater academic performance (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; McCarty et al., Reference McCarty, Mason, Kosterman, Hawkins, Lengua and McCauley2008; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2010). Transactional effects were also observed between academic performance and externalizing symptoms during ages 9–10 years, meaning that children with better academic performance at age 9 were less likely to exhibit externalizing behaviors at age 10 and children with less externalizing behaviors at age 9 were more likely to perform better academically at age 10 (Defoe et al., Reference Defoe, Farrington and Loeber2013; McCarty et al., Reference McCarty, Mason, Kosterman, Hawkins, Lengua and McCauley2008; Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Schütte, Taskinen and Köller2013). Adding to limited evidence of the bidirectional relationship between academic performance and externalizing problems (e.g., Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019; Zimmermann et al., Reference Zimmermann, Schütte, Taskinen and Köller2013), the current findings suggest that children exhibiting externalizing behaviors are apt to distract themselves from school work through disruptive, non-compliant behaviors and low-achieving children may be prone to engage in problem behavior to escape from difficult academic activities. Conversely, it is likely that children who do not engage in hyperactive, disruptive, and aggressive behaviors may immerse themselves in task-focused learning activities and high-achieving children may behave in more socially acceptable ways not to undermine their relationships with others (i.e., classmates, teachers) (e.g., Yu & Gamble, Reference Yu and Gamble2010). Unexpectedly, contrary to the academic incompetence hypothesis and the extant literature, high academic performance at age 8 was found to lead to greater externalizing problems at age 9 among boys. Albeit difficult to interpret, given that the school culture in South Korea values academic performance highly (Yu, Reference Yu2010, Reference Yu2019), high academic achieving boys may feel privileged and inflated self-regard and thus may engage in externalizing behavior (Brummelman et al., Reference Brummelman, Thomaes, Nelemans, de Castro, Overbeek and Bushman2015; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Cavell and Grossman1997; Westen et al., Reference Westen, Shedler, Bradley and DeFife2012). This finding, however, should be interpreted with caution until further evidence comes to light.

Results on the second hypothesis showed that internalizing symptoms at 9 years positively contributed to academic performance at 10 years, running counter to evidence suggesting that internalizing symptoms may undermine academic performance (Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; McCarty et al., Reference McCarty, Mason, Kosterman, Hawkins, Lengua and McCauley2008; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2010; Riglin et al., Reference Riglin, Petrides, Frederickson and Rice2014). This unexpected finding, however, aligns with previous research by Evans et al. (Reference Evans, Gaysina and Field2020), who found that internalizing symptoms of the SDQ (the same as in the present study) at 11 years predicted mathematics attainment at 13–14 and 15–16 years. Similarly, a small body of literature reported positive longitudinal effects of anxiety on academic achievement outcomes in children, suggesting the possible role of anxiety as a spur to greater efforts (e.g., Voltas et al., Reference Voltas, Hernández-Martinez, Aparicio, Arija and Canals2014). Thus, the positive effect of internalizing symptoms → academic performance may make it necessary to investigate effects of depression and anxiety on academic performance separately (Riglin et al., Reference Riglin, Petrides, Frederickson and Rice2014).

Confirming the third hypothesis, academic performance exerted consistently negative cascading effects on internalizing symptoms (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Maughan et al., Reference Maughan, Rowe, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber2003; Obradović et al., Reference Obradović, Burt and Masten2010; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Brittain, McDougall and Duku2013; van Lier et al., Reference van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2012). These findings suggest that academic difficulties that children experience are likely to lead to negative affect and negative self-evaluations and which in turn may put them at greater risk for subsequent internalizing problems during the early school years, supporting the academic incompetence hypothesis (Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010). In opposition, the confidence and competence that children experience in learning situations may carry over into emotional well-being.

In testing the fourth hypothesis, greater internalizing behavior at age 8 predicted less externalizing behavior at age 9, replicating earlier findings where internalizing behavior likely inhibits children from developing externalizing problems (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Roisman, Long, Burt, Obradović, Riley, Boelcke-Stennes and Tellegen2005; Panayiotou & Humphrey, Reference Panayiotou and Humphrey2018; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016). However, internalizing behavior at age 9 was not predictive of externalizing behavior at age 10 probably because internalizing behavior might not have a strong deterrent effect on externalizing problems during middle childhood (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; van Lier et al., Reference van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2012). Likewise, the data did not support the predictive role of externalizing behavior on internalizing behavior, corroborating previous findings (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; van Lier et al., Reference van Lier, Vitaro, Barker, Brendgen, Tremblay and Boivin2012). Together these findings suggest that, at least in middle childhood, cascades between internalizing and externalizing problems barely take place.

Given that little research has been done on whether a child’s psychopathology or academic performance are prospectively associated with paternal parenting and vice versa, one of the unique contributions of this study was to leverage the prospective data collected on both mothers and fathers, which allowed for determination of the relative contributions of maternal and paternal parenting to their child’s outcomes and vice versa. The findings suggest that children’s academic performance may increase the use of both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting practices. That is, if a child exhibited better academic performance at age 8, both mothers and fathers were more apt to adopt authoritative parenting at age 9 (Xiong et al., Reference Xiong, Qin, Wang and Ren2021). Contrarily, when a child had poorer academic performance at age 8, mothers and fathers were less inclined to employ authoritative parenting at age 9. These results echo previous research suggesting the child-driven effects of academic achievement on parenting (Xiong et al., Reference Xiong, Qin, Wang and Ren2021). Further, these findings, along with the above evidence of academic performance → internalizing problem link, may reflect Confucian values of academic achievement and respect for education (Xiong et al., Reference Xiong, Qin, Wang and Ren2021; Yu, Reference Yu2019). Other child-driven effects were observed, such that when children exhibited greater externalizing behaviors at age 8, mothers tended to parent less authoritatively at age 9. In other words, if children showed fewer externalizing behaviors at age 8, mothers were likely to parent more authoritatively at age 9. Similarly, when children showed greater internalizing problems at age 9, fathers were less likely to utilize authoritative parenting at age 10. That is, if children displayed less internalizing behavior at age 9, fathers were more likely to utilize authoritative parenting at age 10. These findings all point to the active role that children play in eliciting authoritative parenting and further the importance of recognizing children as capable agents in family life (Gromoske & Maguire-Jack, Reference Gromoske and Maguire-Jack2012; Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017a, Reference Pinquart2017b; Yu, Reference Yu2010).

The fifth hypothesis concerned the bidirectional associations between maternal/paternal parenting and child academic performance and psychopathology. Surprisingly, when mothers parented more authoritatively at 9 years, daughters, but not sons, were likely to show more internalizing behavior at 10 years. Perhaps, South Korean mothers’ authoritative parenting toward their primary school-aged daughter may somehow be intertwined with less optimal parenting including over-engagement or intrusive control, which may increase the risk of internalizing problems among girls (Holochwost et al., Reference Holochwost, Volpe, Iruka and Mills-Koonce2020; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Eoh, Jeong and Park2017). More scholarly attention deserves to be given to co-occurring dimensions of maternal parenting and how the influence of authoritative parenting may vary by parent gender and child gender, and sociocultural context in this developmental stage. In contrast, when fathers adopted more authoritative parenting at age 9, children, regardless of gender, were less likely to exhibit internalizing behaviors at age 10. To put it differently, if fathers tended to parent less authoritatively at age 9, children (both genders) tended to show greater internalizing problems at age 10. These findings corroborate the results of the meta-analysis showing a stronger association of paternal than maternal parenting with offspring anxiety symptoms (Möller et al., Reference Möller, Nikolić, Majdandžić and Bögels2016). These findings further appear to be in accord with the suggestion that paternal parenting characterized by rationality, practicality, and self-direction becomes more influential as children reach middle childhood (Collins & Russell, Reference Collins and Russell1991; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Nelson and Dix2018; Yu & Ko, Reference Yu and Ko2013). When mothers and fathers engaged in more authoritative parenting at age 9, children were likely to exhibit better academic performance at age 10. The findings not only concur with well-established studies demonstrating positive effects of maternal authoritative parenting on children’s academic achievement (e.g., Masten & Curtis), but also complement the limited literature documenting that fathers play an integral role in their child’s academic engagement above and beyond mothers (Zhu et al., in press). When fathers spent more time engaging in cognitively stimulating activities with their child at age 8, mothers were more likely to do so at age 9, but not vice versa. Similarly, if fathers parented with a more authoritative style at age 9, mothers tended to do so at age 10, but not vice versa. Considered together, the findings highlight the importance of collecting data from both parents and the understudied role that fathers may play in shaping their children’s development and mother-child interactions (Collins & Russell, Reference Collins and Russell1991; Gamble & Yu, Reference Gamble and Yu2014; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Nelson and Dix2018; Yu & Ko, Reference Yu and Ko2013).

The last hypothesis was framed to test the moderating role of child gender in the pathways involving developmental cascades and maternal/paternal parenting. To recap, few child gender differences emerged in the parenting pathways. Adding to a small body of literature suggesting no child gender differences in cascading effects (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Van der Ende et al., Reference Van der Ende, Verhulst and Tiemeier2016; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019), the current data suggested that developmental cascades were not generally gender-specific. In addition, even after controlling for shared risk factors that could underlie cascading effects, the longitudinal cross-domain effects were still observed in accordance with the view that common risk factors play little role in cascading processes (Burt & Roisman, Reference Burt and Roisman2010; Deighton et al., Reference Deighton, Humphrey, Belsky, Boehnke, Vostanis and Patalay2018; Maughan et al., Reference Maughan, Rowe, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber2003; Moilanen et al., Reference Moilanen, Shaw and Maxwell2010; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Brittain, McDougall and Duku2013; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Zhang, Chen, Ji and Deater-Deckard2019). In other words, these findings suggest that cascading effects among academic underperformance and internalizing/externalizing problems were not attributable to child gender, intelligence, or socioeconomic differences.

The negative effect of externalizing behavior at T1 → paternal authoritative parenting at T2 was marginal in the original analysis, whereas it was significant in the sensitivity analysis. No further differences were found in comparisons of the two models. Thus, the results from the sensitivity analysis suggested that the original model was seldom affected by missing data.

Given the prospective effect of externalizing problems on academic performance, school-wide, classroom-based, and/or family-centered interventions aiming to specifically identify and attenuate externalizing problems in early childhood may be effective in improving later academic performance. The current findings support the promotion of children’s academic success that may help prevent or ameliorate externalizing problems and may help elicit more authoritative parenting from their mothers and fathers. The findings also fuel the need for fathering intervention programs that may contribute to more positive maternal parenting and child functioning. Such interventions focus on promoting and strengthening coparenting, father-child relationships, and positive behavioral management strategies.

The findings need to be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, academic performance and internalizing/externalizing problems were measured solely by teacher ratings. Given that children worked with new teachers every year who each rated one child, the data did not come from a single source or have a hierarchical structure (i.e., children were not nested in classrooms or schools). In addition, academic performance and internalizing/externalizing problems showed strong cross-time stability and all longitudinal correlations among them were significant in the expected directions, thus providing some degree of consistency over time and across different raters. Nonetheless, there is some evidence that children may act differently at school than at home and teachers tend to report fewer problems with their students (e.g., Berg-Nielsen et al., Reference Berg-Nielsen, Solheim, Belsky and Wichstrom2012). Thus, future research would benefit from employing multiple sources (e.g., parent) and various assessment methods (e.g., standardized achievement test, observation). Second, because the data did not come from classroom- or school-based surveys, there were large proportions of missing values for academic performance and internalizing/externalizing problems which were all rated by teachers at T2 and T3. Although this limitation was alleviated by the use of MI for missing data along with the sensitivity analysis, caution should be exercised in interpreting the results. Third, although the measures of cognitive stimulation at T1 and authoritative parenting at T2 and T3 are conceptually overlapping and significantly correlated with each other, T1 levels of authoritative parenting were not controlled for. Thus, the paths from child adjustment at T1 to authoritative parenting at T2 may not be causal but simply correlational. Fourth, given that the data covered only a period of 8–10 years of one birth cohort, longer-term longitudinal research with multiple cohorts may paint a more complete picture of developmental cascades and their interplay with maternal and paternal parenting. Finally, because this study’s sample included only children raised in different-sex two-parent families, further work needs to determine whether the current findings can be generalized to children from diverse families (e.g., same-sex, single parents).

Conclusion

This study enhances the body of literature by simultaneously modeling academic performance, internalizing/externalizing problems and maternal/paternal parenting spanning from ages 8 to 10. The study’s findings clearly support the adjustment erosion model where externalizing problems lead to academic underperformance during middle childhood. In a parallel fashion, the findings suggest the importance of academic performance as an antecedent of subsequent internalizing/externalizing problems and maternal/paternal authoritative parenting in middle childhood, thus lending support to the academic incompetence model. These findings might be attributable to widespread aspiration for education in the Confucian culture, warranting replication and extension in future research. The findings also provided promising evidence that paternal parenting might contribute not only to children’s academic development and internalizing problems but to maternal parenting, underscoring the need for greater attention to the role that fathering may play in children’s development and mothering.

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding statement

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Ahn, J.-S., Jun, S.-K., Han, J.-K., Noh, K.-S., & Goodman, R. (2003). The development of a Korean version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. Journal of Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 42, 141147.Google Scholar
Berg-Nielsen, T. S., Solheim, E., Belsky, J., & Wichstrom, L. (2012). Preschoolers’ psychosocial problems: In the eyes of the beholder? Adding teacher characteristics as determinants of discrepant parent-teacher reports. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43(3), 393413. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-011-0271-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). Ecological models of human development. In Gauvain, M., & Cole, M. (Eds.), Readings on the development of children (2nd ed., pp. 3743). Freeman.Google Scholar
Brummelman, E., Thomaes, S., Nelemans, S. A., de Castro, B. O., Overbeek, G., & Bushman, B. J. (2015). Origins of narcissism in children. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(12), 36593662. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.142087011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, K. B., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). Competence and psychopathology: Cascade effects in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 557567. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000271 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, B. M., Bradley, R. H., & University of Arkansas at Little Rock, Center for Child Development and Education (1984). HOME observation for measurement of the environment. University of Arkansas at Little Rock.Google Scholar
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, W. A., & Russell, G. (1991). Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle childhood and adolescence: A developmental analysis. Developmental Review, 11(2), 99136. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(91)90004-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Defoe, I. N., Farrington, D. P., & Loeber, R. (2013). Disentangling the relationship between delinquency and hyperactivity, low achievement, depression, and low socioeconomic status: Analysis of repeated longitudinal data. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(2), 100107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2012.12.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deighton, J., Humphrey, N., Belsky, J., Boehnke, J., Vostanis, P., & Patalay, P. (2018). Longitudinal pathways between mental health difficulties and academic performance during middle childhood and early adolescence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 110126. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Riso, D., Salcuni, S., Chessa, D., Raudino, A., Lis, A., & Altoè, G. (2010). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Early evidence of its reliability and validity in a community sample of Italian children. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 570575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickey, W. C., & Blumberg, S. J. (2004). Revisiting the factor structure of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: United States, 2001. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(9), 11591167. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000132808.36708.a9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, D., Gaysina, D., & Field, A. P. (2020). Internalizing symptoms and working memory as predictors of mathematical attainment trajectories across the primary-secondary education transition. Royal Society Open Science, 7(5), 191433. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191433 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamble, W. C., & Yu, J. J. (2008). Adolescent siblings’ looking glass self-orientations: Patterns of liabilities and associations with parenting. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37(7), 860874. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9276-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamble, W. C., & Yu, J. J. (2014). Young children’s sibling relationship interactional types: Associations with family characteristics, parenting, and child characteristics. Early Education and Development, 25(2), 223239. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2013.788434 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2010). When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(8), 11791191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gromoske, A. N., & Maguire-Jack, K. (2012). Transactional and cascading relations between early spanking and children’s social emotional development. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74(5), 10541068. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01013.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hedeker, D., & Gibbons, R. D. (1997). Application of random-effects pattern-mixture models for missing data in longitudinal studies. Psychological Methods, 2(1), 6478. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.2.1.64 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holochwost, S. J., Volpe, V. V., Iruka, I. U., & Mills-Koonce, W. R. (2020). Maternal warmth, intrusiveness, and executive functions in early childhood: Tracing developmental processes among African American children. Early Child Development and Care, 190(2), 210218. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2018.1461096 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holopainen, L., Hoang, N., Koch, A., & Kofler, D. (2020). Latent profile analysis of students’ reading development and the relation of cognitive variables to reading profiles. Annals of Dyslexia, 70(1), 94114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-020-00196-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, J. N., Cavell, T. A., & Grossman, P. A. (1997). A positive view of self: Risk or protection for aggressive children? Development and Psychopathology, 9(1), 7594. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keith, T. Z. (2019). Multiple regression and beyond: An introduction to multiple regression and structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kjeldsen, A., Nes, R. B., Sanson, A., Ystrom, E., & Karevold, E. B. (2021). Understanding trajectories of externalizing problems: Stability and emergence of risk factors from infancy to middle adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 33(1), 264283. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001755 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J., Hyun, S., & Choi, I. (2014). Multi-factorial intelligence test. Hakjisa.Google Scholar
Lee, J. H., Eoh, Y., Jeong, A., & Park, S. H. (2017). Preschoolers’ emotional understanding and psychosocial adjustment in Korea: The moderating effect of maternal attitude towards emotional expressiveness. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(7), 18541864. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0703-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, R. J. A. (1993). Pattern-mixture models for multivariate incomplete data. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 88(421), 125134. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1993.10594302 Google Scholar
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Curtis, W. J. (2000). Integrating competence and psychopathology: Pathways toward a comprehensive science of adaptation in development. Development and Psychopathology, 12(3), 529550. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457940000314X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., Roisman, G. I., Long, J. D., Burt, K. B., Obradović, J., Riley, J. R., Boelcke-Stennes, K., & Tellegen, A. (2005). Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology, 41(5), 733746. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2003). Reading problems and depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(2), 219229. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022534527021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarty, C. A., Mason, W. A., Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Lengua, L. J., & McCauley, E. (2008). Adolescent school failure predicts later depression among girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43(2), 180187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGroder, S. M. (2000). Parenting among low-income, African American single mothers with preschool-age children: Patterns, predictors, and developmental correlates. Child Development, 71(3), 752771. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00183 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., Benjet, C., Georgiades, K., & Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980989. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moilanen, K. L., Shaw, D. S., & Maxwell, K. L. (2010). Developmental cascades: Externalizing, internalizing, and academic competence from middle childhood to early adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 635653. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000337 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Möller, E. L., Nikolić, M., Majdandžić, M., & Bögels, S. M. (2016). Associations between maternal and paternal parenting behaviors, anxiety and its precursors in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 45, 1733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.03.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obradović, J., Burt, K. B., & Masten, A. S. (2010). Testing a dual cascade model linking competence and symptoms over 20 years from childhood to adulthood. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39(1), 90102. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410903401120 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panayiotou, M., & Humphrey, N. (2018). Mental health difficulties and academic attainment: Evidence for gender-specific developmental cascades in middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 523538. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700102X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, H. W., Kwak, K. J., & Park, K. B. (1996). The development of Korean version of WPPSI: The standardization study. The Korean Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 6070.Google Scholar
Pinquart, M. (2017a). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 873932. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000295 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinquart, M. (2017b). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Marriage and Family Review, 53(7), 613640. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2016.1247761 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinquart, M., & Kauser, R. (2018). Do the associations of parenting styles with behavior problems and academic achievement vary by culture? Results from a meta-analysis. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 24(1), 75100. https://doi.org/10.1037/cdp0000149 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riglin, L., Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N., & Rice, F. (2014). The relationship between emotional problems and subsequent school attainment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Adolescence, 37(4), 335346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.02.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., & Hart, C. H. (1995). Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Development of a new measure. Psychological Reports, 77(3, Pt 1), 819830. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutkowski, L., & Svetina, D. (2014). Assessing the hypothesis of measurement invariance in the context of large-scale international surveys. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 74(1), 3157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164413498257 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, J. K., Nelson, J. A., & Dix, T. (2018). Interdependence among mothers, fathers, and children from early to middle childhood: Parents’ sensitivity and children’s externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 15281541. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000525 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M., & Baumgartner, H. (1998). Assessing measurement invariance in cross-national consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 25(1), 7890. https://doi.org/10.1086/209528 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Dornbusch, S. M., & Darling, N. (1992). Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement: Authoritative parenting, school involvement, and encouragement to succeed. Child Development, 63(5), 12661281. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131532 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaillancourt, T., Brittain, H. L., McDougall, P., & Duku, E. (2013). Longitudinal links between childhood peer victimization, internalizing and externalizing problems, and academic functioning: Developmental cascades. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(8), 12031215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9781-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2016). The bidirectional pathways between internalizing and externalizing problems and academic performance from 6 to 18 years. Development and Psychopathology, 28(3), 855867. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000353 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Lier, P. A. C., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Brendgen, M., Tremblay, R. E., & Boivin, M. (2012). Peer victimization, poor academic achievement, and the link between childhood externalizing and internalizing problems. Child Development, 83(5), 17751788. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01802.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voltas, N., Hernández-Martinez, C., Aparicio, E., Arija, V., & Canals, J. (2014). Psychopathological factors that can influence academic achievement in early adolescence: A three-year prospective study. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 17, e100. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2014.100 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1989). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence-revised. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Westen, D., Shedler, J., Bradley, B., & DeFife, J. A. (2012). An empirically derived taxonomy for personality diagnosis: Bridging science and practice in conceptualizing personality. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(3), 273284. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020274 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiong, Y., Qin, X., Wang, Q., & Ren, P. (2021). Parental involvement in adolescents’ learning and academic achievement: Cross-lagged effect and mediation of academic engagement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50(9), 18111823. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-021-01460-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H. M., & Kim, H. R. (2021). Work-family conflict on children’s internet addiction: Role of parenting styles in Korean working mother. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(11), 5774. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18115774 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yim, D., Kim, S., Song, H.-S., & Lee, H. (2022). SES vs parent-child interaction: What predicts language skills and executive function? The Korean Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35, 2754. http://journal.baldal.or.kr/xml/32469/32469.pdf CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, J. J. (2010). Reciprocal influences between parental knowledge and adolescent self-control: A cross-sequential nationwide study of Korean youth. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(4), 458467. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020425 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, J. J. (2019). Longitudinal typologies of parent-child conflict and their correlates in adolescence. Children and Youth Services Review, 98, 132142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.12.026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, J. J., & Gamble, W. C. (2010). Direct and moderating effects of social affordances on school involvement and delinquency among young adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(4), 811824. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00669.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, J. J., & Ko, Y. K. (2013). Paternal family expressiveness as a mediator between father’s dispositional optimism and child’s dispositional optimism. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174(6), 677695. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2013.783548 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, W., Zhang, L., Chen, L., Ji, L., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2019). Developmental changes in longitudinal associations between academic achievement and psychopathological symptoms from late childhood to middle adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(2), 178188. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12927 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, Q., Cheong, Y., Wang, C., & Tong, J. (2021). The impact of maternal and paternal parenting styles and parental involvement on Chinese adolescents’ academic engagement and burnout. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01611-z Google Scholar
Zimmermann, F., Schütte, K., Taskinen, P., & Köller, O. (2013). Reciprocal effects between adolescent externalizing problems and measures of achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(3), 747761. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032793 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means and standard deviations, and multivariate analysis of variance in the study variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Bivariate correlations for the study variables

Figure 2

Figure 1. Cascades model along with maternal/paternal parenting. Unstandardized regression coefficients are shown. For simplicity, correlations between manifest variables or error terms and nonsignificant structural paths are not presented. †p < 10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.