Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T23:07:30.493Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Preschool executive functions, single-parent status, and school quality predict diverging trajectories of classroom inattention in elementary school

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2014

Tyler R. Sasser
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Charles R. Beekman III
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
Karen L. Bierman*
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Karen L. Bierman, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, 110 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

A sample of 356 children recruited from Head Start (58% European American, 25% African American, and 17% Hispanic; 54% girls; Mage = 4.59 years) were followed longitudinally from prekindergarten through fifth grade. Latent profile analyses of teacher-rated inattention from kindergarten through third grade identified four developmental trajectories: stable low (53% of the sample), stable high (11.3%), rising over time (16.4%), and declining over time (19.3%). Children with stable low inattention had the best academic outcomes in fifth grade, and children exhibiting stable high inattention had the worst, with the others in between. Self-regulation difficulties in preschool (poor executive function skills and elevated opposition–aggression) differentiated children with rising versus stable low inattention. Elementary schools characterized by higher achievement differentiated children with declining versus stable high inattention. Boys and children from single-parent families were more likely to remain high or rise in inattention, whereas girls and children from dual-parent families were more likely to remain low or decline in inattention.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Bauer, D. J. (2007). Observations on the use of growth mixture models in psychological research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 757786. doi:10.1080/00273170701710338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, L., Bohlin, G., & Rydell, A. M. (2003). Relations between inhibition, executive functioning, and ADHD symptoms: A longitudinal study from age 5 to 8½ years. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 255266. doi:10.1076/chin.9.4.255.23519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bierman, K., Domitrovich, C., Nix, R., Gest, S., Welsh, J., Greenberg, M., et al. (2008). Promoting academic and social–emotional school readiness: The Head Start REDI Program. Child Development, 79, 18021817. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01227.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bierman, K. L., & Torres, M. (in press). Promoting the development of executive functions through early education and prevention programs. In Griffin, J. A., Freund, L. S., & McCardle, P. (Eds.), Executive function in preschool age children: Integrating measurement, neurodevelopment and translational research. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Child development in the context of adversity: Experiential canalization of brain and behavior. American Psychologist, 67, 309318. doi:10.1037/a0027493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, J., Miller, E., Breslau, N., Bohnert, K., Lucia, V., & Schweitzer, J. (2009). The impact of early behavior disturbances on academic achievement in high school. Pediatrics, 123, 14721476. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-1406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., Breslau, J., Peterson, E., Miller, E., Lucia, V. C., Bohnert, K., et al. (2010). Change in teachers' ratings of attention problems and subsequent change in academic achievement: A prospective analysis. Psychological Medicine, 40, 159166. doi:10.1017/S0033291709005960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brock, L. L., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Nathanson, L., & Grim, K. J. (2009). The contributions of “hot” and “cool” executive function to children's academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24, 337349. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brocki, K. C., Eninger, L., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2010). Interrelations between executive function and symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention in preschoolers: A two year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 163171. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9354-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G. W., & Moran, P. M. (1997). Single mothers, poverty and depression. Psychological Medicine, 27, 2133. doi:10.1017/S0033291796004060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cairney, J., Boyle, M., Offord, D. R., & Racine, Y. (2003). Stress, social support and depression in single and married mothers. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 38, 442449. doi:10.1007/s00127-003-0661-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 595616. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Celeux, G., & Soromenho, G. (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13, 195212. doi:10.1007/BF01246098CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chronis, A. M., Chacko, A., Fabiano, G. A., Wymbs, B. T., & Pelham, W. E. (2004). Enhancements to the behavioral parent training paradigm for families of children with ADHD: Review and future directions. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 7, 127. doi:10.1023/B:CCFP.0000020190.60808.a4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crick, N. R., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). The development of psychopathology in females and males: Current progress and future challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 719742. doi:10.1017/S095457940300035XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croll, P. (2002). Social deprivation, school-level achievement and special education needs. Educational Research, 44, 4353. doi:10.1080/00131880110107342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H., & Pratt, C. (1996). The development of children's theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 2531. doi:10.1080/00049539508258765CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to do as I say, not as I do. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 315334. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199605)29:4<315::AID-DEV2>3.0.CO;2-T3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiPerna, J. C., & Elliott, S. N. (2000). Academic Competence Evaluation Scales. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., et al. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuPaul, G. J. (1991). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community-based sample. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 20, 245253. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2003_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebejer, J. L., Coventry, W. L., Byrne, B., Willcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., Corley, R., et al. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on inattention, hyperactivity–impulsivity, and reading: Kindergarten to grade 2. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 293316. doi:10.1080/10888430903150642CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foorman, B. R., York, M., Santi, K. L., & Francis, D. (2008). Contextual effects on predicting risk for reading difficulties in first and second grade. Reading and Writing, 21, 371394. doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9079-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483527. doi:10.1016/0885-2014(95)90024-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galéra, C., Bouvard, M. P., Lagarde, E., Michel, G., Touchette, E., Fombonne, E., et al. (2012). Childhood attention problems and socioeconomic status in adulthood: 18-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 201, 2025. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.111.102491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. L. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: A meta-analysis and critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 10361045. doi:10.1097/00004583-199708000-00011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershon, J. (2002). A meta-analytic review of gender differences in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 5, 143154. doi:10.1177/108705470200500302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gjone, H., Stevenson, J., & Sundet, J. M. (1996). Genetic influence on parent-reported attention-related problems in a Norwegian general population twin sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 588598. doi:10.1097/00004583-199605000-00013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Academic underachievement, attention deficits, and aggression: Comorbidity and implications for intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 893. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.60.6.893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. A. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43, 14471459. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. M., Brown, J. L., & Lawrence, A. J. (2011). Two-year impacts of a universal school-based social–emotional and literacy intervention: An experiment in translational developmental research. Child Development, 82, 533554. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01560.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Lee, S. S., & Willcutt, E. (2005). Instability of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD from preschool through elementary school. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 896. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.896CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonigan, C. J., Bloomfield, B. G., Anthony, J. L., Bacon, K. D., Phillips, B. M., & Samwel, C. S. (1999). Relations among emergent literacy skills, behavior problems, and social competence in preschool children from low- and middle-income backgrounds. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 19, 4053. doi:10.1177/027112149901900104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2007). TOPEL: Test of Preschool Early Literacy. Austin, TX: PRO-ED, Inc.Google Scholar
Macmillan, R., McMorris, B. J., & Kruttschnitt, C. (2004). Linked lives: Stability and change in maternal circumstances and trajectories of antisocial behavior in children. Child Development, 75, 205220. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00664.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mashburn, A. J., Justice, L. M., Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Peer effects on children's language achievement during pre-kindergarten. Child Development, 80, 686702. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01291.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miech, R. A., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Wright, B. E., & Silva, P. (1999). Low socio-economic status and mental illnesses: A longitudinal study of selection and causation during young adulthood. American Journal of Sociology, 104, 112147. doi:10.1086/210137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2001). Mplus user's guide. Los Angeles: Author.Google Scholar
Neidell, M., & Waldfogel, J. (2010). Cognitive and non-cognitive peer effects in early education. Review of Economics and Statistics, 92, 562576. doi:10.1162/REST_a_00012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 184214. doi:10.1080/15374410701818681CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J. B., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Carbonneau, R., Genolini, C., Falissard, B., et al. (2011). Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: A 16-year longitudinal population-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 11641170. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabiner, D. L., Carrig, M., & Dodge, K. A. (2013). Attention problems and academic achievement: Do persistent and earlier-emerging problems have more adverse long-term effects? Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Rabiner, D. L., Coie, J. D., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (2000). Early attention problems and children's reading achievement: A longitudinal investigation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 859867. doi:10.1097/00004583-200007000-00014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabiner, D. L., Murray, D. W., Rosen, L., Hardy, K., Skinner, A., & Underwood, M. (2010). Instability in teacher ratings of children's inattentive symptoms: Implications for the assessment of ADHD. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 175180. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181d5a2d8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raver, C. C., McCoy, D. C., Lowenstein, A. E., & Pess, R. (2013). Predicting individual differences in low-income children's executive control from early to middle childhood. Developmental Science, 16, 114. doi:10.1111/desc.12027Google Scholar
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Pianta, R. C., & Cox, M. J. (2000). Teachers' judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15, 147166. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(00)00049-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbers, S. C. C., van Oort, F. V. A., Polderman, T. J. C., Bartels, M., Boomsma, D. I., Verhulst, F. C., et al. (2011). Trajectories of CBCL attention problems in childhood. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 20, 419427. doi:10.1007/s00787-011-0194-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schaefer, B. A., & McDermott, P. A. (1999). Learning behavior and intelligence as explanations for children's scholastic achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 37, 299313. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(99)00007-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stipek, D., & Miles, S. (2008). Effects of aggression on achievement: Does conflict with the teacher make it worse? Child Development, 79, 17211735. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01221.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stipek, D., Newton, S., & Chudgar, A. (2010). Learning-related behaviors and literacy achievement in elementary school-aged children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 385395. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todd, R. D., Huang, H., Todorov, A. A., Neuman, R. J., Reiersen, A. M., Henderson, C. A., et al. (2008). Predictors of stability of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes from childhood to young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 7685. doi:10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a6acaCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). TOWRE: Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Wåhlstedt, C., Thorell, L. B., & Bohlin, G. (2008). ADHD symptoms and executive function impairment: Early predictors of later behavioral problems. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 160178. doi:10.1080/87565640701884253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werthamer-Larsson, L., Kellam, S. G., & Wheeler, L. (1991). Effect of first-grade classroom environment on shy behavior, aggressive behavior, and concentration problems. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 585602. doi:10.1007/BF00937993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44, 575. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9, 490499. doi:10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., et al. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 991. doi;10.1037/a0027347CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, C. C. (2006). Evaluating latent class analysis models in qualitative phenotype identification. Computational Statistics and Data Analysis, 50, 10901104. doi:10.1016/j.csda.2004.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., & Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood. In Nelson, C. & Luciana, M. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 553574). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zhou, Q., Hofer, C., Eisenberg, N., Reiser, M., Spinrad, T. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2007). The developmental trajectories of attention focusing, attentional and behavioral persistence, and externalizing problems during school-age years. Developmental Psychology, 43, 369385. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.369CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed