Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T05:03:36.656Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

(Positive) power to the child: The role of children's willing stance toward parents in developmental cascades from toddler age to early preadolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 October 2015

Grazyna Kochanska*
Affiliation:
University of Iowa
Sanghag Kim
Affiliation:
Hanyang University
Lea J. Boldt
Affiliation:
University of Iowa
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Grazyna Kochanska, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242-1407; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

In a change from the once-dominant view of children as passive in the parent-led process of socialization, children are now seen as active agents who can considerably influence that process. However, these newer perspectives typically focus on the child's antagonistic influence, due either to a difficult temperament or aversive, resistant, negative behaviors that elicit adversarial responses from the parent and lead to future coercive cascades in the relationship. Children's capacity to act as receptive, willing, even enthusiastic, active socialization agents is largely overlooked. Informed by attachment theory and other relational perspectives, we depict children as able to adopt an active willing stance and to exert robust positive influence in the mutually cooperative socialization enterprise. A longitudinal study of 100 community families (mothers, fathers, and children) demonstrates that willing stance (a) is a latent construct, observable in diverse parent–child contexts, parallel at 38, 52, and 67 months and longitudinally stable; (b) originates within an early secure parent–child relationship at 25 months; and (c) promotes a positive future cascade toward adaptive outcomes at age 10. The outcomes include the parent's observed and child-reported positive, responsive behavior, as well as child-reported internal obligation to obey the parent and parent-reported low level of child behavior problems. The construct of willing stance has implications for basic research in typical socialization and in developmental psychopathology as well as for prevention and intervention.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, J. P., Chango, J., Szwedo, D. E., Schad, M., & Marston, E. (2012). Predictors of susceptibility to peer influence regarding substance abuse in adolescence. Child Development, 83, 337350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allen, J. P., McElhaney, K. B., Land, D. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Moore, C. M., O'Beirne-Kelley, H., et al. (2003). A secure base in adolescence: Markers of attachment security in the mother-adolescent relationship. Child Development, 74, 292307.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (1981). Hyperactive children: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, P. (2007). Structural equation modelling: Adjudging model fit. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 815824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, J. E., Schermerhorn, A. C., & Petersen, I. T. (2012). Temperament and parenting in developmental perspective. In Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 425441). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 8195.Google Scholar
Bell, R. Q., & Chapman, M. (1986). Child effects in studies using experimental or brief longitudinal approaches to socialization. Developmental Psychology, 22, 595603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (2007). On tests and indices for evaluating structural models. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 825829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boldt, L. J., Kochanska, G., Yoon, J. E., & Nordling, J. K. (2014). Children's attachment to both parents from toddler age to middle childhood: Links to adaptive and maladaptive outcomes. Attachment and Human Development, 16, 211229.Google Scholar
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2013). From parent to child to parent: Paths in and out of problem behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 515529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M. M., & Stifter, C. A. (1997). Compliance and noncompliance: The roles of maternal control and child temperament. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 41l428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bretherton, I., Golby, B., & Cho, E. (1997). Attachment and the transmission of values. In Grusec, J. E. & Kuczynski, L. (Eds.), Parenting and children's internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 103134). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brock, R. L., Kochanska, G., O'Hara, M. W., & Grekin, R. (2015). Life satisfaction moderates the effectiveness of a play-based parenting intervention in low-income mothers and toddlers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0014-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brody, G. H., Beach, S. R. H., Philibert, R. A., Chen, Y., Lei, M., Murry, V. M., et al. (2009). Parenting moderates a genetic vulnerability factor in longitudinal increases in youth's substance use. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 77, 111.Google Scholar
Cabrera, N. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, S., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. Child Development, 71, 127136.Google Scholar
Clark, M. S. (1984). Record keeping in two types of relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 549557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, W. A., & Laursen, B. (Eds.). (1999). Relationships as developmental contexts: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 30). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Criss, M. M., Shaw, D. S., & Ingoldsby, E. M. (2003). Mother-son positive synchrony in middle childhood: Relation to antisocial behavior. Social Development, 12, 379400.Google Scholar
Dadds, M. R., & Salmon, K. (2003). Punishment insensitivity and parenting: Temperament and learning as interacting risks for antisocial behavior. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 6, 6986.Google Scholar
Darling, N., Cumsille, P., & Martínez, M. L. (2008). Individual differences in adolescents’ beliefs about the legitimacy of parental authority and obligation to obey: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 79, 11031118.Google Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K., & Petrill, S. A. (2004). Parent-child dyadic mutuality and child behavior problems: An investigation of gene-environment processes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 11711179.Google Scholar
Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (2006). The development and ecology of antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 503541). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Emde, R. N., Biringen, Z., Clyman, R. B., & Oppenheim, D. (1991). The moral self of infancy: Affective core and procedural knowledge. Developmental Review, 11, 251270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eyberg, S. M., & Bussing, R. (2010). Parent-child interaction therapy for preschool children with conduct problems. In Murrihy, R. C., Kidman, A. D., & Ollendick, T. H. (Eds.), Clinical handbook of assessing and treating conduct problems in youth (pp. 139162). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Forehand, R., & McMahon, R. J., (1981). Helping the noncompliant child: A clinician's guide to parent training. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Forman, D. R., Aksan, N., & Kochanska, G. (2004). Toddlers’ responsive imitation predicts preschool-age conscience. Psychological Science, 15, 699704.Google Scholar
Forman, D. R., & Kochanska, G. (2001). Viewing imitation as child responsiveness: A link between teaching and discipline domains of socialization. Developmental Psychology, 37, 198206.Google Scholar
Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. N. (2002). Child Symptom Inventory 4: Screening and norms manual. New York: Checkmate Plus.Google Scholar
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., & Zimmermann, P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child-father attachment relationship: Fathers’ sensitive and challenging play as the pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Social Development, 11, 307331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Kindler, H., & Zimmerman, P. (2008). A wider view of attachment and exploration: The influence of mothers and fathers on the development of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 857879). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grusec, J. E., & Davidov, M. (2007). Socialization in the family: The roles of parents. In Grusec, J. & Hastings, P. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (pp. 284308). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hare, A. L., Marston, E. G., & Allen, J. P. (2011). Maternal acceptance and adolescents’ emotional communication: A longitudinal study. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 744751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, M. L. (1975). Moral internalization, parental power, and the nature of parent–child interaction. Developmental Psychology, 11, 228239.Google Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G. (2002). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 191195.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (1995). Mother–child mutually positive affect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal control as correlates of early internalization. Child Development, 66, 236254.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Development of mutual responsiveness between parents and their young children. Child Development, 75, 16571676.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Carlson, J. J. (2005). Temperament, relationships, and young children's receptive cooperation with their parents. Developmental Psychology, 41, 648660.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Nichols, K. E. (2003). Maternal power assertion in discipline and moral discourse contexts: Commonalities, differences, and implications for children's moral conduct and cognition. Developmental Psychology, 39, 949963.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Barry, R. A., Aksan, N., & Boldt, L. J. (2008). A developmental model of maternal and child contributions to disruptive conduct: The first six years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 12201227.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Forman, D. R., & Coy, K. C. (1999). Implications of the mother–child relationship in infancy for socialization in the second year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 249265.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., & Boldt, L. J. (2013). Origins of children's externalizing behavior problems in low-income families: Toddlers’ willing stance toward their mothers as the missing link. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 891901.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Kim, S., Boldt, L. J., & Nordling, J. K. (2013). Promoting toddlers’ positive social-emotional outcomes in low-income families: A play-based experimental study. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42, 700712.Google Scholar
Kuczynski, L., & De Mol, J. (2015). Dialectical models of socialization. In Overton, W. F. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 1. Theory and method (7th ed., pp. 323368). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kuczynski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Development of children's noncompliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 26, 398408.Google Scholar
Kuczynski, L., Kochanska, G., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Girnius-Brown, O. (1987). A developmental interpretation of young children's noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 23, 799806.Google Scholar
Laible, D. (2004a). Mother-child discourse about a child's past behavior at 30 months and early socioemotional development at age 3. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 50, 159180.Google Scholar
Laible, D. (2004b). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology, 40, 979992.Google Scholar
Laible, D., & Thompson, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse, attachment security, shared positive affect, and early conscience development. Child Development, 71, 14241440.Google Scholar
Lay, K. L., Waters, E., & Park, K. A. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and child compliance: The role of mood as mediator. Child Development, 60, 14051411.Google Scholar
Lipscomb, S. T., Leve, L. D., Harold, G. T., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., Ge, X., et al. (2011). Trajectories of parenting and child negative emotionality during infancy and toddlerhood: A longitudinal analysis. Child Development, 82, 16611675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Londerville, S., & Main, M. (1981). Security of attachment, compliance, and maternal training methods in the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 17, 289299.Google Scholar
Lorber, M. F., & Egeland, B. (2011). Parenting and infant difficulty: Testing a mutual exacerbation hypothesis to predict early onset conduct problems. Child Development, 82, 20062020.Google Scholar
Lytton, H. (1977). Correlates of compliance and the rudiments of conscience in two-year-old boys. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 9, 242251.Google Scholar
Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys’ conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psychology, 26, 683697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Let's not overattribute to the attribution process: Comments on social cognition and behavior. In Higgins, E. T., Ruble, D. N., & Hartup, W. W. (Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A sociocultural perspective (pp. 356370). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parents in the socialization of children: An historical overview. Developmental Psychology, 28, 10061017.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1999). The uniqueness of the parent-child relationship. In Collins, W. A. & Laursen, B. (Eds.), Relationships as developmental contexts: Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 157175). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (2007). Historical overview of socialization research and theory. In Grusec, J. & Hastings, P. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization (pp. 1341). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interactions. In Mussen, P. H. (Series Ed.) & Hetherington, E. M. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed., pp. 1101). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Markland, D. (2007). The golden rule is that there are no golden rules: A commentary on Paul Barrett's recommendations for reporting model fit in structural equation modelling. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 851858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, J. A. (1981). A longitudinal study of the consequences of early mother–infant interaction: A microanalytic approach. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46(3, Serial No. 190), 158.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491495.Google Scholar
Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49, 547556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahon, R. J., & Forehand, R. (2003). Helping the noncompliant child: A clinician's guide to effective parent training. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Moss, E., Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., Tarabulsy, G. M., St-Laurent, D., & Bernier, A. (2011). Efficacy of a home-visiting intervention aimed at improving maternal sensitivity, child attachment, and behavioral outcomes for maltreated children: A randomized control trial. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 195210.Google Scholar
Pardini, D. A. (2008). Novel insights into longstanding theories of bidirectional parent-child influences: Introduction to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 627631.Google Scholar
Parke, R. D., & Buriel, R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and ecological perspectives. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 429504). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Parpal, M., & Maccoby, E. E. (1985). Maternal responsiveness and subsequent child compliance. Child Development, 56, 13261334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 329335.Google Scholar
Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Bank, L. (1984). Family interaction: A process model of deviancy training. Aggressive Behavior, 10, 253267.Google Scholar
Patterson, G. R., Forgatch, M. S., & Degarmo, D. S. (2010). Cascading effects following intervention. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 949970.Google Scholar
Pettit, G. S., & Arsiwalla, D. D. (2008). Commentary on special section on “Bidirectional parent-child relationships”: The continuing evolution of dynamic, transactional models of parenting and youth behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 711718.Google Scholar
Pleck, J. H. (2010). Paternal involvement: Revised conceptualization and theoretical linkages with child outcomes. In Lamb, M. E. (Ed.), The role of father in child development (5th ed., pp. 5893). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child temperament and parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 255277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reid, J. B., & Patterson, G. R. (1989). The development of antisocial behaviour patterns in childhood and adolescence. European Journal of Personality, 3, 107119.Google Scholar
Ridenour, T. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Cook, E. T. (2006). Structure and validity of people in my life: A self-report measure of attachment in late childhood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 10371053.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype → environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424435.Google Scholar
Schaffer, H. R., & Crook, C. K. (1980). Child compliance and maternal control techniques Developmental Psychology, 16, 5461.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S. (2003). Innovative approaches and methods to the study of children's conduct problems. Social Development, 12, 309313.Google Scholar
Shaw, D. S., & Bell, R. Q. (1993). Developmental theories of parental contributors to antisocial behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 21, 493518.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Winter, C. C., & Patterson, G. R. (2014). Coercive family process and early-onset conduct problems from age 2 to school entry. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 917932.Google Scholar
Sprafkin, J., Gadow, K. D., Salisbury, H., Schneider, J., & Loney, J. (2002). Further evidence of reliability and validity of the Child Symptom Inventory—4: Parent checklist in clinically referred boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 513524.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1997). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stayton, D. J., Hogan, R., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1971). Infant obedience and maternal behavior: The origins of socialization reconsidered. Child Development, 42, 10571069.Google Scholar
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71, 10721085.Google Scholar
Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically based tests for the number of common factors. Paper presented at the annual spring meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand, P. S. (2002). Coordination of maternal directives with preschoolers’ behavior: Influence of maternal coordination training on dyadic activity and child compliance. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 615.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2006a). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Series Eds.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 2498). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2006b). Conversation and developing understanding: Introduction to the special issue. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 52, 116.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2014). Conscience development in early childhood. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 7392). New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2015). Relationships, regulation, and early development. In Lamb, M. E. & Garcia Coll, C. (Eds.), Social, emotional, and personality development: Vol. 3. Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., pp. 201246). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Attachment, emergent morality, and aggression: Toward a developmental socioemotional model of antisocial behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 703728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waters, E. (1987). Attachment Behavior Q-Set (Revision 3.0). Unpublished manuscript, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Waters, E., & Deane, K. (1985). Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1/2, Serial No. 209), 4165.Google Scholar
Waters, E., Hay, D., & Richters, J. (1986). Infant-parent attachment and the origins of prosocial and antisocial behavior. In Olweus, D., Block, J., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior: Research, theories, and issues (pp. 97125). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (2011). The Incredible Years program for children from infancy to pre-adolescence: Prevention and treatment of behavior problems. In Murrihy, R. C., Kidman, A. D., & Ollendick, T. H. (Eds.), Clinical handbook of assessing and treating conduct problems in youth (pp. 117138). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with non-normal variables: Problems and remedies. In Hoyle, R. (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 5675). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Westerman, M. A. (1990). Coordination of maternal directives with preschoolers’ behavior in compliance-problem and healthy dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26, 621630.Google Scholar