Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T21:26:17.192Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pathways to substance use: Examining conduct problems and parenting behaviors from preschool to adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 February 2023

Megan M. Hare*
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA
Elisa M. Trucco
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA
Samuel W. Hawes
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA
Michelle Villar
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA
Robert A. Zucker
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
*
Corresponding author: Megan M. Hare, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While many studies have identified risk and protective factors of substance use (SU), few have assessed the reciprocal associations of child conduct problems (CP) and parenting practices and behaviors in the prediction of SU across development. A greater understanding of how these factors relate over time is needed to improve the timing of targeted prevention efforts. This study examined how child CP, parenting behaviors, and parents’ own antisocial behavior relate from preschool to adolescence and eventuate in SU. Participants included 706 youth (70.6% male; 89.7% white) enrolled in the Michigan Longitudinal Study. Data from waves 1 (ages 3–5), 2 (ages 6–8), 3 (ages 9–11), 4 (ages 12–14), and 5 (ages 15–17) were included. A random intercept cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM) examined reciprocal associations between parenting practices, parents’ antisocial behavior, and child CP over time (waves 1–4) and how these factors contribute to adolescent alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use (wave 5). At the within-person level, negative parenting and parents’ own antisocial behavior had a strong influence in late childhood/early adolescence. Only child CP emerged as a significant predictor of SU. Results highlight the importance of early intervention and the potential influence of parenting and child factors throughout development in the prevention of SU.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Adolescent substance use (SU) can lead to a cascade of maladaptive outcomes, including academic failure, delinquency, suicidality, and SU disorders (Meier et al., Reference Meier, Caspi, Ambler, Harrington, Houts, Keefe and Moffitt2012; Miotto et al., Reference Miotto, Heydari, Tartter, Chang, Thomas and Ray2013; Schulte & Hser, Reference Schulte and Hser2013). This is concerning as 25.8% of high school seniors reported using alcohol, 19.5% reported using marijuana, and 4.1% reported smoking cigarettes in the past 30 days (Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, Miech, O’Malley, Bachman, Schulenberg and Patrick2022). While research has identified multiple risk factors for adolescent SU, including child characteristics and parenting behaviors (Trucco, Reference Trucco2020; Trucco & Hartmann, Reference Trucco and Hartmann2021), limited work has examined how these factors unfold across development and how transactional influences increase risk or protection for subsequent SU. Understanding how these bidirectional associations relate over time and lead to adolescent SU can help identify potential critical periods and early targets for preventative efforts. The current study examines how child conduct problems (CP), parenting practices, and parents’ own antisocial behavior relate over time and eventuate in adolescent SU.

In the SU literature, many studies have focused on the cascade model of development, which posits that early risk factors (e.g. child externalizing behaviors) start a negative developmental trajectory that leads to the onset of SU (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Greenberg and Malone2008). It also highlights how disruptions in salient issues at each stage of development can predict negative outcomes at the next stage. Within this model, one of the strongest and most well-documented predictors of later SU is child CP (i.e., lying, stealing, getting into fights). This is important as children who display CP in early childhood tend to show a more persistent and chronic trajectory of antisocial behavior extending into later childhood and adulthood (Moffitt & Caspi, Reference Moffitt and Caspi2001), with some research suggesting these children go on to represent a large portion of adolescent crime (Offord et al., Reference Offord, Boyle and Racine1991). Furthermore, CP in early childhood has shown modest to moderate correlations with CP and more serious forms of antisocial behavior in middle childhood and adolescence (Caspi et al.,Reference Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt and Silva1995; Olson et al., Reference Olson, Bates, Sandy and Lanthier2000). Across development, children with CP may display behaviors such as physical aggression, lying, and destruction of property, while other behaviors (e.g., stealing) may appear as children get older and/or have the opportunity to engage in such behaviors. Given that CP can be identified as early as preschool, combined with findings that CP typically precedes SU initiation (Fergusson et al., Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Ridder2007; Le Blanc & Loeber, Reference Le Blanc and Loeber1998), it is important to understand not only the etiology of CP, but also how the early development of CP may impact later SU. Additionally, not all children with early CP initiate SU in adolescence (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Shaw and Gilliom2000), underscoring the need to identify what factors (e.g. parenting) may contribute to CP and mitigate or exacerbate the risk for SU.

Parenting

In addition to child factors, the cascade model also emphasizes the importance of socialization contexts, including parenting practices and behaviors (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Greenberg and Malone2008; Dodge & Pettit, Reference Dodge and Pettit2003) in shaping an adolescent’s decision to engage in SU. Positive parenting (i.e. warm, supportive parenting) has been shown to serve as a protective factor by reducing the early onset of SU (Krohn et al., Reference Krohn, Larroulet, Thornberry and Loughran2019; Tharp & Noonan, Reference Tharp and Noonan2012; Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Donovan, Masten, Mattson and Moss2008). When parents provide warm and supportive environments, youth are more likely to become self-confident, develop coping skills, and have increased resistance to deviant peers (Wills et al., Reference Wills, Gibbons, Gerrard, Murry and Brody2003; Wolfradt et al., Reference Wolfradt, Hempel and Miles2003; Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Brendgen and Tremblay2000), all of which protect against early SU (Mayberry et al., Reference Mayberry, Espelage and Koenig2009). On the other hand, negative parenting (i.e., hostility, criticism) can lead to low levels of self-regulation in youth and increased engagement with deviant peers, leading to increased SU (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Steinberg, Houts and Halpern-Felsher2010; Hummel et al., Reference Hummel, Shelton, Heron, Moore and van den Bree2013).

Parenting is also critical in the development and maintenance of CP. Though some children with CP can demonstrate moderate stability in symptoms over time, positive and negative parenting practices have been shown to impact the frequency and severity of CP across development. For example, interventions targeting positive parenting (e.g. positive reinforcement, warmth) have been shown to improve children’s early problem behavior (Dishion et al., Reference Dishon, Shaw, Connell, Gardner, Weaver and Wilson2008). In contrast, negative parenting and child behavior problems can follow an escalating coercive cycle (Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Patterson, Cicchetti and Cohen2006; Patterson, Reference Patterson1982). Parenting involves an ongoing dynamic process between the parent and child, such that negative parenting increases the problem behaviors of the child, which in turn, is followed by an increase in maladaptive parenting. Understanding how different parenting behaviors and child CP unfold across development and lead to adolescent SU is critical, as there tends to be decreases in positive parenting and increases in negative parenting during early adolescence (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Stattin and Ozdemir2012; Paikoff & Brooks-Gunn, Reference Paikoff and Brooks-Gunn1991). As children with CP present unique challenges, parents may get frustrated over time and feel that their positive parenting behaviors have limited effects, leading them to decrease their positive behaviors and increase their negative, punitive parenting practices.

It is important to note that research supports homotypic continuity with respect to both positive and negative parenting practices. That is, positive and negative parenting practices are represented by the same behaviors and underlying process across development (e.g., Pauli et al., Reference Pauli, Tino, Rogers, Baker, Clanton, Birch, Brown, Daniel, Ferreira, Grisley, Kohls, Baumann, Bernhard, Martinelli, Ackermann, Lazaratou, Tsiakoulia, Bali, Oldenhof and De Brito2021; Smokowski et al., Reference Smokowski, Bacallao, Cotter and Evans2015). Further, a lack of positive parenting (e.g., low warmth) does not equate to high negative parenting (e.g., negative criticism) (Parke et al., Reference Parke, Burks, Carson, Neville, Boyum, Parke and Kellam1994; Pettit et al., Reference Pettit, Bates and Dodge1997). Numerous empirical investigations have shown dimensions of positive and negative parenting differentially relate to child outcomes (Karavasilis et al., Reference Karavasilis, Doyle and Markiewicz2003; Kawabata et al., Reference Kawabata, Alink, Tseng, Van Ijzendoorn and Crick2011; Raby et al., Reference Raby, Roisman, Fraley and Simpson2015), even at the biological level (Hackman et al., Reference Hackman, O’Brien and Zalewski2018; O’Brien et al., Reference O’Brien, Loi, Byrne, Zalewski and Casement2021), which is why they are often differentially targeted in treatment (e.g., Eyberg, Reference Eyberg1988).

Accordingly, examining the unique influence of positive and negative parenting behaviors together across development is critical to gain a more nuanced understanding on the influence of CP and development of adolescent SU.

Furthermore, consistent with Social Learning Theory (Bandura & Walters, Reference Bandura and Walters1977), parent modeling of antisocial behavior (e.g. aggression, impulsivity) may also lead to increased risk for CP and later SU in youth (Smith & Farrington, Reference Smith and Farrington2004; Thornberry, Reference Thornberry2005). Although some research has demonstrated that parents with a history of antisocial behavior and young children report higher levels of stress, which in turn, may lead to ineffective parenting (e.g. negative parenting; Smith & Farrington, Reference Smith and Farrington2004), more work is needed in understanding how these dynamic associations influence each other over time. It may be the case that a more accurate depiction of the negative coercive cycle includes parent antisocial behavior, where parent antisocial behavior impacts CP both directly and indirectly (i.e. through increases in negative parenting), which in turn leads to increased risk for SU during adolescence. While both parenting practices and parent antisocial behavior have been linked to youth CP and SU, little work has examined these aspects of the parenting context in the same model to understand their joint role in the unfolding of youth problem behavior from early childhood to adolescence. Therefore, longitudinal research is necessary to model bidirectional effects to help illuminate critical periods where interventions aimed at reducing CP and SU can have the most utility.

The current study

While there is supportive evidence for the cascade model of SU (Otten et al., Reference Otten, Mun, Shaw, Wilson and Dishion2019; Trucco et al., Reference Trucco, Hicks, Villafuerte, Nigg, Burmeister and Zucker2016), few studies have assessed the reciprocal associations proposed in the model in the prediction of SU. Furthermore, most of this literature has utilized a cross-lagged panel design (CLPM), which can conflate within-person and between-person effects, leading to potentially distorted estimates of cross-lagged effects (Berry & Willoughby, Reference Berry and Willoughby2017; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). In addition, although research has linked CP to SU (Fergusson et al., Reference Fergusson, Horwood and Ridder2007), and separately linked parenting to CP (Goulter et al., Reference Goulter, McMahon, Pasalich and Dodge2020; Pasalich et al., Reference Pasalich, Witkiewitz, McMahon and Pinderhughes2016) and SU (Cleveland et al., Reference Cleveland, Feinberg, Bontempo and Greenberg2008; Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Stattin and Ozdemir2012), few studies have concurrently examined these associations from early childhood to adolescence (Krohn et al., Reference Krohn, Larroulet, Thornberry and Loughran2019; Sitnick et al., Reference Sitnick, Shaw and Hyde2014). This substantially limits our knowledge of how CP, parenting practices, and antisocial behaviors become interwoven across the child’s development and contribute to early SU. A more precise understanding of how these factors relate over time is needed to identify critical developmental periods to inform more targeted prevention programs. Therefore, the current study fills these gaps by using a random intercept CLPM (RI-CLPM) to examine reciprocal associations between parenting practices, parent antisocial behavior, and child CP over time and how these factors contribute to alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use (substances that are typically used among youth) during adolescence. Specifically, the RI-CLPM appropriately accounts for the trait-like, time-invariant stability of psychological constructs by parsing out the within-person and between-person effects across time. While between- and within person-effects will be presented, the current study focuses on the within-person associations, as they may help to identify modifiable targets for intervention (Berry & Willoughby, Reference Berry and Willoughby2017; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015).

Overall, we hypothesized a transactional relation in which parenting practices (i.e., positive and negative parenting) and child CP mutually influence each other over time. Regarding the predicted associations across the model, although research has shown the importance of positive parenting across development, previous research suggests that positive parenting may decline in late childhood/early adolescence, especially in children with CP (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Stattin and Ozdemir2012; Paikoff & Brooks-Gunn, Reference Paikoff and Brooks-Gunn1991). Therefore, we predict that positive parenting will have a stronger association with CP in early/middle childhood (ages 3–8; waves 1–2) compared to later waves. On the other hand, as negative parenting has been shown to increase in late childhood/early adolescence (Laursen et al., Reference Laursen, Coy and Collins1998; Meeus, Reference Meeus2018), we hypothesized that negative parenting will have a stronger association with CP in late childhood/early adolescence (ages 9–14; waves 3–4) compared to earlier waves.

Given prior work demonstrating that a parent’s own antisocial behavior can increase children’s behavior problems (Smith & Farrington, Reference Smith and Farrington2004; Thornberry, Reference Thornberry2005), we hypothesized that parent antisocial behavior will impact child CP across all waves (ages 3–14; waves 1–4). However, as previous work has shown that antisocial behavior in adulthood tends to be stable over time, with limited work suggesting child factors have a significant influence on parent antisocial behavior (Coie & Dodge, Reference Coie, Dodge, Damon and Eisenberg1998; Conger et al., Reference Conger, Neppl, Kim and Scaramella2003), we hypothesized that child CP would not impact parent antisocial behavior at any wave. Lastly, given previous research demonstrating the unique importance of parent and child factors influencing SU (Trucco, Reference Trucco2020), we hypothesized that all parenting practices and parent antisocial behavior, as well as child CP, assessed during early adolescence (ages 12–14; wave 4) would uniquely be associated with use of all substances (i.e., alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use) at wave 5 (ages 15–17).

Materials and method

Participants

Participants were youth enrolled in the Michigan Longitudinal Study (MLS), a prospective study examining the development of SU disorders among high-risk families (for a more detailed description of study design, recruitment strategies and participants, see Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Fitzgerald, Refior, Puttler, Pallas, Ellis, Fitzgerald, Lester and Zuckerman2000). The study recruited community families in three risk categories: (1) high risk families had fathers convicted of drunk driving with a high blood alcohol concentration and an alcohol use disorder (24.9%); (2) moderate risk families were community-identified fathers with an alcohol use disorder (AUD) diagnosis, but no drunk driving offense (43.7%); and (3) control group/low risk families from the same neighborhoods but without an AUD diagnosis or a drunk driving conviction (31.4%). Mother diagnosis was free to vary in both high and moderate risk groups. Study exclusionary criteria ruled out the presence of fetal alcohol syndrome. The MLS maintained an 89% retention rate over the course of the study. Inclusion criteria of the original study protocol led to the sample being primarily male (70.6%), while the geographic region in which the study took place led to the sample being largely white (79.4%) (see Table 1 for full demographic information).

Table 1. Demographics

Note.

a In this data set, child sex was conceptualized as a binary variable (i.e., male/female).

Procedure

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Michigan. Parents and children completed assessments following initial enrollment (wave 1, ages 3 to 5; M age = 4.40, SD = 1.01) with assessments occurring every 3 years. For this study, analyses were conducted on measures collected during wave 1, wave 2 (ages 6–8; M age = 7.61, SD = 0.96), wave 3 (ages 9–11; M age = 10.51, SD = 0.93), wave 4 (ages 12–14; M age = 13.51, SD = 0.92) and wave 5 (ages 15–17; M age = 16.58, SD = 0.97). Informed consent was obtained from the parents, and assent was obtained from the child after study procedures were reviewed. Given the proposed longitudinal models, analyses focused on participants with available data for at least two of the waves in the cross-lagged analyses (i.e., waves 1–4) for each variable (i.e., positive parenting, negative parenting, parent antisocial; n = 706; see Supplementary Table 1). Because of their generally stronger association to offspring during the childhood years, increased availability of complete data, and to reduce variablity in having multiple reporters, mothers were selected as the primary reporter for the measures. Participants included in the current analyses did not differ significantly from the full MLS sample (N = 1,250) on biological sex, race, income, parenting, or CP.

Participants with missing data at each time point were compared to participants with no missing data. Youth with available negative parenting wave 1 data (χ 2 = 56.82, p < .001), positive parenting wave 1 data (χ 2 = 56.82, p < .001), and child CP wave 1 data (χ 2 = 22.53, p < .001), significantly differed by child sex. Namely, there was more missing information from parents of females compared to males. Additionally, youth with available negative parenting data at waves 1–4 (χ 2 range 57.46–105.91, p < .001), positive parenting data at waves 1–4 (χ 2 range 57.46–109.10, p < .001), parent antisocial behavior data at wave 1 (χ 2 = 161.04, p < .001), child CP data at wave 1 (χ 2 = 23.30, p < .001), and child CP data at wave 4 (χ 2 = 31.78, p < .001) significantly differed by race. Namely, there was more missing information from parents of non-White youth. Differences in patterns of missingness across race and biological sex may be due in part to the original study design (see Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Fitzgerald, Refior, Puttler, Pallas, Ellis, Fitzgerald, Lester and Zuckerman2000).

Measures

Conduct problems

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach & Edelbrock, Reference Achenbach and Edelbrock1991) was completed at waves 1–4 (ages 3–14). Mothers were asked to rate their child’s social and emotional functioning. This questionnaire has shown good construct validity and good test-retest reliability (Achenbach & Edelbrock, Reference Achenbach and Edelbrock1991). For the purposes of this study, a mean score of the delinquency subscale of the CBCL was used to assess CP. The delinquency subscale has been used in previous studies to represent CP in children, has shown to correlate with other measures of CP, and has differentiated children with and without conduct disorder (Ebesutani et al., Reference Ebesutani, Bernstein, Nakamura, Chorpita, Higa-McMillan and Weisz2010; Eiraldi et al., Reference Eiraldi, Power, Karustis and Goldstein2000). However, given that SU was an outcome of interest in the current study, the questions assessing SU were removed (α range = 0.66–0.80 across waves).

Positive and negative parenting

For waves 1 and 2, parenting was assessed via the Child Rearing Practices Report (Block, Reference Block1965). Mothers were asked to rate how descriptive statements are of their actual behavior toward their child. In line with previous research (Deković et al., Reference Deković, Janssens and Gerris1991; Hofer et al., Reference Hofer, Buckels, White, Beall and Schaller2018; Rickel & Biasatti, Reference Rickel and Biasatti1982), two subscales were created: positive parenting (18 items) and negative parenting (21 items). For each subscale, scores were summed together and then an average was taken. Positive parenting broadly reflects warmth. Example items are, “I feel a child should be given comfort and understanding when upset,” and “I express affection by hugging, kissing, and holding my child” (αs = 0.66, 0.76). The negative parenting subscale broadly describes criticism and harsh parenting. Example items are, “I believe that scolding and criticism makes my child improve” and “I control my child by warning them about the bad things that can happen to them” (αs = 0.69, 0.75).

For waves 3 and 4, parenting was assessed using the Parent Perception Inventory (PPI-P) (Hazzard et al., Reference Hazzard, Christensen and Margolin1983), an 18-item questionnaire developed to assess parenting styles from the parent’s point of view. The PPI-P is divided into two subscales: positive and negative parenting. Scores for each subscale were summed together and then an average was taken. Sample questions for positive parenting include, how often do you, “talk to your child when they feel bad,” and “help them to feel better, comfort them.” Sample questions for negative parenting are, how often do you, “tell your child they are no good, criticize them,” and “threaten them, tell them if they will get into trouble if they do something wrong.” The positive (αs = 0.78, 0.82) and negative parenting (αs = 0.78, 0.80) subscales demonstrated good internal consistency across waves. As the two parenting measures were on slightly different scales (i.e. 7-point Likert scale and 5-point Likert scale) scores for positive and negative parenting were z-scored on both measures across all waves.

Parent antisocial behavior

Mother’s own antisocial behavior was assessed using the Antisocial Behavior Checklist-R (ASB-R) (Zucker & Noll, Reference Zucker and Noll1980), where mothers were asked to rate their frequency of participation in a variety of antisocial activities. This instrument is a revision of an earlier antisocial behavior inventory utilized in the Rutgers Community Study (Zucker & Barron, Reference Zucker and Barron1973; Zucker & Fillmore, Reference Zucker and Fillmore1968), that has been modified so that items are also salient for adult antisocial activity. A total of 35 items were culled from the original adolescent measure and others were added from clinical descriptions of adult antisocial personalities. The ASB-R has demonstrated good reliability and validity and adequate test-retest reliability across populations (Eiden et al., Reference Eiden, Chavez and Leonard1999; Zucker & Noll, Reference Zucker and Noll1980), with the current study demonstrating good internal consistency (αs range = 0.80–0.84) across all waves. Example items include, “Shoplifted merchandise valued over $25” and “Been fired for poor job performance.” An average score was used.

Substance use

SU in adolescence was measured by the Drinking and Drug History Questionnaire - Youth Version (Zucker et al., Reference Zucker, Fitzgerald and Noll1990) assessed at wave 5. Alcohol use was assessed with a single item capturing the number of days the participant used per month during the past 6 months. Cigarette use was assessed with a single item, asking participants how frequently they have smoked cigarettes in the past 30 days (0 = not at all to 6 = two packs or more per day). Marijuana use was also assessed with a single item, asking participants how frequently they smoked marijuana in the past 30 days (0 = never to 9 = 500 times and above).

Data analytic plan

First, zero-order correlations among the variables were examined. Then, a RI-CLPM was estimated in MPlus 8.1 (Muthén et al., Reference Muthén, Muthén and Asparouhov2017) using full information maximum likelihood to examine the bidirectional associations between child CP, parenting practices, and antisocial behavior on subsequent adolescent SU. Given that some variables were non-normally distributed, maximum likelihood parameter estimates with robust standard errors (MLR) was used to account for non-normality.

Autoregressive paths for each variable were systemically constrained and unconstrained and tested using the Satorra-Bentler Difference Tests to determine whether these paths should be freely estimated or constrained (Satorra & Bentler, Reference Satorra and Bentler1988). The following fit statistics were used to evaluate model fit: chi-square (p > .05 excellent), comparative fit index (CFI; >.90 acceptable, >.95 excellent), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; <.08 acceptable; <.05 excellent), and the standard root mean square residual (SRMR; <.08 acceptable, <.05 excellent) (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999). The model included autoregressive and cross-lagged paths for positive parenting, negative parenting, parent antisocial behavior, and child CP across ages 3–14 (waves 1–4). Paths were also estimated between CP and each parenting variable at wave 4 on alcohol, marijuana, and cigarette use for ages 15–17 (wave 5).

Post-hoc

Post hoc indirect effects of positive parenting, negative parenting, and parent antisocial behavior at wave 3 on SU at wave 5, through CP at wave 4 were also estimated. The Model Indirect command with 500 bootstrapped samples in Mplus was utilized to calculate a standardized indirect effect parameter and bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals. Importantly, it is not possible to estimate bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals while utilizing MLR. Therefore, indirect effects using the IND command and MLR estimation were compared. As results were the same, results for post hoc analyses are presented using bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals.

Results

Means, standard deviations, and correlations for study variables are presented in Table 2. Utilizing the Satorra-Bentler Scaled Difference Tests, constraining the autoregressive paths to be equal across waves for positive parenting (Δχ 2 (2) = 22.71 p < .001), negative parenting (Δχ 2 (2) = 38.89, p < .001), parent antisocial behavior (Δχ 2 (2) = 45.78, p < .001), and CP (Δχ 2 (2) = .98, p = .615), did not result in a significant decrement in model fit, Therefore, all paths were constrained to be equal. The final model demonstrated acceptable model fit (χ 2 (166) = 327.71, p < .001, CFI = .930, RMSEA = .050, 90% CI [.042, .057], and SRMR = .052).

Table 2. Descriptive and correlation

Note. CP = conduct problems, W = wave, PP = positive parenting, NP = negative parenting.

Model results

Between effects

Between-person effects for positive parenting were negatively correlated with between-person effects for negative parenting, CP, and parent antisocial behavior (see Table 3). Between-person effects for negative parenting were positively correlated with between-person effects for CP and parent antisocial behavior. Lastly, between-person effects for CP were positively correlated with between-person effects for parent antisocial behavior.

Table 3. Random intercept cross-lagged panel model results

Note. CP = conduct problems, W = Wave.

* p < .05,

** p < .01,

*** p < .001.

Within-person effects

CP at wave 1 were significantly associated with negative parenting at wave 2, such that more CP led to greater negative parenting (see Figure 1). There were no other significant cross-lagged paths from wave 1 to wave 2 (see Table 3).

Figure 1. Bidirectional associations of positive parenting, negative parenting, parent antisocial behavior, and child conduct problems in the prediction of substance use. Figure shows significant associations. Within-time correlations were estimated but not shown in the figure for parsimony. Between-person effects are also not presented in figure (see Table 3). *p< .05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. Anti = parent antisocial behavior; Neg = negative parenting; Pos = positive parenting; CP = conduct problems.

Parent antisocial behavior at waves 2 and 3 were significantly associated with greater CP at waves 3 and 4, respectively. Parent antisocial behavior and CP at wave 2 were significantly associated with greater negative parenting at wave 3, which in turn lead to greater CP at wave 4. Additionally, CP at wave 3 was significantly associated with greater negative parenting at wave 4. Lastly, CP at wave 4 was significantly associated with greater alcohol use, marijuana use, and cigarette use at wave 5. Positive parenting, negative parenting, and parent antisocial behavior at wave 4 did not significantly predict SU at wave 5 (see Table 3).

Post-hoc results

Negative parenting at wave 3 demonstrated a significant indirect effect on alcohol use (β = .05, SE = .02, p = .008, 95% CI [.01, .09]), marijuana use (β = .06, SE = .02, p = .008, 95% CI [.01, .08]), and cigarette use (β = .05, SE = .02, p = .009, 95% CI [.01, .08]) at wave 5, through child CP at wave 4. Similarly, parent antisocial behavior at wave 3 also demonstrated a significant indirect effect on alcohol use (β = .07, SE = .02, p = .002, 95% CI [.02, .11]), marijuana use (β = .07, SE = .02, p = .002, 95% CI [.01, .10]), and cigarette use (β = .06, SE = .02, p = .002, 95% CI [.02, .09]) at wave 5, through child CP at wave 4. In contrast, positive parenting at wave 3 did not demonstrate a significant indirect effect on alcohol use (β = –.02, SE = .11, p = .223, 95% CI [–.06, .01]), marijuana use (β = –.02, SE = .05, p = .224, 95% CI [–.06, .01]), or cigarette use (β = –.02, SE = .05, p = .224, 95% CI [–.06, .01]) at wave 5, through child CP at wave 4.

Discussion

Despite numerous prevention and intervention efforts, SU remains an international public health problem. Preventing the onset of SU and/or delaying the age of SU initiation can significantly reduce the risk of later SU addiction severity (Harerimana et al., Reference Harerimana, Kerr, Csiernik, Ng, Rutembesa and Forchuk2022). When examining bidirectional associations of parenting behaviors and child CP over time, multiple pathways to SU emerged. In line with cascade models (Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Greenberg and Malone2008), child CP emerged as an early risk factor for adolescent SU. Inconsistent with our hypotheses, positive parenting showed no cross-lagged effects across development. Lastly, only CP was associated with later SU in adolescence.

The current study supports the cascade model utilizing more rigorous techniques (i.e., RI-CLPM), with greater CP at wave 1 leading to increased negative parenting at wave 2. On the other hand, parent antisocial behavior at wave 1 did not predict child CP at wave 2; however, parent antisocial behavior did predict CP at all subsequent waves. In line with Social Information Processing theory (Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1994; Dodge et al., Reference Dodge, Pettit, McClaskey, Brown and Gottman1986), young children may not be fully aware of their parents’ antisocial behavior. For example, children (ages 6–8) may be less aware if their parents shoplifted or why they lost their job. When children are this young, parents may also make more of an effort to hide these types of behaviors. Further, younger children may not have the means to engage in antisocial behaviors, and therefore only model antisocial behaviors when they are older and spend more unsupervised time with peers.

Although parent antisocial behavior and negative parenting at wave 1 had no significant cross-lagged effects to wave 2, as children moved into middle childhood to early adolescence (i.e., waves 2–4), coercive cycles between parent antisocial behavior, negative parenting, and CP emerged. For youth, the developmental period of late childhood/early adolescence, is marked by an increase in challenging rules and authority figures, independence from parents, peer influence, and exposure to more opportunities for risky behavior (Steinberg & Silk, Reference Steinberg, Silk and Bornstein2002). This increase in autonomy, mobility, and inclination to challenge rules can lead to increased parent–child conflict. Parents may want more control and closeness with their teen, while teens may get annoyed and want more independence, leading parents to become upset and adopt a more negative parenting style (Laursen & Collins, Reference Laursen and Collins2009; Smetana, Reference Smetana2011; Steinberg & Silk, Reference Steinberg, Silk and Bornstein2002). In many families, this period of conflict is gradually resolved when youth get older, as parents come to respect the autonomy and decision making of their maturing child, while youth learn to communicate and regulate their emotions more effectively (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2001; Steinberg & Silk, Reference Steinberg, Silk and Bornstein2002).

However, this negative coercive cycle may be more severe in youth with CP, as they tend to be more impulsive, display lower empathy, and affiliate with more deviant peers. As parents of youth with CP experience higher rates of parenting stress and more conflict in the parent-child relationship (Muñoz-Silva et al., Reference Muñoz-Silva, Lago-Urbano, Sanchez-Garcia and Carmona-Márquez2017; Raudino et al., Reference Raudino, Woodward, Fergusson and Horwood2012), this developmental transition may be more difficult and more of a trigger for parents who also demonstrate antisocial behaviors. The current study highlights the importance of considering how parents’ own psychopathology, specifically antisocial behavior, may influence their own parenting practices in addition to child CP. Further, although no cross-lagged paths emerged from parent antisocial behavior and negative parenting at wave 1 to wave 2, both constructs play an important role across development. Therefore, it may be the case that early intervention on such risk factors may help prevent or reduce symptoms of CP, and hopefully, later SU.

In contrast with our hypotheses, positive parenting had no significant effect across any of the cross-lagged associations. Children high in CP, even at younger ages (i.e., prior to wave 1), may exhaust parents (Caprara et al., Reference Caprara, Dodge, Pastorelli and Zelli2007; McLoyd, Reference McLoyd1990), leading to lower levels of positive parenting (e.g., less warmth). However, as parents become more frustrated and feel that their positive parenting behaviors have minimal impact on shaping child behavior, lower levels of positive parenting may develop into increased levels of negative parenting (e.g., increased hostility). Results from between-person effects support this theory, indicating that parents with lower positive parenting across measurement waves reported higher negative parenting across measurement waves compared to parents high in positive parenting. From a prevention/treatment framework, it may be the case that these families, especially those at higher risk due to parental antisocial behavior or early CP, require early intervention. For example, programs aimed to prevent adolescent problem behaviors have been initiated beginning as early as the prenatal period (Olds, Reference Olds2002), during infancy (Van Zeijl et al., Reference Van Zeijl, Mesman, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, Stolk, Koot and Alink2006), and through the preschool period (Eyberg & Bussing, Reference Eyberg and Bussing2011; Webster-Stratton & Reid, Reference Webster-Stratton, Reid, Weisz and Kazdin2018). While some of these programs have also demonstrated positive outcomes on child CP, they also focus on general indices of positive adjustment, maternal mental and physical health, and improving the mother–child relationship (Shaw, Reference Shaw2013). Further, at the between-person level, results indicate that across time, parenting and CP are related. Results suggest that parents with lower positive parenting are more likely to have greater negative parenting, CP, and antisocial behavior compared with parents high in positive parenting. Results suggest that these variables influence each other and may need to be considered as part of an overall intervention.

Lastly, although parenting behaviors and CP influenced each other across development, at wave 4 (ages 12–14) only CP emerged as having a significant direct effect on SU, further underscoring the importance of early intervention. While our findings are not in line with previous research showing parenting behaviors directly contribute to adolescent SU (e.g., Krohn et al., Reference Krohn, Larroulet, Thornberry and Loughran2019), it is important to note that many of these models did not concurrently include a proximal measure of child CP or parent antisocial behavior. This highlights the importance of considering multiple parent and child factors over time to better understand early pathways to SU. Further, much of the past literature has not separated within- and between-person effects. It may be the case that some of the auto-regressive and cross-lagged parameters in previous literature may represent an overestimation (Mulder & Hamaker, Reference Mulder and Hamaker2021).

Additionally, post-hoc analyses demonstrated that negative parenting and parent antisocial behavior at wave 3 had an indirect effect on SU at wave 5 through CP at wave 4. As youth have increased independence during adolescence, the direct impact of parenting behaviors on a youth’s decision to engage in SU may be reduced. Although parents are the key socialization unit during childhood and early adolescence, as more time is spent outside of the family, this shifts to peers becoming the primary socialization unit during middle to late adolescence (Catalano & Hawkins, Reference Catalano, Hawkins and Hawkins1996; Furman & Buhrmester, Reference Furman and Buhrmester1992). Therefore, it may be the case that other factors that have more direct links with deviant peer affiliation, such as parental monitoring, or peer relationships themselves may have a stronger, direct impact on SU and may be more important to target in treatment during this developmental period (Crawford & Novak, Reference Crawford and Novak2002; Rusby et al., Reference Rusby, Light, Crowley and Westling2018).

Limitations

While this is one of the first studies to examine the bidirectional associations between parenting practices, parent antisocial behavior, and youth CP in the prediction of SU across development using a RI-CLPM, it is not without its limitations. The sample was predominantly white and male, limiting generalizability. Testing sex as a potential moderator in this study was not possible given the low percentage of females in the sample. As there are higher rates of CP and SU in males, and potential sex differences among parenting practices (Camacho-Thompson & Simpkins, Reference Camacho-Thompson and Simpkins2020; Griffin et al., Reference Griffin, Botvin, Scheier, Diaz and Miller2000), future work should examine whether sex moderates these associations. In addition, it may also be the case that mother’s own psychopathology or parenting style might influence their response in terms of reporting their own antisocial behavior and parenting practices, as well as the behavior of their child (i.e., CP). Future research should replicate these findings using more objective measures of parenting behaviors and child CP across multiple reporters (e.g., observation).

Additionally, the link between parent antisocial behavior and CP has been shown to have genetic underpinnings (e.g., Beaver et al., Reference Beaver, Wright, DeLisi, Walsh, Vaughn, Boisvert and Vaske2007), which is not accounted for in the current study. However, research has also demonstrated that non-biologically related caregiver’s antisocial behavior impacts child CP (e.g., Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Leve, Harold, Natsuaki, Neiderhiser, Shaw and Reiss2013), suggesting the influence of parents own antisocial behavior on child CP may also occur through socialization consistent with Social Learning Theory (Bandura & Walters, Reference Bandura and Walters1977). As this study employed secondary data analysis, it was also limited in terms of measurement, leading to separate parenting measures for waves 1–2 compared to waves 3–4. However, both measures assessed the same constructs (i.e., positive and negative parenting), had almost identical items, and were significantly correlated with one another across waves (ps <.05). While that does caution the interpretation of the cross-lagged paths that emerged across wave 2 to wave 3 for positive and negative parenting, similar cross-lagged paths were found across waves 2–3 (i.e., different parenting measures), as waves 3–4 (i.e., same parenting measure). Lastly, adolescent SU during the timeframe the study was conducted (1985–2007) may not represent current trends, necessitating replication.

Conclusion

Overall, findings support previous research identifying CP as the start of the developmental cascade to SU, while adding that negative parenting and parents’ antisocial behavior should also be considered. Further, while focusing on parenting behaviors may indirectly impact SU once children reach early adolescence (i.e., 12–14), interventions should also focus on treating CP and reducing the risk for SU. Given previous findings that parents and peers influence adolescent SU (Elkington et al., Reference Elkington, Bauermeister and Zimmerman2011; Trucco, Reference Trucco2020), targeting various socialization influences in treatment could have utility (e.g., Multisystemic Therapy; Randall et al., Reference Randall, Cunningham and Henggeler2018). Overall, our results highlight the importance of early intervention and considering different parenting and child factors throughout development in the prevention of SU, as different associations emerge across developmental periods.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422001328

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the [National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism] under Grants [K08 AA023290; E.M.T. and R37/R01 AA07065; RAZ]; [National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities 2] under Grant [U54 MD012393; E.M.T]; and [National Institute of Drug Abuse] under Grant [T32 DA043449; M.M.H.].

Author contributions

MMH conceived the design of the study, conducted the data analysis, and drafted the manuscript. EMT contributed to the design of the work, assisted in the interpretation of data, and provided substantial edits to the manuscript. MV cleaned the data for analysis and assisted in drafting the Method section. SWH assisted with the interpretation of data and provided significant edits to the manuscript. RAZ was responsible for overseeing data collection for the original Michigan Longitudinal Study and provided significant edits to the manuscript. All authors reviewed the final manuscript.

Funding statement

MMH, EMT, SWM, MV, and RAZ report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

Consent from the subjects was obtained.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1991). Child behavior checklist. Burlington (Vt), 7, 371392.Google Scholar
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1977). Social learning theory. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Beaver, K. M., Wright, J. P., DeLisi, M., Walsh, A., Vaughn, M. G., Boisvert, D., & Vaske, J. (2007). A gene× gene interaction between DRD2 and DRD4 is associated with conduct disorder and antisocial behavior in males. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3, 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-30 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., Houts, R. M., & Halpern-Felsher, B. L. (2010). The development of reproductive strategy in females: Early maternal harshness→ earlier menarche→ increased sexual risk taking. Developmental Psychology, 46, 120. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015549 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, D., & Willoughby, M. T. (2017). On the practical interpretability of cross-lagged panel models: Rethinking a developmental workhorse. Child Development, 88, 11861206. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12660 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J. H. (1965). The child-rearing practices report (CRPR): A set of Q items for the description of parental socialization attitudes and values. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Camacho-Thompson, D. E., & Simpkins, S. D. (2020). Parental involvement in organized after-school activities and adolescent motivational beliefs. Applied Developmental Science, 26, 176191. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2020.1750400 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, S. B., Shaw, D. S., & Gilliom, M. (2000). Early externalizing behavior problems: Toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later maladjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 467488. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579400003114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caprara, G. V., Dodge, K. A., Pastorelli, C., & Zelli, A. (2007). How marginal deviations sometimes grow into serious aggression. Child Development Perspectives, 1, 3339. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00007.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From age three to age fifteen. Child Development, 66, 5568. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00855.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalano, R. F., & Hawkins, J. D. (1996). The social development model: A theory of antisocial behavior. In Hawkins, J. D. (Ed.), Delinquency and crime: Current theories (pp. 149197). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cleveland, M. J., Feinberg, M. E., Bontempo, D. E., & Greenberg, M. T. (2008). The role of risk and protective factors in substance use across adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43, 157164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1998). Aggression and antisocial behavior. In Damon, W. & Eisenberg, N. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 779862). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Conger, R. D., Neppl, T., Kim, K. J., & Scaramella, L. (2003). Angry and aggressive behavior across three generations: A prospective, longitudinal study of parents and children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 143160. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1022570107457 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, L. A., & Novak, K. B. (2002). Parental and peer influences on adolescent drinking: The relative impact of attachment and opportunity. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 12, 126. https://doi.org/10.1300/J029v12n01_01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deković, M., Janssens, J. M., & Gerris, J. R. (1991). Factor structure and construct validity of the block child rearing practices report (CRPR). Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3, 182. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.3.2.182 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., Cicchetti, D., & Cohen, D. J. (2006). Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation. Wiley.Google Scholar
Dishon, T. J., Shaw, D., Connell, A., Gardner, F., Weaver, C., & Wilson, M. (2008). The family check-up with high-risk indigent families: Outcomes of positive parenting and problem behavior from ages 2 through 4 years. Child Development, 79, 13951414. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01195.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., Malone, P. S., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research (2008). Testing an idealized dynamic cascade model of the development of serious violence in adolescence. Child Development, 79, 19071927. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01233.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39, 349. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.349 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., McClaskey, C. L., Brown, M. M., & Gottman, J. M. (1986). Social competence in children. Monographs of the society for research in child development, i-85. https://doi.org/10.2307/1165906 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebesutani, C., Bernstein, A., Nakamura, B. J., Chorpita, B. F., Higa-McMillan, C. K., & Weisz, J. R. (2010). Concurrent validity of the child behavior checklist DSM-oriented scales: Correspondence with DSM diagnoses and comparison to syndrome scales. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32, 373384. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-009-9174-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiden, R. D., Chavez, F., & Leonard, K. E. (1999). Parent–infant interactions among families with alcoholic fathers. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 745762. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579499002308 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eiraldi, R. B., Power, T. J., Karustis, J. L., & Goldstein, S. G. (2000). Assessing ADHD and comorbid disorders in children: the child behavior checklist and the Devereux scales of mental disorders. Journal of Clinical Child & Psychology, 29, 316. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424jccp2901_2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elkington, K. S., Bauermeister, J. A., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2011). Do parents and peers matter? A prospective socio-ecological examination of substance use and sexual risk among African American youth. Journal of Adolescence, 34, 10351047. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.11.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eyberg, S. (1988). Parent-child interaction therapy: Integration of traditional and behavioral concerns. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 10, 3346. https://doi.org/10.1300/J019v10n01_04 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eyberg, S. M., & Bussing, R. (2011). Parent–child interaction therapy for preschool children with conduct problems. In R. Murrihy, A. Kidman, & T. Ollendick (Eds.), Clinical handbook of assessing and treating conduct problems in youth (pp. 139162). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-6297-3_6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Ridder, E. M. (2007). Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: Results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88, S14S26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63(1), 103115. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb03599.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goulter, N., McMahon, R. J., Pasalich, D. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2020). Indirect effects of early parenting on adult antisocial outcomes via adolescent conduct disorder symptoms and callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 49, 930942. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2019.1613999 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, K. W., Botvin, G. J., Scheier, L. M., Diaz, T., & Miller, N. L. (2000). Parenting practices as predictors of substance use, delinquency, and aggression among urban minority youth: Moderating effects of family structure and gender. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14, 174. https://doi.org/10.1037//0893-164x.14.2.174 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gryczkowski, M. R., Jordan, S. S., & Mercer, S. H. (2010). Differential relations between mothers’ and fathers’ parenting practices and child externalizing behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 539546. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-009-9326-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hackman, D. A., O’Brien, J. R., & Zalewski, M. (2018). Enduring association between parenting and cortisol: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89, 14851503. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harerimana, B., Kerr, M., Csiernik, R., Ng, L. C., Rutembesa, E., & Forchuk, C. (2022). Predicting the contribution of age at first substance use and post-traumatic stress disorder to later addiction severity in a clinical sample from Sub-Saharan Africa: Implications for prevention and treatment. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 20, 13701384. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00449-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazzard, A., Christensen, A., & Margolin, G. (1983). Children’s perceptions of parental behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 4959. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00912177 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, M. K., Buckels, E. E., White, C. J., Beall, A. T., & Schaller, M. (2018). Individual differences in activation of the parental care motivational system: An empirical distinction between protection and nurturance. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 907916. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617728994 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hummel, A., Shelton, K. H., Heron, J., Moore, L., & van den Bree, M. B. (2013). A systematic review of the relationships between family functioning, pubertal timing and adolescent substance use. Addiction, 108, 487496. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, L. D., Miech, R. A., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E., & Patrick, M. E. (2022). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use 1975–2021: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Karavasilis, L., Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (2003). Associations between parenting style and attachment to mother in middle childhood and adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27, 153164. https://doi.org/10.1080/0165025024400015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawabata, Y., Alink, L. R., Tseng, W. L., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Crick, N. R. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles associated with relational aggression in children and adolescents: A conceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 31, 240278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.08.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, D. C., Leve, L. D., Harold, G. T., Natsuaki, M. N., Neiderhiser, J. M., Shaw, D. S., & Reiss, D. (2013). Influences of biological and adoptive mothers’ depression and antisocial behavior on adoptees’ early behavior trajectories. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 723734. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9711-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Ozdemir, M. (2012). Perceived parenting style and adolescent adjustment: Revisiting directions of effects and the role of parental knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 48, 15401553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krohn, M. D., Larroulet, P., Thornberry, T. P., & Loughran, T. A. (2019). The effect of childhood conduct problems on early onset substance use: An examination of the mediating and moderating roles of parenting styles. Journal of Drug Issues, 49, 139162. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022042618811784 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laursen, B., & Collins, W. A. (2009). Parent-child relationships during adolescence. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy002002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laursen, B., Coy, K. C., & Collins, W. A. (1998). Reconsidering changes in parent-child conflict across adolescence: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 69, 817832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Blanc, M., & Loeber, R. (1998). Developmental criminology updated. Crime and Justice, 23, 115198. https://doi.org/10.1086/449270 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, M. L., Espelage, D. L., & Koenig, B. (2009). Multilevel modeling of direct effects and interactions of peers, parents, school, and community influences on adolescent substance use. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 10381049. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9425-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development, 61, 311346. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02781.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meeus, W. (2018). Adolescent development: Longitudinal research into the self, personal relationships, and psychopathology. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429465338 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, M. H., Caspi, A., Ambler, A., Harrington, H., Houts, R., Keefe, R. S., … Moffitt, T. E. (2012). Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, E2657E2664. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1206820109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miotto, K., Heydari, A., Tartter, M., Chang, E., Thomas, P. S., & Ray, L. A. (2013). Adolescent alcohol use. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118340851.ch18 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 355375. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579401002097 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28, 638648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñoz-Silva, A., Lago-Urbano, R., Sanchez-Garcia, M., & Carmona-Márquez, J. (2017). Child/adolescent’s ADHD and parenting stress: The mediating role of family impact and conduct problems. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2252. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, B. O., Muthén, L. K., & Asparouhov, T. (2017). Regression and mediation analysis using Mplus. Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
O’Brien, J. R., Loi, E. C., Byrne, M. L., Zalewski, M., & Casement, M. D. (2021). The link between positive and negative parenting behaviors and child inflammation: A systematic review. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-021-01224-4 Google ScholarPubMed
Offord, D. R., Boyle, M. H., & Racine, Y. A. (1991). The epidemiology of antisocial behavior in childhood and adolescence. The Development and Treatment of Childhood Aggression, 17, 3154.Google Scholar
Olds, D. L. (2002). Prenatal and infancy home visiting by nurses: From randomized trials to community replication. Prevention Science, 3, 153172. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1019990432161 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., Sandy, J. M., & Lanthier, R. (2000). Early developmental precursors of externalizing behavior in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 119133. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005166629744 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otten, R., Mun, C. J., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., & Dishion, T. J. (2019). A developmental cascade model for early adolescent-onset substance use: The role of early childhood stress. Addiction, 114, 326334. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14452 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paikoff, R. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1991). Do parent-child relationships change during puberty? Psychological Bulletin, 110, 47. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.47 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parke, R. D., Burks, V. M., Carson, J. L., Neville, B., & Boyum, L. A. (1994). Family–peer relationships: A tripartite model. In Parke, R. D. & Kellam, S. G. (Eds.), Exploring family relationships with other social contexts (pp. 115145). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Pasalich, D. S., Witkiewitz, K., McMahon, R. J., & Pinderhughes, E. E. (2016). Indirect effects of the fast track intervention on conduct disorder symptoms and callous-unemotional traits: Distinct pathways involving discipline and warmth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44, 587597. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0059-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Castalia publishing company.Google Scholar
Pauli, R., Tino, P., Rogers, J. C., Baker, R., Clanton, R., Birch, P., Brown, A., Daniel, G., Ferreira, L., Grisley, L., Kohls, G., Baumann, S., Bernhard, A., Martinelli, A., Ackermann, K., Lazaratou, H., Tsiakoulia, F., Bali, P., Oldenhof, H., … De Brito, S. A. (2021). Positive and negative parenting in conduct disorder with high versus low levels of callous–unemotional traits. Development and Psychopathology, 33, 980991. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000279 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children’s adjustment: A 7-year longitudianl study. Child Development, 68, 908923. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb01970.x Google Scholar
Raby, K. L., Roisman, G. I., Fraley, R. C., & Simpson, J. A. (2015). The enduring predictive significance of early maternal sensitivity: Social and academic competence through age 32 years. Child Development, 86, 695708. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12325 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randall, J., Cunningham, P. B., & Henggeler, S. W. (2018). The development and transportability of multisystemic therapy-substance abuse: A treatment for adolescents with substance use disorders. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 27, 5966. https://doi.org/10.1080/1067828X.2017.1411301 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raudino, A., Woodward, L. J., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2012). Childhood conduct problems are associated with increased partnership and parenting difficulties in adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 251263. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9565-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rickel, A. U., & Biasatti, L. L. (1982). Modification of the block child rearing practices report. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 129134.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rusby, J. C., Light, J. M., Crowley, R., & Westling, E. (2018). Influence of parent-youth relationship, parental monitoring, and parent substance use on adolescent substance use onset. Journal of Family Psychology, 32, 310320. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000350 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (1988). Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis. ASA 1988 proceedings of the business and economic statistics section (pp. 308–313). American Statistical Association.Google Scholar
Schulte, M. T., & Hser, Y.-I. (2013). Substance use and associated health conditions throughout the lifespan. Public Health Reviews, 35, 3. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03391702 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, D. S. (2013). Future directions for research on the development and prevention of early conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 42, 418428. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.777918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sitnick, S. L., Shaw, D. S., & Hyde, L. W. (2014). Precursors of adolescent substance use from early childhood and early adolescence: Testing a developmental cascade model. Development and Psychopathology, 26, 125140. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579413000539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G. (2011). Adolescents’ social reasoning and relationships with parents: Conflicts and coordinations within and across domains. In E. Amsel & J. Smetana (Eds.), Adolescent vulnerabilities and opportunities: Constructivist and developmental perspectives (vol. 38, pp. 139158). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Continuities in antisocial behavior and parenting across three generations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 230247. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00216.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smokowski, P. R., Bacallao, M. L., Cotter, K. L., & Evans, C. B. (2015). The effects of positive and negative parenting practices on adolescent mental health outcomes in a multicultural sample of rural youth. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 46, 333345. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-014-0474-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2001). We know some things: Parent–adolescent relationships in retrospect and prospect. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 11, 119. https://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.00001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Children and parenting (pp. 103133). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Tharp, A. T., & Noonan, R. K. (2012). Associations between three characteristics of parent–youth relationships, youth substance use, and dating attitudes. Health Promotion Practice, 13, 515523. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524839910386220 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornberry, T. P. (2005). Explaining multiple patterns of offending across the life course and across generations. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 602, 156195. https://doi.org/10.1177/00027162052806 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trucco, E. M. (2020). A review of psychosocial factors linked to adolescent substance use. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 196, 172969. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2020.172969 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trucco, E. M., & Hartmann, S. A. (2021). Understanding the etiology of adolescent substance use through developmental perspectives. Child Development Perspectives, 15, 257264. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12426 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trucco, E. M., Hicks, B. M., Villafuerte, S., Nigg, J. T., Burmeister, M., & Zucker, R. A. (2016). Temperament and externalizing behavior as mediators of genetic risk on adolescent substance use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 565. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000143 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Juffer, F., Stolk, M. N., Koot, H. M., & Alink, L. R. (2006). Attachment-based intervention for enhancing sensitive discipline in mothers of 1-to 3-year-old children at risk for externalizing behavior problems: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 994. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.994 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2000). Influence of deviant friends on delinquency: Searching for moderator variables. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 313325. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005188108461 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (2018). The Incredible years parents, teachers, and children training series: A multifaceted treatment approach for young children with conduct problems. In Weisz, J. R. & Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents (pp. 122141). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Murry, V. M., & Brody, G. H. (2003). Family communication and religiosity related to substance use and sexual behavior in early adolescence: A test for pathways through self-control and prototype perceptions. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17, 312. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.17.4.312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfradt, U., Hempel, S., & Miles, J. N. (2003). Perceived parenting styles, depersonalisation, anxiety and coping behaviour in adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 521532. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00092-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zucker, R., Fitzgerald, H., & Noll, R. (1990). Drinking and drug history version 4. Michigan State University.Google Scholar
Zucker, R., & Noll, R. (1980). The antisocial behavior checklist. Michigan State University.Google Scholar
Zucker, R. A., & Barron, F. H. (1973). Parental behaviors associated with problem drinking and antisocial behavior among adolescent males. In M. E. Chafetz (Ed.), Proceedings of the first annual alcoholism conference of the national institute on alcohol abuse and alcoholism. Research on alcoholism: I. Clinical problems and special populations (pp. 276–296). National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Zucker, R. A., Donovan, J. E., Masten, A. S., Mattson, M. E., & Moss, H. B. (2008). Early developmental processes and the continuity of risk for underage drinking and problem drinking. Pediatrics, 121, S252S272. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2243B CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zucker, R. A., & Fillmore, K. M. (1968). Motivational factors and problem drinking among adolescents. Paper presented at the 28th International Congress on Alcohol and Alcoholism, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Zucker, R. A., Fitzgerald, H. E., Refior, S. K., Puttler, L. I., Pallas, D. M., Ellis, D. A. (2000). The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholics: Description of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy. In Fitzgerald, H. E., Lester, B. M., & Zuckerman, B. S. (Eds). Children of addiction: Research, health and policy issues (pp. 174222). Garland Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive and correlation

Figure 2

Table 3. Random intercept cross-lagged panel model results

Figure 3

Figure 1. Bidirectional associations of positive parenting, negative parenting, parent antisocial behavior, and child conduct problems in the prediction of substance use. Figure shows significant associations. Within-time correlations were estimated but not shown in the figure for parsimony. Between-person effects are also not presented in figure (see Table 3). *p< .05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. Anti = parent antisocial behavior; Neg = negative parenting; Pos = positive parenting; CP = conduct problems.

Supplementary material: File

Hare et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Hare et al. supplementary material(File)
File 16.4 KB