Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T21:04:08.009Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parent conflict predicts infants' vagal regulation in social interaction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2010

Ginger A. Moore*
Affiliation:
Pennsylvania State University
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ginger A. Moore, Pennsylvania State University, 303 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

Parent conflict during infancy may affect rapidly developing physiological regulation. To examine the association between parent conflict and infants' vagal tone functioning, mothers (N = 48) reported levels of parent conflict and their 6-month-old male and female infants' respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) was measured in the still-face paradigm. Higher parent conflict was related to lower RSA at baseline and each episode of the still-face paradigm. Infants in relatively higher conflict families showed attenuated RSA withdrawal in response to mothers' disengagement and attenuated RSA activation when interacting with mothers. Findings suggest atypical RSA regulation and reliance on self-regulation for infants in families with moderate levels of parent conflict. Implications for later development and future research are discussed.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bar-Haim, Y., Marshall, P. J., & Fox, N. A. (2000). Developmental changes in heart period and high-frequency heart period variability from 4 months to 4 years of age. Developmental Psychobiology, 37, 4456.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bazhenova, O. V., Plonskaia, O., & Porges, S. W. (2001). Vagal reactivity and affective adjustment in infants during interaction challenges. Child Development, 72, 13141326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007). Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74, 174184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., & Hseih, K. (1998). Patterns of marital change during the early childhood years: Parent personality, coparenting, and division-of-labor correlates. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, 511528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Lang, M., & Rovine, M. (1985). Stability and change in marriage across the transition to parenthood: A second study. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 47, 855865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1990). Patterns of marital change across the transition to parenthood: Pregnancy to three years postpartum. Journal of Marriage & the Family, 52, 519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Integrative physiology: Homeostasis, allostasis, and the orchestration of systemic physiology. In Cacioppo, J., Tassinary, L., & Berntson, G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 433452). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braiker, H., & Kelly, H. (1979). Conflict in the development of close relationship. In Burgess, R. & Huston, T. (Eds.), Social exchange and developing relationships. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D. (1997). Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and behavioral regulation in young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 31, 125135.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., & Meyers, T. (1995). Depression in first-time mothers: Mother–infant interaction and depression chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, M. J., Paley, B., & Harter, K. (2001). Interparental conflict and parent–child relationships. In Grych, J. H. & Fincham, F. D. (Eds.), Interparental conflict and child development: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 249272). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1994). Children and marital conflict: The impact of family dispute and resolution. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2006). Interpersonal discord, family process, and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 86128). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psycholologcial Review, 96, 84100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienstbier, R. A., & Zillig, L. M. (2002). Toughness. In Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 515527). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A., & Walsh, R. O. (1998). Conflict and depression predict maternal sensitivity to infant cries. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 505517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donzella, B., Gunnar, M. R., Krueger, W. K., & Alwin, J. (2000). Cortisol and vagal tone responses to competitive challenge in preschoolers: Associations with temperament. Developmental Psychobiology, 37, 209220.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El-Sheikh, M., Harger, J., & Whitson, S. M. (2001). Exposure to interparental conflict and children's adjustment and physical health: The moderating role of vagal tone. Child Development, 72, 16171636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El-Sheikh, M., & Whitson, S. A. (2006). Longitudinal relations between marital conflict and child adjustment: Vagal regulation as a protective factor. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 3039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantuzzo, J., Boruch, R., Beriama, A., & Atkins, M. (1997). Domestic violence and children: Prevalence and risk in five major U.S. cities. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 116122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother–infant affect synchrony as an antecedent of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Pickens, J., Fox, N. A., & Nawrocki, T. (1995). Vagal tone in infants of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 227231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., Schmidt, L. A., Henderson, H. A., & Marshall, P. J. (2007). Developmental psychophysiology: Conceptual and methodological issues. In Cacioppo, J., Tassinary, L., & Berntson, G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (pp. 453481). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1989). The effects of marital discord on young children's peer interaction and health. Developmental Psychology, 25, 273381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children's internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940950.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Mangelsdorf, S., Larson, M., & Hertsgaard, L. (1989). Attachment, temperament, and adrenocortical activity in infancy: A study of psychoendocrine regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25,355363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huffman, L. C., Bryan, Y. E., Del Carmen, R., Pedersen, F. A., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., & Porges, S. W. (1998). Infant temperament and cardiac vagal tone: Assessments at twelve weeks of age. Child Development, 69, 624635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, L. F. (2001). Physiological processes and mediators of the impact of marital conflict on children. In Grych, J. H. & Fincham, F. D. (Eds.), Interparental conflict and child development: Theory, research, and applications (pp. 188212). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children's internalizing and externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 940950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1995). Vagal tone protects children from marital conflict. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 8392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Buffering children from marital conflict and dissolution. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 157171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, G. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Infants' vagal regulation in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother–infant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 40, 10681080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, G. A., Hill-Soderlund, A. L., Propper, C. B., Calkins, S. D., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Cox, M. J. (2009). Mother–infant vagal regulation in the face-to-face still-face paradigm is moderated by maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 80, 209223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 894906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (2001). The long-term psychobiological consequence of infant emotions: Prescriptions for the twenty-first century. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 132173.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollack, S. D. (2005). Early adversity and mechanisms of plasticity: Integrating affective neuroscience with developmental approaches to psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 735752.Google Scholar
Porges, S. W. (1985). Method and apparatus for evaluating rhythmic oscillations in a periodic physiological response system. US Patent 4,510,944.Google Scholar
Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74, 116143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A., Portales, A. L., & Suess, P. E. (1994). Cardiac vagal tone: Stability and relation to difficulties in infants and 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, C. L., Wouden-Miller, M., Silva, S., & Porter, A. E. (2003). Marital harmony and conflict: Links to infants' emotional regulation and cardiac vagal tone. Infancy, 4, 297307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 717731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Propper, C., & Moore, G. A. (2006). The influence of parenting on infant emotionality: A multi-level psychobiological perspective. Developmental Review, 26, 427460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Propper, C.B., Moore, G. A., Mills-Koonce, W. R., Halpern, C., Hill, A., Calkins, S. D., et al. (2008). Gene–environment contributions to the development of vagal functioning: An examination of DRD2 and maternal sensitivity. Child Development, 78, 13781395.Google Scholar
Quigley, K. S., & Stifter, C. A. (2006). A comparative validation of sympathetic reactivity in children and adults. Psychophysiology, 43, 357365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salomon, K., Matthews, K. A., & Allen, M. T. (2000). Patterns of sympathetic and parasympathetic reactivity in a sample of children and adolescents. Psychophysiology, 37, 842849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2000). The self-organization of the right brain and the neurobiology of emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. & Granic, I. (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization (pp. 155185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, D. J. (2001). Toward an interpersonal neurobiology of the developing mind: Attachment relationships, “mindsight,” and neural integration. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 6794.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Corey, J. M. (2001). Vagal regulation and observed social behavior in infancy. Social Development, 10, 189201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Fox, N. (1991). Infant reactivity: Physiological correlates of infant temperament: Stability of behavior and autonomic patterning from 5 to 18 months. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 379391.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Jain, A. (1996). Psycho physiological correlates of infant temperament: Stability of behavior and autonomic patterning from 5 to 18 months. Developmental Psychobiology, 29, 379391.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suess, P. E., Porges, S. W., & Plude, D. J. (1994). Cardiac vagal tone and sustained attention in school-age children. Psychophysiology, 31, 1722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tronick, E. Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, B. (1978). The infants' response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinberg, K. M., & Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infants' affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face. Child Development, 67, 905914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelenko, M., Kraemer, H., Huffman, L., Gschwendt, M., Pageler, N., & Steiner, H. (2005). Heart rate correlates of attachment status in young mothers and their infants. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 470476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed