Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-29T16:26:13.776Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mothers with zero Beck depression scores act more “depressed” with their infants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Tiffany Field*
Affiliation:
Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School
Connie Morrow
Affiliation:
Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School
Brian Healy
Affiliation:
Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School
Tamar Foster
Affiliation:
Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School
Diane Adlestein
Affiliation:
Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School
Sheri Goldstein
Affiliation:
Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Tiffany Field, Mailman Center for Child Development, University of Miami Medical School, P.O. Box 016820, Miami, Florida 33101.

Abstract

Mothers who scored zero on the Beck Depression Inventory (N = 25) were compared to “depressed” mothers (high scores on the Beck) (N = 39) and nondepressed mothers (N = 98) during face-to-face interactions with their 5-month-old infants. The interaction videotapes were rated on the Interaction Rating Scales and were coded second-by-second for attentive/affective behavior states. The zero Beck mothers and their infants received lower ratings and were in less positive behavior states (alone or together) than the high scoring Beck “depressed” mother/infant dyads and even more frequently than the nondepressed mother/infant dyads. The lower activity levels, lesser expressivity, and less frequent vocalizing were suggestive of “depressed” behavior in both the mothers and their infants. In addition, the infants of the zero Beck mothers had lower vagal tone and lower growth percentiles (weight, length, and head circumference) than the infants of nondepressed mothers, although they did not differ from the infants of depressed mothers on these measures. These data suggest that mothers who report no depressive symptoms may present as much, if not greater risk, for their infants than mothers who do report depressive symptoms on the Beck Depression Inventory.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bartko, J., & Carpenter, W. (1976). On the methods and theory of reliability. The Journal of Nervous Mental Disease, 163, 307317.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. E., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571.Google Scholar
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. In Belsky, J. & Nezworski, T. (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300323). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: Nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 213220.Google Scholar
Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Tronick, E. Z., Connell, D., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (1986). Face-to-face interactions of depressed mothers and their infants. In Tronick, E. Z. & Field, T. (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant disturbance (pp. 3145). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Coyne, J. C., & Gottlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression: A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 472505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T. (1980). Interactions of preterm and term infants with their lower- and middle-class teenage and adult mothers. In Field, T., Goldberg, S., Stern, D., & Sostek, A. (Eds.), High-risk infants and children: Adult and peer interactions. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Field, T. (1989). Maternal depression effects on infant interaction and attachment behavior. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: The emergence of a discipline (pp. 139163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 527532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T., & Widmayer, S. (1981). Mother-infant interactions among lower SES Black, Cuban, Puerto Rican and South American immigrants. In Field, T., Sostek, A., Vietze, P., & Leiderman, A. H. (Eds.), Culture and early interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Bendell, D., Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E. A., & Kuhn, C. (1988). Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with non-depressed adults. Child Development, 59, 15691579.Google Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., & Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior state matching in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed mother-infant dyads. Developmental Psychology, 26, 714.Google Scholar
Field, T., Healy, B., & Leblanc, W. (1989). Matching and synchrony of behavior states and heart rate in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus “depressed” dyads. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 357376.Google Scholar
Field, T., Widmayer, S., Adler, S., & DeCubas, M. (1990). Teenage parenting in different cultures, family constellations and caregiving environments: Effects on infant development. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 158174.Google Scholar
Fox, N., & Field, T. (1989). Individual differences in preschool entry behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 527540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. R. (1987). The MMPI. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Guthertz, M., & Field, T. (1989). Lap computer or on-line coding and data analysis for laboratory and field observations. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 305320.Google Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Zoll, D., Connell, D., & Grunebaum, H. E. (1986). The depressed mother and her one-year-old infant: Environment, interaction, attachment and infant development. In Tronick, E. & Field, T. (Eds.), Maternal depression and infant disturbance (pp. 6182). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Myers, J. L. (1972). Fundamentals of experimental design. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Myers, J. K., & Weissman, M. M. (1980). Use of self-report symptom scale to detect depression in a community sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 10811083.Google Scholar
Porges, S. W. (1985). Spontaneous oscillations in heart rate. Potential index of stress. In Animal Stress (pp. 97111). New York: American Psychological Society.Google Scholar
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tronick, E., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1980). Monadic phases: A structural descriptive analysis of infant-mother face-to-face interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 26, 324.Google Scholar