Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:17:42.252Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Meanings in motion and faces: Developmental associations between the processing of intention from geometrical animations and gaze detection accuracy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2006

RUTH CAMPBELL
Affiliation:
University College London
KATE LAWRENCE
Affiliation:
Institute of Child Health, London
WILLIAM MANDY
Affiliation:
Institute of Child Health, London
CHEYTNA MITRA
Affiliation:
University College London
LALITHA JEYAKUMA
Affiliation:
University College London
DAVID SKUSE
Affiliation:
Institute of Child Health, London

Abstract

Aspects of face processing, on the one hand, and theory of mind (ToM) tasks, on the other hand, show specific impairment in autism. We aimed to discover whether a correlation between tasks tapping these abilities was evident in typically developing children at two developmental stages. One hundred fifty-four normal children (6–8 years and 16–18 years) and 13 high-IQ autistic children (11–17 years) were tested on a range of face-processing and IQ tasks, and a ToM test based on the attribution of intentional movement to abstract shapes in a cartoon. By midchildhood, the ability accurately and spontaneously to infer the locus of attention of a face with direct or averted gaze was specifically associated with the ability to describe geometrical animations using mental state terms. Other face-processing and animation descriptions failed to show the association. Autistic adolescents were impaired at both gaze processing and ToM descriptions, using these tests. Mentalizing and gaze perception accuracy are associated in typically developing children and adolescents. The findings are congruent with the possibility that common neural circuitry underlies, at least in part, processing implicated in these tasks. They are also congruent with the possibility that autism may lie at one end of a developmental continuum with respect to these skills, and to the factor(s) underpinning them.The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Alliance for Autism Research in funding this work.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abell, F., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states top animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 120.Google Scholar
Akshoomoff, N., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2002). The neurobiological basis of autism from a developmental perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 613634.Google Scholar
Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 267278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batki, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Connellan, J., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Is there an innate gaze module? Evidence from human neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 223229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that reads minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, 513552.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mind-blindness: An essay on the psychology of autism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 3746.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A., et al. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 18911898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S. J., Boyer, P., Pachot-Clouard, M., Meltzoff, A., Segebarth, C., & Decety, J. (2003). The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 837844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowler, D. M., & Thommen, E. (2000). Attribution of mechanical and social causality to animated displays by children with autism. Autism, 4, 147171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buitelaar, J. K., van Engeland, H., de Kogel, K. H., de Vries, H., & van Hooff, J. A. (1991). Differences in the structure of social behavior of autistic children and non-autistic retarded controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 9951015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., Keane, J., Scott, S. K., Owen, A. M., Christoffels, I., et al. (2002). Reading the mind from eye gaze. Neuropsychologia, 40, 11291138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 18391849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12, 314325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L. (1999). The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 5765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2003). Automatic attention cueing through eye movement in 2-year-old children with autism. Child Development, 74, 11081122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 524530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A. S., & Tantam, D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 10331057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1976). The pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Elgar, K., & Campbell, R. (2001). Face recognition disorders in developmental neuroscience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 707725.Google Scholar
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 99, 96029205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. (2004). Mechanisms of eye gaze perception during infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 13201326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farroni, T., Mansfield, E. M., Lai, C., & Johnson, M. H. (2003). Infants perceiving and acting on the eyes: Tests of an evolutionary hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 199212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York: W. W. Norton.
Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Evolution and facial action in reflex, social motive, and paralanguage. Biological Psychology, 32, 3100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. (2003). What do imaging studies tell us about the neural basis of autism? In Autism: Neural basis and treatment possibilities. Novartis Symposium 251 (pp. 149176). Chichester: Wiley.
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 459473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, N., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Seen gaze-direction modulates fusiform activity and its coupling with other brain areas during face processing. Neuroimage, 13, 11021112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 216222.Google Scholar
Happé, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., et al. (1996). “Theory of mind” in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport, 8, 197201.Google Scholar
Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 5967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 2632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1986). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 671680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1991). Methodological issues for experiments on autistic individual's perception and understanding of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 11351158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horstmann, G. (2003). What do facial expressions convey: Feeling states, behavioral intentions, or action requests? Emotion, 3, 150166.Google Scholar
Hutt, C., & Ounsted, C. (1966). The biological significance of gaze aversion with particular reference to the syndrome of infantile autism. Behavioral Science, 11, 346356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, J., & Langton, S. R. (2003). Configural processing in the perception of eye-gaze direction. Perception, 32, 11811188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, R. M., & Tanaka, J. (2002). Holistic and part-based face recognition in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 4, 114.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 43024431.Google Scholar
Kingstone, A., Tipper, C., Ristic, J., & Ngan, E. (2004). The eyes have it!: An fMRI investigation. Brain and Cognition, 55, 269271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher functioning Autism and Asperger syndrome: The social attribution task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 831846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R, Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 6, 499508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leekam, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Perret, D., Milders, M., & Brown, S. (1997). Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 7795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A., & Weisberg, J. (2003). Neural foundations for understanding social and mechanical concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 575587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 255263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., & Durnham, P. (Eds.). (1994). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 24, 247257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 10811105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. C. (2004). Theory-of-mind development in oral deaf children with cochlear implants or conventional hearing aids. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 10961106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 21882199.Google Scholar
Puce, A., & Perrett, D. (2003). Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 435445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 87124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” Neuroimage, 19, 18351842.Google Scholar
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R. K., Anderson, A. W., Volkmar, F., et al. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, R. T., Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A., Kleinman, J., Van der Gaag, C., Marois, R., et al. (2003). The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: Implications for the pathobiology of autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 415427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senju, A., Yaguchi, K., Tojo, Y., & Hasegawa, T. (2003). Eye contact does not facilitate detection in children with autism. Cognition, 89, B43B51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skuse, D., Warrington, R., Bishop, D., Chowdhury, U., Lau, J., Mandy, W., et al. (2004). The Developmental, Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview (3Di): A novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 4, 548558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swettenham, J., Condie, S., Campbell, R., Milne, E., & Coleman, M. (2003). Does the perception of moving eyes trigger reflexive visual orienting in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 358, 325334.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Needham, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Tantam, D., Monaghan, L., Nicholson, H., & Stirling, J. (1989). Autistic children's ability to interpret faces: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 623630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teunisse, J. P., & de Gelder, B. (2001). Impaired categorical perception of facial expressions in high-functioning adolescents with autism. Neuropsychology Developmental Cognition Section C Child Neuropsychology, 7, 114.Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K. (1984). The recognition memory test. London: NFER-Nelson.
Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wild, B., Erb, M., & Bartels, M. (2001). Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: Quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Research, 102, 109124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yarbus, A. L. (1961). Eye movements during the examination of complicated objects. Biofizika, 6, 5256.Google Scholar