Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T19:31:27.433Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal relationships across emotional distress, perceived emotion regulation, and social connections during early adolescence: A developmental cascades investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 February 2023

Ola Demkowicz*
Affiliation:
Manchester Institute of Education, The University of Manchester, UK
Margarita Panayiotou
Affiliation:
Manchester Institute of Education, The University of Manchester, UK
Pamela Qualter
Affiliation:
Manchester Institute of Education, The University of Manchester, UK
Neil Humphrey
Affiliation:
Manchester Institute of Education, The University of Manchester, UK
*
Corresponding author: Ola Demkowicz, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Early adolescence is a vulnerable period for emotional distress. Both emotion regulation and social connection to peers and family adults are understood to be associated with distress. However, existing longitudinal work has not explored these constructs jointly in a way that estimates their reciprocal relationships over adolescence. We present a three-wave random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model of reciprocal relationships between emotional distress, perceived emotion regulation, and social connections during early adolescence, among 15,864 participants from education settings in disadvantaged areas of England, over three annual waves (at ages 11/12, 12/13, and 13/14 years). Findings showed that emotional distress and perceived emotion regulation share a negative relationship over time, and that higher perceived emotion regulation predicts greater family connection in the initial stages of early adolescence (from age 11–12 to 12–13 years). Findings also indicated that connection to peers is positively associated with family connection, but also positively predicts slightly greater distress in the later stages of early adolescence (from age 12–13 to 13–14 years). Findings indicate a risk of negative spiral between emotional distress and perceived emotion regulation in early adolescence, and that social connection may not necessarily play the role we might expect in reducing distress.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Developmental cascades theory suggests that domains of functioning are developmentally intertwined, with particular functions and behaviors interacting over time to create complex chain reactions (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). A growing body of research has explored such relationships over time, although recent investigation has highlighted fundamental flaws with the modelling approach traditionally used in this area (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper, Grasman, Sciences and Sciences2015). In the current study, we explore mental health development in the critical early adolescent period, examining developmental cascades in emotional distress, perceived emotion regulation, and social connections, using a large sample and robust analytic methods to explore within-person cascades over time. Existing longitudinal work has not yet explored these constructs jointly within a developmental cascades framework, and their influence on one another over time is unclear.

Adolescence is a developmental period conceptualized by the World Health Organization (2021) as occurring between ages 10 and 19 years, and by others as up to age 24 years (Sawyer et al., Reference Sawyer, Azzopardi, Wickremarathne and Patton2018), with discrete early, mid, and late adolescence/young adulthood stages. This period is transformative, and includes important developments: biological changes, including puberty and heightened synaptic pruning (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2019); psychological changes such as increasing capacity for complex thought and heightened emotionality (Arnett, Reference Arnett1999); and, social shifts including a widening social world, emphasizing peer connection (Umberson et al., Reference Umberson, Crosnoe and Reczek2010), with increasing concerns about one’s standing in the social group alongside a need for intimacy with a close friend (Qualter et al., Reference Qualter, Vanhalst, Harris, Lodder, Bangee, van Roekel, Verhagen and Maes2015). Theory and research have demonstrated that adolescence is also a critical and vulnerable period for the development of mental health. Evidence indicates that 75% of lifetime case of mental health disorders begin by age 24 (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005), with a peak age-of-onset of 14.5 years (Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo, Il Shin, Kirkbride, Jones, Kim, Kim, Carvalho, Seeman, Correll and Fusar-Poli2021).

Early adolescence, between 10 and 14 years, appears particularly pivotal in depressive and anxious symptomatology and disorder trajectories (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Copeland and Angold2011; Jones, Reference Jones2013). Depressive and anxious symptoms are distinct but closely related, often presenting comorbidly in adolescence (Balázs et al., Reference Balázs, Miklõsi, Keresztény, Hoven, Carli, Wasserman, Apter, Bobes, Brunner, Cosman, Cotter, Haring, Iosue, Kaess, Kahn, Keeley, Marusic, Postuvan, Resch and Wasserman2013). They can be conceptualized as a shared construct of emotional distress Footnote 1 wherein individuals experience symptoms such as low mood and feelings of worry. Research has indicated that adolescent emotional distress poses a short- and long-term risk for other outcomes, including negative health behaviors/disorders (e.g., eating disorders; Lewinsohn et al., Reference Lewinsohn, Striegel-Moore and Seeley2000), deliberate self-harm (Moran et al., Reference Moran, Coffey, Romaniuk, Olsson, Borschmann, Carlin and Patton2011), suicidal ideation and behaviors (Kandel et al., Reference Kandel, Raveis and Davies1991; Murphy, Reference Murphy2014), and broader mental health difficulties (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005; Roza et al., Reference Roza, Hofstra, Van Der Ende and Verhulst2003). Thus, there is a need to explore etiological pathways contributing to emotional distress in early adolescence and, in turn, examine how distress influences wider adolescent life and development.

Emotion regulation and adolescent psychopathology

Emotion regulation may be an important transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology, including emotional distress (Bradley, Reference Bradley2000; Southam-Gerow & Kendall, Reference Southam-Gerow and Kendall2002). Emotion regulation is not consistently defined, but refers broadly to efforts to manage one’s emotions and emotion expression (Castro et al., Reference Castro, Cheng, Halberstadt and Grühn2016; Gross & Muñoz, Reference Gross and Muñoz1995). This involves varied knowledge and skills, including (a) knowing that one’s emotions can be controlled and (b) awareness of possible strategies and associated advantages/limitations (Castro et al., Reference Castro, Cheng, Halberstadt and Grühn2016), such as suppressing emotions, reappraising situations, or pre-emptively modifying one’s experiences and environment to reduce or increase the probability of particular feelings (Castro et al., Reference Castro, Cheng, Halberstadt and Grühn2016; Gross & Muñoz, Reference Gross and Muñoz1995). Emotion regulation develops throughout childhood and adolescence and then across adulthood, with individual differences in the extent and nature of knowledge, skills, and strategies. Notably, emotion regulation appears to develop relatively linearly over the course of adolescence as a whole, and across the period researchers have observed a complex pattern of both growth and decline in specific aspects of regulation, such as in use of specific strategies (even after accounting for heightened emotionality; Zimmermann & Iwanski, Reference Zimmermann and Iwanski2014). This complex process of emotion regulation development may not effectively match the rapid changes adolescents experience more widely, including heightened emotionality. That is, adolescents’ emotional processes and experiences may exceed the capacity of still-developing and potentially fluctuating regulatory systems, potentially then relating to distress and other difficulties among those for whom this imbalance is more pronounced (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Jones, Levita, Libby, Pattwell, Ruberry, Soliman and Somerville2010; Dahl, Reference Dahl2001; Hollenstein & Lougheed, Reference Hollenstein and Lougheed2013).

There is a distinction between a) specific regulatory strategy usage, and b) an individual’s perception of their regulatory skills as effective or sufficient. Emotion regulation is an individualized process contextually embedded in experience, and individuals vary in their perception of what is appropriate or efficient (Hollenstein & Lougheed, Reference Hollenstein and Lougheed2013; Thompson, Reference Thompson2011). That is, individuals who may externally be judged to have low or high emotion regulation on a skills-based assessment may not necessarily perceive themselves to perform in that way (Qualter et al., Reference Qualter, Davis, Keefer, Parker, Saklofske, Wigelsworth, Simmons and Stough2017). These distinct emotion regulation constructs need to be investigated separately to understand their distinct influence upon, and possible reciprocal relationships with, emotional distress. In the current study, we focus upon individuals’ perception of their emotion regulation, in order to understand how this relates to experiences of emotional distress during early adolescence.

Various studies, spanning self-report, behavioral, and neurological designs, have investigated the relationship between emotion regulation components and emotional distress. There is evidence of cross-sectional associations between emotion regulation and depressive and/or anxious symptoms in adolescence, often focusing upon specific knowledge and strategies (Garnefski et al., Reference Garnefski, Rieffe, Jellesma, Terwogt and Kraaij2007; Schäfer et al., Reference Schäfer, Naumann, Holmes, Tuschen-Caffier and Samson2017; Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Abramson and Alloy2016; Silk et al., Reference Silk, Steinberg and Morris2003; Tortella-Feliu et al., Reference Tortella-Feliu, Balle and Sesé Albert2010). A small number of longitudinal studies have reported that emotion regulation predicts emotional distress over time, including both self-reported emotion dysregulation (poor emotion understanding, dysregulated emotion expression, and rumination; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011) and self-reported difficulties (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Arch, Landy and Hankin2018). Although this longitudinal work has offered some insight, further investigation is needed. Specifically, we need exploration that capitalizes on recent advances in statistical analytic techniques that more accurately measure change over developmental periods, as prior work has not effectively isolated change at the within-person level (as discussed later in this section). This is critical to inform adolescent prevention efforts, which frequently target regulatory knowledge and skills (Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019). In the current study we build on this gap, exploring reciprocal relationships over time between perceived emotion regulation and emotional distress using robust modelling and data from a very large sample of adolescents.

Social connections and emotional processes in adolescence

Emotional distress and emotion regulation happen within the context of social relationships. Social interaction and relationships encompass a range of components; we focus here on connection, or perceived interpersonal closeness with others, which itself is a multifaceted construct, encompassing a range of structures (i.e., type of relationship), functions and behaviors (e.g., support), and quality considerations in terms of positive and negative aspects (Berkman et al., Reference Berkman, Glass, Brissette and Seeman2000; Holt-Lunstad et al., Reference Holt-Lunstad, Robles and Sbarra2017). Emotion regulation is shaped and developed through social experience with others. Parents/carers play a key role through modelling, parenting practices, providing a secure attachment base, and their setting of the wider family climate (Bariola et al., Reference Bariola, Gullone and Hughes2011; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007). In turn, emotion regulation appears critical to developing and maintaining social relationships. Such skills facilitate optimistic expectations about social interactions, affect the tone of interactions, and support wider skills often favored socially (e.g., better decision-making in stressful situations), all of which can facilitate acceptance by, and connection with, others (Blair et al., Reference Blair, Gangle, Perry, O’Brien, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2016; Lopes et al., Reference Lopes, Salovey, Côté and Beers2005). Evidence suggests perceived connection to others in childhood and adolescence is also associated with lower rates of emotional distress and wider internalizing difficulties (Bond et al., Reference Bond, Butler, Thomas, Carlin, Glover, Bowes and Patton2007; Ewell Foster et al., Reference Ewell Foster, Horwitz, Thomas, Opperman, Gipson, Burnside, Stone and King2017; Malaquias et al., Reference Malaquias, Crespo and Francisco2015), while feelings of loneliness predict greater distress (Hall-Lande et al., Reference Hall-Lande, Eisenberg, Christenson and Neumark-Sztainer2007; Harris et al., Reference Harris, Qualter and Robinson2013; Qualter et al., Reference Qualter, Brown, Munn and Rotenberg2010, Reference Qualter, Brown, Rotenberg, Vanhalst, Harris, Goossens, Bangee and Munn2013). Likewise, there is indication that emotional distress may have the capacity to negatively impact upon aspects of adolescents’ social engagement and perceptions; for instance, a recent longitudinal study indicated that distress predicts lowered social self-efficacy over time (Kristensen et al., Reference Kristensen, Danielsen, Jeno, Larsen and Urke2021). Given evidence of such links between social connections and emotion regulation and distress, respectively, it is crucial to explore interplay across these constructs within a singular model and to examine their longitudinal relationships (Ladd, Reference Ladd, Parke and Ladd2017).

Here, we explore respective connections to school peers and family adults, given that, in early adolescence, individuals place increasing emphasis on peer relationships, but retain close relationships with parents/carers (Umberson et al., Reference Umberson, Crosnoe and Reczek2010). We would expect to see concurrent relationships between family and peer relationships within timepoints, given that positive relationships with family adults provide a foundation for the development of social skills and thus for peer relationships (Brown & Bakken, Reference Brown and Bakken2011; Pallini et al., Reference Pallini, Baiocco, Schneider, Madigan and Atkinson2014). Indeed, there may also be reciprocal relationships over time as social behaviors and aspects of both family and peer relationships evolve over adolescence (Brown & Bakken, Reference Brown and Bakken2011)

Developmental cascades

Theory and research suggest a complex interplay between emotional distress, emotion regulation, and social connections during the critical early adolescent period. This reflects the concept of developmental cascades, “the cumulative consequences for development of the many interactions and transactions occurring in developing systems that result in spreading effects across different levels, among domains at the same level, and across different systems or generations” (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010, p. 491). This theory suggests that functioning within and across domains, levels, and systems is developmentally intertwined, and particular functions and behaviors ‘spread’ over time into other functions and behaviors through complex chain reactions. Greater competence in one aspect of development may provide a scaffold for functioning within this and other aspects at a later time; conversely, difficulties could lead to negative consequences within the same and other functions.

There has been considerable interest in empirically exploring developmental cascades in relation to aspects of adolescent psychopathology, including emotional distress. However, very few studies have investigated distress in relation to social connections, and we have been unable to identify developmental cascades studies exploring how emotion regulation and distress interact over time in adolescence, though related work provides a useful empirical foundation for our study. For example, Murray et al., (Reference Murray, Obsuth, Speyer, Murray, McKenzie, Eisner and Ribeaud2021) found self-reported peer relationships prospectively predicted lower levels of internalizing difficulties (a wider construct that includes emotional distress) across a two-year period in adolescence, although difficulties did not predict peer relationships. A recent study by Antony et al., (Reference Antony, Pihlajamäki, Speyer and Murray2022) showed that emotion dysregulation predicted later internalizing problems in childhood, including mediating the relationship between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms and internalizing problems. In addition, there is some evidence of some adjacent social constructs in relation to distress, such as that social competence and peer victimization link to internalizing difficulties in childhood and early adolescence (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Hahn and Haynes2010; Vaillancourt et al., Reference Vaillancourt, Brittain, McDougall and Duku2013). However, there has been little attention given to cascading effects with social connections specifically. There is also little work in this area that has explored cascade effects between distress and aspects of emotion regulation or social connections relate over time, although available research does offer some insight into mechanisms linking emotion regulation and peer-level connection. For instance, Blair et al. (Reference Blair, Perry, O’Brien, Calkins, Keane and Shanahan2015) identified that, among children, emotion regulation had cascading effects into greater social skills and, in turn, greater relationship quality and peer acceptance.

Despite emotion regulation being considered a central factor in the development of psychopathology, and the emphasis on social connection as a critical protective factor, to our knowledge no study has examined reciprocal relationships between these constructs in relation to emotional distress, either generally or within early adolescence. This is not to say there has been no longitudinal evidence whatsoever. As noted earlier, for instance, Schneider et al. (Reference Schneider, Arch, Landy and Hankin2018) found that emotion regulation prospectively predicted anxious symptoms among children and adolescents; however, this and other studies cited above have not been undertaken within a cascades model to offer insights into complex, interwoven longitudinal relationships.

Finally, and most critically, recent investigation highlighted a fundamental flaw in the predominant analytic approach in developmental cascades studies (including some, though not all, of the studies cited above). Such research traditionally relied upon traditional cross-lagged panel modelling (CLPM) to model relationships between constructs over time, controlling for temporal stability in constructs (‘autoregressives’) and relationships between constructs within time (‘concurrent relationships’), to examine their interplay over time (‘cross-lagged pathways’). However, Hamaker et al. (Reference Hamaker, Kuiper, Grasman, Sciences and Sciences2015) indicated that CLPM pathways capture stability and changes in group means, assuming that people varied over time around the same mean, thus failing to account for stable individual differences present within most psychological constructs. Hence, traditional CLPM pathways do not show change across functioning within individuals (the primary level of interest).

Hamaker et al. (Reference Hamaker, Kuiper, Grasman, Sciences and Sciences2015) proposed an alternative: the random intercepts cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM). In RI-CLPM, a variable is estimated to separate participants’ time-invariant deviation from the grand mean, and a second variable captures the difference between a participant’s actual measurement and expected measurement. Pathways are modelled using this second latent variable, capturing within-person stability and change within and across constructs. Thus, in the RI-CLPM, it is possible to investigate cascading effects across individual-level functioning. Hamaker et al.'s (2015) simulations, alongside recent empirical investigations (e.g., Burns et al., Reference Burns, Crisp and Burns2020) showed that the traditional CLPM can produce spurious results when compared to RI-CLPM, including the wrong construct identified as the causally dominant in cascade effects, misidentification of statistical significance and of the sign (+/−) of relationships, and differing effect sizes. Thus, much of the existing developmental cascades evidence (including across emotional distress, emotion regulation, and social connections) is likely confounded by between-person relationships and fails to adequately identify cascades within individuals. Here, we contribute to efforts to narrow that gap by exploring these relationships using RI-CLPM.

The current study

In the current study, we explore reciprocal relationships between self-reported emotional distress, perceived emotion regulation, and the family and peer domains of social connection across early adolescence. Figure 1 presents a conceptual diagram showing the modelled cross-lagged relationships between variables (note that this figure does not show autoregressive or concurrent relationships for ease, as the key focus in our hypotheses is upon cross-lagged relationships; in the following section a detailed statistical diagram is shown).

Figure 1. Conceptual model for developmental cascades showing cross-lagged pathways only. Model shows cross-lagged pathways between emotional distress, emotion regulation, and family and peer connection. For simplicity we have not shown autoregressive pathways over time or concurrent relationships, or detailed statistical features (see Figure 2 for a detailed statistical model).

Specifically, we have four hypotheses regarding cross-lagged relationships between these constructs:

H1: Emotional distress will negatively predict later perceived emotion regulation and social connections;

H2: Perceived emotion regulation will negatively predict later emotional distress and positively predict social connections;

H3: Social connections will negatively predict later emotional distress and positively predict perceived emotion regulation; and

H4: Peer and family social connections will positively predict one another over time.

We use RI-CLPM to investigate within-person change over time, which can offer important insights into how these aspects of functioning affect one another across early adolescence and inform both prevention and treatment in addressing emergent distress among adolescents. These specific domains, of course, do not occur uniformly among individuals and are influenced by a range of individual characteristics and contextual features; we control for a range of such factors in our analysis. For instance, evidence shows greater risk of emotional distress among particular groups, such as girls (Kessler, Reference Kessler2003; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2017; Zahn-Waxler et al., Reference Zahn-Waxler, Crick, Shirtcliff, Woods, Cicchetti and Cohen2006); those who have experienced low family income and poverty during childhood (Palacios-Barrios & Hanson, Reference Palacios-Barrios and Hanson2019; Wadsworth et al., Reference Wadsworth, Evans, Grant, Carter, Duffy and Cicchetti2016); and (in the current generation) White young people (Patalay & Fitzsimons, Reference Patalay and Fitzsimons2021; Sadler et al., Reference Sadler, Vizard, Ford, Marcheselli, Pearce, Mandalia, David, Brodie, Forbes, Goodman, Goodman and McManus2018; Terhaag et al., Reference Terhaag, Fitzsimons, Daraganova and Patalay2021). Similarly, there are factors commonly found to be associated with components of functioning including emotion regulation and social connections, such as low academic attainment (Graziano et al., Reference Graziano, Reavis, Keane and Calkins2007; Kwon et al., Reference Kwon, Kupzyk and Benton2018). The influence of these factors can occur in various ways that are closely intertwined over time; as an example, for individuals with particular special educational needs (SEN), evidence suggests that among adolescents with a history of childhood language difficulties there is a stronger relationship between poor emotion regulation and emotional problems, as well as peer difficulties (Forrest et al., Reference Forrest, Gibson, Halligan and St Clair2020). Thus, we control for various factors at the variable level in exploring these relationships, to account for their potential influence.

Method

Data are from the HeadStart longitudinal cohort study (see National Lottery Community Fund, n.d.) HeadStart is a 6-year program set up by The National Lottery Community Fund that explores and tests ways to improve the mental health and wellbeing of young people aged 10-16 years across six disadvantaged areas of England. A variety of low intensity interventions have been implemented in HeadStart. These can be summarized through five discrete categories of whole-school wellbeing promotion, staff training, targeted wellbeing and/or mental health interventions, and parent/carer interventions (see Bear et al., Reference Bear, Stock and Garland2020). Summative evidence to date, which has been limited to targeted interventions, indicates modest impact that varies as a function of implementation quality (see for example Gill et al., Reference Gill, Panayiotou, Demkowicz and Humphrey2019; Humphrey & Panayiotou, Reference Humphrey and Panayiotou2020).

As part of the evaluation of HeadStart, a sample of adolescents from these areas have been surveyed annually since 2017, beginning in Year 7 (age 11–12 years). Here, we used data across the first three waves of data generation (2017 to 2019). We note that Authors Demkowicz, Panayiotou, and Humphrey are researchers on the HeadStart evaluation and have been involved in various design, data generation, and analysis decisions for this; this is the first cross-lagged panel model we have fitted to this data.

Participants

Our sample comprised 15,864 participants. Their characteristics are presented in Table 1, alongside national norms. Because data were drawn from a project focused on disadvantaged areas of England, sampling was not undertaken to be representative against national norms and our sample characteristics reflect a range of sociodemographic contexts (for this reason, we have not applied weights in the current sample, because the intention was not to create a representative dataset and sampling was not random, and indeed weights do not currently exist for this dataset). However, as can be seen in Table 1, the sample generally reflects national norms, although we note a slightly higher prevalence of SEN and slightly lower prevalence of free school meal eligibility in our sample.

Table 1. Sample characteristics and national norms

Note. All characteristics were recorded in Spring 2017 at the cohort baseline, gathered from the National Pupil Database (NPD). * Note that the NPD uses gender to refer to ‘female’ and ‘male’. We use ‘girls’ and ‘boys’ here in alignment with gender (rather than sex) language, and use this data as an imperfect proxy for gender information.

a Department for Education and Office for National Statistics (2017). Schools, pupils, and their characteristics: January 2017.

b Department for Education and Office for National Statistics (2017). Special educational needs in England: January 2017.

Measures

Participants completed a range of self-report measures annually for the evaluation of HeadStart, including measures for emotional distress, perceived emotion regulation, and family adult and peer connection. The survey was administered using a secure online system in teacher-facilitated sessions in participating schools at a time convenient to the school during the March to July periods of 2017 (Timepoint 1; T1), 2018 (Timepoint 2; T2), and 2019 (Timepoint 3; T3). Data generation processes were informed through piloting and wider guidance (see Demkowicz et al., Reference Demkowicz, Ashworth, Mansfield, Stapley, Miles, Hayes, Burrell, Moore and Deighton2020). Demographic data were obtained from the National Pupil Database (NPD) in 2017 and reflect information recorded for participants as of 31st March 2017.

Emotional distress. This was assessed using the ‘emotional symptoms’ subscale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Meltzer and Bailey1998). The subscale includes five items (e.g., “I am often unhappy”; see Table S1 in Supplementary Materials, Appendix A for all items in the current study) and participants respond on a three-point scale: 0 = “not true”, 1 = “somewhat true”, and 2 = “certainly true”. Scores are summed to total 0–10, where higher scores indicate greater distress. Research from Goodman et al. (Reference Goodman, Meltzer and Bailey1998) indicated that the SDQ can distinguish between community and clinical samples and that the emotional symptoms subscale demonstrates satisfactory internal consistency. In the current sample, we found acceptable internal consistency (T1 α = .72; T2 α = .74; T3 α = .76) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model fit (see Table S2, Appendix B, in Supplementary Materials).

Perceived emotion regulation. This was measured using a revised subscale from the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire – Adolescent Short Form (TEIQue-ASF). The original TEIQue is a measure of self-perceived trait emotional intelligence facets, and the TEIQue-ASF is a shorter, simplified version that can be used for adolescents aged 11+ years (Petrides, Reference Petrides, Parker, Saklofske and Stough2009). Participants respond on a seven-point scale from “disagree” to “agree”; higher summed scores reflecting greater perceived emotion regulation skills. While participants responded to six items from the TEIQue-ASF, following inspection of item content and factor analysis, we reduced these to four items, removing two relating to impulsiveness (see Appendix C in Supplementary Materials for a more detailed overview of this process). We conceptualize this construct more narrowly as ‘perceived emotion regulation’ in line with retained item content (e.g., “I find it hard to control my feelings”; see Table S1 in Appendix A, Supplementary Materials). This revised four-item subscale showed good fit (see Table S2 in Supplementary Materials) and acceptable internal consistency (T1 α = .69; T2 α = .70; T3 α = .71).

Family adult and peer connection. Connection to family and peers was measured using two subscales from the Student Resilience Survey (SRS),Footnote 2 a self-report measure for adolescents aged 11+ years capturing self-perceived exposure to potential protective factors (i.e., factors expected to be associated with resilience; Sun & Stewart, Reference Sun and Stewart2007). We used Lereya et al.'s (Reference Lereya, Humphrey, Patalay, Wolpert, Böhnke, Macdougall and Deighton2016) slightly adapted version, where wording on a small number of items was edited to suit English schoolchildren (e.g., using “join in” rather than “play”). We measured family adult connection using the family support subscale, which includes four items asking respondents to reflect on a relationship with an adult ‘at home’ (e.g., “at home, there is an adult who believes that I will be a success”), and peer connection using the peer support subscale, with thirteen items asking respondents to reflect on relationships with their school-based peers (e.g., “are there students at your school who would choose you on your team at school?”); see Table S1 in Supplementary Materials for all items. Participants respond to items on a five-point scale from 1 for “never” to 5 for “always”, with higher summed scores denoting greater perceived protective factor exposure. The SRS has shown good construct validity and acceptable internal consistency (Lereya et al., Reference Lereya, Humphrey, Patalay, Wolpert, Böhnke, Macdougall and Deighton2016; Sun & Stewart, Reference Sun and Stewart2007). In the current sample, good internal consistency was observed for the family subscale (T1 α = .76; T2 α = .81; T3 α = .84) and peer subscale (T1 α = .93; T2 α = .94; α = .95), and acceptable CFA model fit for both subscales (see Table S2 in Supplementary Materials).

Covariates and demographic information. Demographic variables were collected from the NPD and recorded as up to date as of March 2017, at the start of baseline data generation. We included gender, ethnicity, SEN, low family income, and academic attainment as covariates. A brief justification and overview of measurement approaches for each are outlined below. We note that the dataset also had available information on looked after status, caregiving responsibilities, more detailed special educational needs information, neighborhood deprivation, school absences, and exclusions. However, it is important to be selective in including covariates in RI-CLPM to reduce burden in the model, rather than including all available information, and we deduced the greatest justification for selection of the included covariates outlined below.

Gender. We relied on male/female data as an imperfect proxy for gender, in the absence of a more precise or inclusive measurement approach in available data. This is recorded as a binary ‘gender’ variable by the NPD and we do not have access to data that is inclusive of those outside of that binary.

Ethnic group. We converted ethnic group to a binary variable, grouping a) White and b) UK ethnic minority backgrounds. This has been implemented in past mental health studies that have shown significant difference across this binary, with children and adolescents from UK ethnic minority groups reporting lower emotional difficulties than their White peers (Patalay & Fitzsimons, Reference Patalay and Fitzsimons2021; Sadler et al., Reference Sadler, Vizard, Ford, Marcheselli, Pearce, Mandalia, David, Brodie, Forbes, Goodman, Goodman and McManus2018; Terhaag et al., Reference Terhaag, Fitzsimons, Daraganova and Patalay2021). Additionally, the small number of participants from some groups in our sample (e.g., just 27 participants were identified as Chinese) necessitated treatment of ethnic group as binary rather a more granular variable.

Special educational needs status. ‘Special educational needs’ (SEN) is a term in the English education system capturing a range of additional needs, with common categories such as learning difficulties, speech, language and communication needs, autism, physical disability, and social, emotional, and mental health needs. SEN status was designated for participants identified by the NPD as having SEN.Footnote 3 By early adolescence, individuals are more likely to have been formally identified as having SEN, with a peak in identification around aged ten years (Department for Education & Office for National Statistics, 2021).

Low family income. We identified low family income using free school meal eligibility. Pupils are entitled to receive free school meals in England if their parents have low income or receive income-related support (Department for Education, 2018a). We included those were recorded as having been eligible for free school meals at any point since 2011, even if they were no longer eligible as of March 2017, given the long-term implications of low income and poverty in childhood.

Prior academic attainment. We measured assessment using scores from Key Stage Two Statutory Assessment Tests (SATs), which participants undertook approximately one year prior to baseline survey data generation. The SATs are a set of assessments that pupils undertake in England in the final year of primary education, at aged 11 years. Research has indicated that English and Mathematics SAT scores have good predictive validity for later academic performance, showing strong relationships with assessment scores at ages 14 and 16 years (Education Endowment Foundation, 2013; Strand, Reference Strand2006); thus, SATs performance could be considered to function as a relatively stable proxy in the absence of ongoing attainment data during the timepoints under investigation. We followed guidance from the Department for Education (2016) to create a composite score by deriving the mean of scaled scores, which are converted test scores reflecting standardized expectations, across English reading, English writing (grammar, punctuation, and spelling), and Mathematics scores, double weighting Mathematics.

Ethical considerations

Ethical approval was granted for the HeadStart evaluation by University College London’s ethics committee (reference 8097/003), also covering secondary analysis of data generated in the evaluation, such as the current study. Information sheets were provided to parents/carers and participants. Parents/carers were able to opt their child out of the study and participants were asked to provide assent via a tick box before completing measures; 222 young people, 1.38% of possible participants, were opted out by parents/carers. Measures relating to more sensitive constructs such as emotional distress were presented in the middle of the survey, with more ‘positive’ measures (e.g., social connection) positioned at the beginning and end of the survey (Demkowicz et al., Reference Demkowicz, Ashworth, Mansfield, Stapley, Miles, Hayes, Burrell, Moore and Deighton2020).

Data analysis plan

Main analyses were undertaken in Mplus Version 8.7 and various preliminary analysis in both SPSS Version 25 and Mplus. We reviewed data to explore normality, multicollinearity, and missingness. No major normality violations were identified; an overview is shown in Supplementary Materials (Appendix D, Table S4). Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations are shown in Table 2; these showed statistically significant relationships across all variables, although none exceeded the .70 level that would indicate multicollinearity (Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013).

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations (r)

Note. T1 = Timepoint 1; T2 = Timepoint 2; T3 = Timepoint 3. All bivariate correlations significant at the .001 alpha level. Within-domain correlations across time are shown in bold type.

Missing data across item-level survey data varied from 2.9% to 6.0% at T1, 16.6% to 20.0% at T2, and 26.5% to 28.6% at T3, with 25.0% of participants (n = 3,965) showing missing data across all items at T3. We performed a binary logistic regression to examine whether there were systematic differences between the individuals lost through attrition at the final timepoint (i.e., those with missing data on all T3 items) and those still engaged at T3, including a range of demographic variables as predictors, namely gender, ethnic group, first language, SEN status, free school meal eligibility, and academic attainment.Footnote 4 Results and odd ratio (OR) values indicated that participants had a greater likelihood of being lost to follow-up if they were White (OR = 1.19, p < .01), spoke English as their primary language (OR = 1.25, p < .01), had a history of free school meal eligibility (OR = 1.39, p < .001), and had low academic attainment relative to their peers (OR = 1.43, p < .001). Gender (p = .16) and SEN status (p = .02) was not a significant predictor of missingness using an alpha threshold of .01; see later in this section for decision-making around p-values and sample size). Thus, data were assumed missing at random (wherein missingness is predicted by other available variables) and we used multiple imputation to treat missingness. We opted for imputation over full information maximum likelihood (FIML) because we are using robust weighted least squares (WLSMV) as the estimator in the main analysis (see later in this section) and this relies on pairwise presence and not FIML. To do this, we used the imputation feature in Mplus and generated 100 datasets, in line with guidance (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Olchowski and Gilreath2007), and included ethnicity, language, free school meal eligibility, attainment, and SEN to predict missing values.

Longitudinal measurement invariance. Prior to testing the RI-CLPM model the longitudinal measurement invariance of the variables was explored. Given the sensitivity of the chi-square difference test to large samples, such as the one of the current study, the measurement invariance was explored for both categorical items with WLSMV and continuous with MLR (Sass et al., Reference Sass, Schmitt and Marsh2014). This followed a 3-step (baselines, configural, scalar) and a 4-step (baselines, configural, metric, scalar) procedure for the WLSMV and MLR models, respectively. The fit of the baseline and configural models was judged against typical model fit indices, as described below and only models with acceptable model fit received measurement invariance testing. Metric and scalar models with a CFI difference <.01 (Chen, Reference Chen2007) were considered to indicate full metric and scalar longitudinal measurement invariance, respectively. All latent constructs achieved this (see Supplementary Materials, Appendix E).

RI-CLPM. As outlined in the introduction we used an RI-CLPM approach to modelling data, drawing on guidance from Hamaker et al. (Reference Hamaker, Kuiper, Grasman, Sciences and Sciences2015) and Mulder and Hamaker (Reference Mulder and Hamaker2021); code is shown in Appendix F in Supplementary Materials. Figure 2 shows a simplified statistical diagram for our developmental cascades analysis using this approach. Here, we briefly outline the key features modelled in this RI-CLPM analysis to guide the reader. Between-person differences (shown in the left-hand panel of Figure 2) are isolated through the estimation of latent variables, constrained to equality across timepoints, to partial out each participant’s time-invariant deviation from a given construct’s grand mean, thus capturing stable between-person differences. These between-person latent factors are allowed to covary. Within-person components (shown in Figure 2 in panels spanning the width of the model) are then identified by specifying a latent variable for each measurement (i.e., each individual construct at a single timepoint). Autoregressive effects (paths labelled A in Figure 2) are modelled as the within-person carry-on effects over time, based on the relative increase or decrease of one’s score over time. Concurrent relationships (labelled B in Figure 2) are modelled as the covariance between the within-person component residuals at each timepoint; that is, the relationships between constructs within individuals at each timepoint. Cross-lagged effects (labelled C in Figure 2) show within-person cascading effects across constructs over time, based on a deviation in individuals’ expected score on a given construct showing an association with an earlier score on a second construct. By partialling out between-person differences, constructing within-person variables for each construct, and then controlling for autoregressive effects (Pathways A) and concurrent relationships (Pathways B), the RI-CLPM approach allowed us to isolate of cross-lagged effects (Pathways C) as the primary area of interest and therefore assess whether and how emotional distress, emotion regulation, and peer and family adult social connections predict one another over time.

Figure 2. Statistical model for main RI-CLPM developmental cascades analysis. A = autoregressive effects; B = concurrent relationships; C = within-person cross-lagged effects. Left-hand panel shows between-person differences; panels across the model show within-person components specified as latent variables; grand means used to estimate these shown in dotted lines. Some features not shown here for simplicity, including covariates and the items for emotional distress and emotion regulation.

In specifying the original construct variables upon which to base the RI-CLPM, we found that it was not feasible to specify all four constructs as latent variables given the complexity of the model (i.e., the model could not converge). As such, we opted to specify only emotional distress and emotion regulation as latent variables, given the complexity of these constructs and the close relationship between them and given that both the family and peer connection variables showed good unidimensional and highly uniform factor loadings for all items (see Supplementary Materials Appendix B). Following the full longitudinal measurement invariance of the constructs (see above), we constrained the factor loadings and thresholds of each categorical indicator to equality across timepoints, in line with guidance (Mulder & Hamaker, Reference Mulder and Hamaker2021). We reviewed a measurement model prior to the RI-CLPM, allowing these two latent variables to freely correlate and found this to be acceptable (see Supplementary Materials Appendix G and Table S6).

A WLSMV estimator was utilized because emotional distress and emotion regulation were modelled as a latent variable with categorical indicators (Brown, Reference Brown2015; Li, Reference Li2016). As outlined in our introduction, these relationships do not occur uniformly among individuals; we included covariates capturing individual characteristics and contextual features that evidence suggests could influence the constructs in our panel model or the relationships between them. Namely, we included as covariates: gender (Kessler, Reference Kessler2003; Kuehner, Reference Kuehner2017; Zahn-Waxler et al., Reference Zahn-Waxler, Crick, Shirtcliff, Woods, Cicchetti and Cohen2006), low family income, (Palacios-Barrios & Hanson, Reference Palacios-Barrios and Hanson2019; Wadsworth et al., Reference Wadsworth, Evans, Grant, Carter, Duffy and Cicchetti2016), ethnicity (Patalay & Fitzsimons, Reference Patalay and Fitzsimons2021; Sadler et al., Reference Sadler, Vizard, Ford, Marcheselli, Pearce, Mandalia, David, Brodie, Forbes, Goodman, Goodman and McManus2018; Terhaag et al., Reference Terhaag, Fitzsimons, Daraganova and Patalay2021), academic attainment (Graziano et al., Reference Graziano, Reavis, Keane and Calkins2007; Kwon et al., Reference Kwon, Kupzyk and Benton2018), and SEN (Forrest et al., Reference Forrest, Gibson, Halligan and St Clair2020), which were available at baseline, following guidance (Mulder & Hamaker, Reference Mulder and Hamaker2021). Specifically, these covariates were specified as predictors of all constructs (at the main factor level) at all timepoints, with the exception of attainment, which was specified as a predictor of constructs at the first timepoint only, as this was assessed one year before data collection began. The stable individual demographic factors were entered as predictors of all constructs (again, at the main factor level) at all three timepoints, the effects of which were set to be equal. As participants took part in surveys within their education settings, thus we controlled for the school clustering in all SEM analyses, using the Type = Complex command in Mplus. This accounts for the non-independence of observations by adjusting the standard errors and goodness-of-fit statistics (intracluster correlations at the item level ranged from .01 to .04, M = .02).

In reviewing model fit, we followed guidance from Hu and Bentler (Reference Hu and Bentler1999) and considered root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) values below .06, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) values greater than .95, and root mean square residual (SRMR) values below .08 indicative of good fit. We report chi-square for reference. In interpreting specific pathways and relationships between variables in the RI-CLPM, we considered an alpha of < .01 to indicate statistical significance, to correct for multiple comparison and given our large sample size and the influence these features can have for p-values (Abdi, Reference Abdi2007; Sullivan & Feinn, Reference Sullivan and Feinn2012). We were guided by Adachi and Willoughby (Reference Adachi and Willoughby2015) in interpreting cross-lagged parameters dynamically in the context of the wider model. Cohen’s (Reference Cohen1992) widely used thresholds for standardized beta coefficients were not developed for longitudinal autoregressive models, meaning that dramatically smaller relationships between constructs not only are common but can be meaningful. Thus, in presenting our results we offer commentary on these cross-lag relationships with attention to how these sit contextually within the wider model.

Results

The RI-CLPM model showed good fit to the data: χ 2 (605) = 9,484.72 (Standard Deviation [SD] = 124.27); RMSEA = .03 (SD = .00); CFI = .93 (SD = .00), TLI = .92 (SD = .00); SRMR = .04 (SD = .00). Results are presented in Figure 3, which shows statistically significant autoregressive effects and cross-lagged pathways. Concurrent relationships are presented separately for ease in Table 3. As they are not of primary interest in our study, we report on covariate effects on within-level constructs in our Supplementary Materials (see Table S7, Appendix H).

Figure 3. RI-CLPM developmental cascades results. Model shows statistically significant standardized (β) autoregressive and cross-lagged pathways; concurrent relationships shown separately in Table 3. Solid lines show positive relationships and dotted lines show inverse relationships. Some features not shown for simplicity, including covariates and the modelling of random intercepts and within-person latent factors from the grand means (indicated as reminder in upper left corner). ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

Table 3. Concurrent Relationships between variables (includes across between-person difference random intercept factors, and between within-person variables within each timepoint; β)

Note. RI = random intercepts; T1 = Timepoint 1; T2 = Timepoint 2; T3 = Timepoint 3. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

Autoregressive effects

Results indicated stability for all within-person constructs, with scores on each construct predicted by earlier scores at the preceding timepoint (Figure 3). The largest autoregressive effects were present for emotional distress, followed by emotion regulation and peer connection. Notably, family connection showed smaller autoregressive effects relative to other constructs; scores at T1 predicted scores at T2 with a beta (β) of .08 (p = .001) and scores at T2 predicted those at T3 with a beta (β) =of .22 (p < .001).

Concurrent relationships

As can be seen in Table 3, statistically significant within-person concurrent relationships were observed across all constructs, with similar effect sizes observed in these respective relationships at each timepoint. At each stage, emotional distress showed strong inverse relationships with emotion regulation and small relationships with both connection variables, while emotion regulation, family adult connection, and peer connection each showed moderate relationships with one another. We also note that associations between the random intercept latent factors representing between-person differences (also shown in Table 3) were moderate to large, with differences in emotional distress and regulation showing an especially large relationship, family relationships showing moderate relationships with distress and regulation, and peer connection showing large relationships with distress, regulation, and family connection.

Cross-lagged pathways

Inspection of cross-lagged pathways showed several statistically significant pathways; some pathways were nonsignificant and those are not shown in Figure 3 for ease of reader interpretation.

H1: Emotional distress will negatively predict later emotion regulation and social connections. Greater levels of emotional distress at T1 and T2 significantly predicted lower levels of emotion regulation at subsequent timepoints (β = −.21 [p < .001] and β = −.17 [p = .003], respectively). Greater emotional distress did not significantly predict change in perceived connection to family adults or peers at any timepoints.

H2: Emotion regulation will negatively predict later emotional distress and positively predict social connections. Greater emotion regulation at T2 predicted lower distress at T3 (β = −.13, p = .008), although this was not observed across T1 to T2. Greater emotion regulation at T1 predicted greater family connection at T2 (β = .10, p = .002). Wider autoregressive relationships show relative instability in family connection across these timepoints, indicating change in this area is more likely than other constructs. Thus, this kind of small effect may be less informative than it may be in the context of other relationships where there is greater stability over time, but it does indicate that regulation may act as one potential contributor to the instability in family connection at this time. This relationship was not present from T2 to T3, with no statistically significant relationship across this stage. Finally, emotion regulation did not significantly predict later peer connection at any timepoint.

H3: Social connections will negatively predict later emotional distress and positively predict emotion regulation. Family connection did not predict emotional distress or emotion regulation at any subsequent timepoints, and peer connection at T1 did not predict emotional distress or emotion regulation at T2. However, peer connection at T2 did predict emotional distress at T3, but not in the expected direction, with greater perceived connection predicting greater distress (β = −.06, p = .003). We highlight that concurrent relationships showed more theoretically expected signs and indeed moderate relationships, wherein greater perceived peer connection was instead associated with lower levels of distress and greater emotion regulation, as might be more theoretically anticipated.

H4: Peer and family social connections will positively predict one another over time. Greater peer connection at T2 predicted greater family connection at T3 (β = .05, p = .006). No other significant cross-lagged relationships were observed across these constructs. In the absence of evidence for reciprocal effects, H4 could not be supported.

Discussion

We sought to explore possible reciprocal relationships between emotional distress, emotion regulation, and family adult and peer social connection over the course of early adolescence. Theory and past research have indicated a complex interplay between these constructs, especially during early adolescence as a vulnerable period for the emergence of psychopathology. However, to our knowledge, no previous study had directly examined developmental cascades across these areas in combination and with an emphasis on this developmental stage. Thus, we set out to offer a robust empirical investigation of these complex relationships, with a large sample and a longitudinal design examining within-person change over time. We found that a) emotional distress and emotion regulation share a reciprocal negative longitudinal relationship, b) emotion regulation predicts greater family connection in the initial stages of early adolescence, and c) connection to peers positively predicts family connection, but also slightly greater distress, in the later stages of early adolescence. Findings indicate a risk of a negative spiral between emotional distress and emotion regulation in early adolescence, and that social connection may not necessarily play the role we might expect it to in reducing difficulties over time.

Before considering reciprocal effects across emotional distress and emotion regulation, we comment briefly here on the temporal stability of these constructs observed in autoregressive effects (as shown in Figure 3). The large autoregressive effects for emotional distress (T1→T2 β = .34, T2→T3 β = .37) are relatively unsurprising. Previous research has similarly indicated that distress and wider internalizing difficulties tend to be stable during adolescence (e.g., Danneel et al., Reference Danneel, Nelemans, Spithoven, Bastin, Bijttebier, Colpin, Van Den Noortgate, Van Leeuwen, Verschueren and Goossens2019); our results further contribute to this evidence by showing that distress tends to persist over time at the within-person level. Emotion regulation showed moderate stability at T1 to T2 (β = .24) and stronger stability from T2 to T3 (β = .32). This lower stability across the initial two timepoints perhaps echoes suggestions that the changes during early adolescence may exceed young people’s current regulatory capacity (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Jones, Levita, Libby, Pattwell, Ruberry, Soliman and Somerville2010; Dahl, Reference Dahl2001; Hollenstein & Lougheed, Reference Hollenstein and Lougheed2013). This finding also indicates that although emotion regulation does show some stability, this is amenable to change over time in early adolescence and could be targeted through intervention, as explored later in this section.

We found greater emotional distress was related to reductions in self-reported emotion regulation over time, across both T1 to T2 (β = −.21) and T2 to T3 (β = −.17). Considering these relationships against Adachi & Willoughby’s (Reference Adachi and Willoughby2015) guidance to interpret effect sizes dynamically in the context of the wider model, we note these are considerable effect sizes; these constructs each show moderate to strong stability over time (Distress T1→T2 β = .34, T2→T3 β = .37; Regulation T1→T2 β = .24, T2→T3 β = .32) and a strong concurrent relationship with one another (T1 = −.65, T2 = −.71, T3 = −.74). This may highlight that, for many young people, emotional distress can change the way they think about emotional situations, and whether they think they can respond in effective ways. The cognitive model of depression holds that wider negative self-perception and self-evaluation may feature as a key depressive symptom, and, indeed, there is evidence to support this among adolescents experiencing depression (Orchard et al., Reference Orchard, Pass, Marshall and Reynolds2017, Reference Orchard, Pass and Reynolds2019). It may however be useful for researchers to explore this connection further to better elucidate such potential mechanisms for distress affecting perception of regulation (e.g., whether changes to emotional reactivity may play a role), and to understand how this may also relate to more objectively measured emotion regulation behaviors and capacity.

Further, given our finding that emotion regulation at T2 also negatively predicted later emotional distress at T3 (β = −.13), it seems this reciprocal relationship between emotional distress and emotion regulation could potentially lead to a spiral of negative effects during adolescence. Because we did not examine directly observed emotion regulation behaviors or skills, but explored self-perceptions of emotion regulation abilities, our findings focus attention on the fluidity of emotional self-beliefs (that may or may not match actual skills). Thus, we recommend that intervention efforts should be focused on teaching children and young people to manage shifting self-beliefs during times of emotional distress, which could facilitate more optimal transitions during the stressful adolescent years. This is, for instance, a common feature of CBT for child and adolescent depression (Kennard et al., Reference Kennard, Hughes and Foxwell2016). However, such targeted interventions should be underpinned and preceded by earlier, preventive interventions, especially given the context of our findings (e.g., very large community sample as opposed to clinical sample). In this vein, universal school-based social and emotional learning interventions that emphasize the development of emotional awareness and management skills offer considerable promise (Durlak et al., Reference Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor and Schellinger2011). As above, it would be helpful to explore and understand whether this is also replicated for more objectively measured emotion regulation.

Findings in relation to family adult connection were notable in two respects. First, autoregressive effects were smaller than for other variables (T1→T2 β = .08, T2→T3 β = .22), with effect sizes being, substantially smaller than those observed for emotional distress, emotion regulation, and peer connection. This relative lack of temporal stability may have a developmental explanation, given that the time period covered (ages 11–12 to 13–14) is one of significant flux and transformation in terms of family relationships. For example, the transition from childhood to adolescence is often marked by increased conflict with, and reduced attachment to, parents and carers (Allison, Reference Allison2000; Nickerson & Nagle, Reference Nickerson and Nagle2005). However, such changes are typically transient, and likely reflect a ‘relational discontinuity’ caused by the reorganization of responsibilities and a move towards a more egalitarian relationship (Branje, Reference Branje2018).

Second, there was little evidence that family adult connection exerted any kind of within-person influence on later emotional distress, perceived emotion regulation, or peer connection. Indeed, it appeared to be influenced by perceived emotion regulation and peer connection, from T1 to T2 (β = .10) and T2 to T3 (β = .05). These are small effect sizes and when viewed in the context of the observed stability and concurrent relationships may not be especially meaningful, though should not be set aside altogether. That is, if a variable shows less stability, as family connection does from T1 to T2 here, this suggests there is considerable change in this variable over time, and so an effect of this size may not be practically significant (Adachi & Willoughby, Reference Adachi and Willoughby2015). This broad pattern of findings may have methodological explanations, including that our RI-CLPM approach accounted for stable between-person differences, and that we made use of a community sample, rather than a clinical sample of those identified as having a heightened level of difficulties. Our reasoning here is that the former is known to attenuate within-person cross-lagged effects (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper, Grasman, Sciences and Sciences2015), while the latter likely means that the prevalence of adolescent-parent conflict and related difficulties of a magnitude strong enough to trigger, for example, the onset of later emotional distress, would be relatively low.

Finally, we highlight several features of our findings in relation to peer connection. First, we found peer connection was not prospectively predicted by any other constructs included in the model. However, there were concurrent relationships between peer connection and all other variables within timepoints, to varying extents (β absolute values range = .13–.33). Taken together, it seems that while those who feel more connected to their peers are at the same time more likely to feel more connected to their family, more capable in managing their emotions, and less distressed, these constructs are not necessarily in turn predicting how connected one feels to peers over time – or at least not at one-year intervals. Wider research examining this relationship longitudinally is scarce, and varied conceptualizations, foci, and measurement approaches have complicated the ability to draw comparisons with our findings. For instance, English et al. (Reference English, John, Srivastava and Gross2012) reported that emotion regulation strategy usage has long-term social effects, but this study captures strategy rather than perception of one’s ability to manage emotions effectively as in our study, and focuses on late adolescence. Furthermore, as noted above, our use of a RI-CLPM approach modelling within-person effects has more accurately estimated these cross-lagged relationships, which are often attenuated in this approach (Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper, Grasman, Sciences and Sciences2015).

Second, we highlight that like family connection, peer connection showed limited impact on other constructs over time, which is somewhat surprising given the increasing emphasis on peers during early adolescence. One possible explanation is the focus in item content on school peers along with the timing of our first wave; the first wave of our data corresponds with the first year of secondary school in England, where adolescents experience an expanded peer group and a range of changes in their friendships (Jindal-Snape et al., Reference Jindal-Snape, Hannah, Cantali, Barlow and MacGillivray2020). This period of flux could have had implications for the meaningfulness of connectedness to peers. However, there were two exceptions: a positive prediction of later family connection from T2 to T3 (as discussed above) and, surprisingly, a positive prediction of later emotional distress from T2 to T3 (β = −.06). While both can be considered small relationships in the context of the wider model, the stability of emotional distress suggests that the effects of peer connection upon this outcome should be given close consideration. That is, greater perceived connection to one’s peers predicted greater levels of emotional distress 1 year later, which directly contradicts our hypothesis and basic interpersonal theory expectations. We are unclear what the explanation for this relationship may be. We cannot rule out methodological contributors, although cannot find a satisfying explanation in this regard. Our sample is particularly large, which could lead to spurious results, but this is not a very small relationship (e.g., β = .01) easily explainable as a quirk of increased test sensitivity. We wondered whether this was the result of treating peer connection as manifest rather than latent, but CFA of the peer connection variable had shown this to be a good unidimensional model with highly uniform factor loadings (see Appendix B in Supplementary Materials). While we therefore doubt treatment of these as manifest variables would influence findings, we cannot rule this out, since a model with peer connection as a latent factor was not possible, as this could not converge.

If we assume this effect is genuine, there are several possible explanations. One is that these relationships were modelled at the within-person level; it could be that experiencing a fluctuation in peer relationships over time creates personal pressure to retain heightened connection and avoid rejection, since adolescent peer relationships can be ‘high stakes’ (Blakemore, Reference Blakemore2018). However, we might expect such relational insecurity to translate into lowered connection scores, suggesting this is likely not a driver. Alternatively, there may be a dynamic in which relationships with peers at school include aspects of conflict and drama that are difficult to navigate over time. Certainly adolescents are navigating increasingly complex social situations and circles, and evidence suggests that negative friendship qualities are associated with adolescent depressive symptoms (Schwartz-Mette et al., Reference Schwartz-Mette, Shankman, Dueweke, Borowski and Rose2020). Another possible explanation is co-rumination, a dyadic process wherein individuals excessively discuss, rehash, dwell on, and speculate about problems (Rose et al., Reference Rose, Schwartz-Mette, Glick, Smith and Luebbe2014). Evidence suggests a greater tendency toward co-rumination in adolescence and that this can be associated with emotional distress (Rose et al., Reference Rose, Schwartz-Mette, Glick, Smith and Luebbe2014; Stone et al., Reference Stone, Hankin, Gibb and Abela2011). Interestingly, a recent study showed that within-person shifts in social communication mediated the relationship between stressful life events and internalizing difficulties, suggesting that co-rumination may play a key role in psychopathology processes (Rodman et al., Reference Rodman, Vidal Bustamante, Dennison, Flournoy, Coppersmith, Nook, Worthington, Mair and McLaughlin2021). Thus, it may be that those with greater connection also show a tendency toward co-rumination in the current sample, and it would be helpful to explore this further in a sample with available co-rumination data. Despite these methodological and conceptual considerations, then, we cannot confidently explain this finding. We encourage researchers to examine such relationships using RI-CLPM and similar within-person approaches within other samples to see if this result is replicated. One potential avenue here could be examining whether there is evidence of a U-shaped relationship between peer connection and emotional distress, wherein both low and high levels of social connection increase risk of distress over time.

Strengths and limitations

This study is a robust empirical investigation of a complex developmental trajectory, with many methodological strengths to aid confidence in findings. Our investigation focuses on a large sample, uses a longitudinal design to offer insight into the way these constructs interact over time, benefits from a RI-CLPM-approach modelling relationships at the within-person level, and includes covariates to aid precision in estimates.

However, although the demographics of our sample are broadly similar to national patterns, this should not be considered a representative sample, as the HeadStart programme and evaluation from which we draw our data has been implemented across six disadvantaged areas of England. Thus, readers should be cautious in generalizing findings, and it would be valuable to further explore these relationships in a large-scale representative sample. Having said this, we highlight value in understanding these relationships among a sample of young people living in disadvantaged areas and thus at greater risk of experiencing worsened mental health outcomes (though of course we do not suggest this is representative of adolescents in all disadvantaged areas).

We note some limitations in our measurement approaches. The SDQ subscale is brief and so captures a narrow grouping and range of emotional distress symptoms; future research could investigate these relationships using more comprehensive assessments. Similarly, the TEIQue-ASF subscale required some revision to be viable as a single-factor construct of perceived emotion regulation, and it would be valuable to further explore this relationship with other regulation measures. This includes complementary use of more objective approaches akin to skills testing, to more fully understand the respective roles of perceived and actual emotion regulation (Qualter et al., Reference Qualter, Davis, Keefer, Parker, Saklofske, Wigelsworth, Simmons and Stough2017). Similarly, all variables used here are self-report, and future research could explore wider strategies, such as use of multiple informants and observation approaches. Furthermore, our measures capture specific functions within regulation and social domains. While this is not a limitation per se—it is valuable to understand the role these particular functions play—it may be helpful to explore these relationships with other aspects of these regulation and social domains. For instance, reviews and meta-analyses have indicated that specific emotion regulation behaviors may influence rates of distress to differing extents (Shapero et al., Reference Shapero, Abramson and Alloy2016; Young et al., Reference Young, Sandman and Craske2019); such different components are not captured in our construct of perceived emotion regulation at a more overarching level. Ongoing investigation drawing on varied measurement approaches and constructs, such as approaches that more directly assess emotion regulation skills and behaviors, or measures of social competence, could offer useful alternative insights into how these complex domains of functioning interact developmentally with psychopathology across adolescence.

Analytically, although use of RI-CLPM allows modelling of within-person relationships, this does still provide a summation of an overarching pattern, rather than capturing more individualistic trajectories. In future work it would be helpful to use latent class analysis to understand individual-level trajectories in the stability of particular constructs and how they may express alongside one another (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Qualter and Humphrey2019). Further, we note that the RI-CLPM is vulnerable to the effects of unobserved time-varying confounders that might affect these relationships, meaning that if individual differences in changes over time are related to such confounders then cross-lagged parameters may produce biased estimates (Usami et al., Reference Usami, Murayama and Hamaker2019). A recent variant of panel modelling, the general cross-lagged panel modelling (GLCM), can be specified to control for such confounders, but this approach does not partial out stable between-person differences in the way that RI-CLPM does (Usami, Reference Usami2021).

Finally, we recognize that readers may wonder about the role of the HeadStart programme in the reciprocal relationships we report here, given that our data is drawn from the evaluation of this program, which aims to explore and test ways to improve the mental health and wellbeing of young people. This means that individuals in our sample were likely exposed to a range of embedded provision and potentially more targeted interventions, including approaches that may target their emotion regulation or peer relationships. However, we emphasize that this is not at all unusual for adolescents in English schools, given a continued emphasis on schools as a site for mental health prevention and promotion (Department of Health & Department for Education, 2017; Public Health England, 2021). Thus, we would not expect that our participants’ engagement with the HeadStart programme would render these findings contextually specific, and indeed would not expect the nature of relationships between such constructs to be markedly affected by such a program (e.g., an intervention targeting emotion regulation may increase perceived regulatory capacity, which could in turn predict lower distress, but we would not expect it to affect how this predicted distress). Nevertheless, we note that ongoing investigation across different samples and contexts would be valuable.

Conclusion

The current study has provided evidence of reciprocal relationships over time between emotional distress and perceived emotion regulation in early adolescence in a large sample and using robust modelling techniques. We note risk of a negative spiral for those experiencing initial distress in adolescence, which may become self-perpetuating over time by contributing to feeling less in control of one’s emotions. It would be helpful to explore and better understand the extent to which this is related to negative self-evaluation for those experiencing distress or whether feelings of distress are genuinely disrupting and/or overwhelming regulatory capacity—or, more likely, a combination of the two. Our findings raise questions regarding the role of social connection in relation to these emotional experiences, given that family connection did not exert a great deal of influence over distress and regulation over time, and it appeared connection to one’s peers may potentially increase distress. This emphasizes the need for ongoing research examining specific dimensions of relationships in relation to such functions in adolescence to better understand such processes. Taken together, the current study emphasizes the importance of early intervention for those with emergent difficulties and the importance of providing support in positive self-belief and regulatory strategies during early adolescence as a potentially difficult transition.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422001407

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the work of the wider research teams at the Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families and The University of Manchester for their role in generating and managing data, and to the participating Local Authorities and schools for coordinating data collection. We thank the participants for taking the time to complete measures and to the National Pupil Database, from which demographic data was obtained.

Author contributions

Demkowicz led the conception and design of the current study, the analysis of data, and the development of the manuscript. Panayiotou was a major contributor to the design of the study and analysis of data, and reviewed and refined the manuscript. Qualter and Humphrey were major contributors to the design of the study, made contributions to the writing of the manuscript, and reviewed and refined the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding statement

The data used in this study was collected as part of the HeadStart learning programme and supported by funding from The National Lottery Community Fund. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and it does not reflect the views of The National Lottery Community Fund.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

Ethical approval was granted for the HeadStart evaluation by University College London’s ethics committee (reference 8097/003), also covering secondary analysis of data generated in the evaluation, such as the current study. All participants provided informed assent, and parent/carer opt-out consent was established.

Footnotes

1 Emotional distress is also sometimes referred to as emotional symptoms, problems, or difficulties, or as psychological distress. Emotional distress may also be referred to as internalizing difficulties, which can be considered a wider category of symptoms that can be conceptualized as also capturing, for instance, obsessive behaviors or suicidal ideation and behaviors. We use ‘emotional distress’ to aid specificity (e.g., the oft-used term emotional symptoms does not semantically clarify what symptoms relate to, as ‘emotional’ encompasses a wider range than mood and worry) and to avoid implicit problematizing adolescents for their experiences (as is implied in 'symptoms', ‘problems’, and ‘difficulties).

2 Our construct/subscale names differ slightly from the original SRS, where names refer narrowly to ‘support’ but item content captures connection more broadly. Thus, these subscales appropriately capture our constructs of interest, and we use these conceptual terms to aid content validity and specificity in results and interpretations. We also highlight that peer connection items refer to school-based peers, and family connection items to ‘an adult at home’ rather than wider family (e.g., siblings).

3 Here, we grouped together those identified as having SEN both with and without formal designation of need via an Education, Health, and Care plan (previously known as a statement of SEN), as some pupils are identified as having SEN without this kind of plan (Department for Education, 2018b).

4 Here, we converted multigroup and continuous demographic variables into binary variables to facilitate analysis: we created a binary ethnic group variable with White and UK ethnic minority group categories; and low academic attainment was converted by identifying those in the lowest quartile (99 and below on the 80–120 range) as having low academic attainment

References

Abdi, H. (2007). The Bonferonni and Šidák corrections for multiple comparisons. Encyclopedia of Measurement and Statistics, 103107. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412952644 Google Scholar
Adachi, P., & Willoughby, T. (2015). Interpreting effect sizes when controlling for stability effects in longitudinal autoregressive models: Implications for psychological science. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 116128. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2014.963549 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, B. N. (2000). Parent-adolescent conflict in early adolescence: Research and implications for middle school programs. Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences Education, 18, 16.Google Scholar
Antony, E. M. A., Pihlajamäki, M., Speyer, L. G., & Murray, A. L. (2022). Does emotion dysregulation mediate the association between ADHD symptoms and internalizing problems? A longitudinal within-person analysis in a large population-representative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 63(12), 15831590. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13624 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress: Reconsidered. American Psychologist, 54, 317326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balázs, J., Miklõsi, M., Keresztény, Á., Hoven, C. W., Carli, V., Wasserman, C., Apter, A., Bobes, J., Brunner, R., Cosman, D., Cotter, P., Haring, C., Iosue, M., Kaess, M., Kahn, J. P., Keeley, H., Marusic, D., Postuvan, V., Resch, F.Wasserman, D. (2013). Adolescent subthreshold-depression and anxiety: Psychopathology, functional impairment and increased suicide risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54, 670677. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bariola, E., Gullone, E., & Hughes, E. K. (2011). Child and adolescent emotion regulation: The role of parental emotion regulation and expression. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 198212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-011-0092-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 843857. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00065-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, B. L., Gangle, M. R., Perry, N. B., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2016). Indirect effects of emotion regulation on peer acceptance and rejection: The roles of positive and negative social behaviours. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 62, 415439. https://doi.org/10.13110/merrpalmquar1982.62.4.0415 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, B. L., Perry, N. B., O’Brien, M., Calkins, S. D., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2015). Identifying developmental cascades among differentiated dimensions of social competence and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychopathology, 51, 10621073. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039472 Google ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J. (2018). Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 116122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417738144 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S. J. (2019). Adolescence and mental health. The Lancet, 393, 20302031. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(19)31013-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, L., Butler, H., Thomas, L., Carlin, J., Glover, S., Bowes, G., & Patton, G. (2007). Social and school connectedness in early secondary school as predictors of late teenage substance use, mental health, and academic outcomes. Journal of Adolescent Health, 40, 918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.10.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (2010). Social competence, externalizing, and internalizing behavioral adjustment from early childhood through early adolescence: Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 717735. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000416 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, S. J. (2000). Affect regulation and the development of psychopathology. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Branje, S. (2018). Development of parent–adolescent relationships: Conflict interactions as a mechanism of change. Child Development Perspectives, 12, 171176. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12278 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, B. B., & Bakken, J. P. (2011). Parenting and peer relationships: Reinvigorating research on family-peer linkages in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 153165. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00720.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Burns, R. A., Crisp, D. A., & Burns, R. B. (2020). Re-examining the reciprocal effects model of self-concept, self-efficacy, and academic achievement in a comparison of the Cross-Lagged Panel and Random-Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel frameworks. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 7791. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12265 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., Levita, L., Libby, V., Pattwell, S. S., Ruberry, E. J., Soliman, F., & Somerville, L. H. (2010). The storm and stress of adolescence: Insights from human imaging and mouse genetics. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 225235. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castro, V. L., Cheng, Y., Halberstadt, A. G., & Grühn, D. (2016). EUReKA! A conceptual model of emotion understanding. Emotion Review, 8, 258268. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073915580601 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 98101. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costello, E. J., Copeland, W., & Angold, A. (2011). Trends in psychopathology across the adolescent years: What changes when children become adolescents, and when adolescents become adults? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52, 10151025. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02446.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. E. (2001). Affect regulation, brain development, and behavioral/emotional health in adolescence. CNS Spectrums, 6, 6072. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852900022884 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danneel, S., Nelemans, S., Spithoven, A., Bastin, M., Bijttebier, P., Colpin, H., Van Den Noortgate, W., Van Leeuwen, K., Verschueren, K., & Goossens, L. (2019). Internalizing problems in adolescence: Linking loneliness, social anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms over time. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47, 16911705. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00539-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demkowicz, O., Ashworth, E., Mansfield, R., Stapley, E., Miles, H., Hayes, D., Burrell, K., Moore, A., & Deighton, J. (2020). Children and young people’s experiences of completing mental health and wellbeing measures for research: Learning from two school-based pilot projects. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 14, 35. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-020-00341-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department for Education. (2018b). Special educational needs: Transfer of statements of SEN to education, health and care plans, end March 2018. Ad hoc notice. https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/705243/Transfer_of_statements_of_SEN_to_education__health_and_care_plans.pdf Google Scholar
Department for Education, & Office for National Statistics. (2021). Special educational needs in England: Academic year 2020/21. https://explore-education-statistics.service.gov.uk/find-statistics/special-educational-needs-in-england Google Scholar
Department of Health, & Department for Education. (2017). Transforming children and young people’s mental health provision: a green paper. https://doi.org/979-1-5286-0061-3 Google Scholar
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82, 405432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Education Endowment Foundation. (2013). Pre-testing in EEF evaluations.Google Scholar
English, T., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Emotion regulation and peer-rated social functioning: A 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 780784. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.09.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ewell Foster, C., Horwitz, A., Thomas, A., Opperman, K., Gipson, P., Burnside, A., Stone, D. M., & King, C. A. (2017). Connectedness to family, school, peers, and community in socially vulnerable adolescents. Children and Youth Services Review, 81, 321331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2017.08.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, C. L., Gibson, J. L., Halligan, S. L., & St Clair, M. C. (2020). A cross-lagged analysis of emotion regulation, peer problems, and emotional problems in children with and without early language difficulties: Evidence from the Millennium Cohort Study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 12271239. https://doi.org/10.23641/asha CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnefski, N., Rieffe, C., Jellesma, F., Terwogt, M. M., & Kraaij, V. (2007). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotional problems in 9-11-year-old children: The development of an instrument. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-006-0562-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gill, V., Panayiotou, M., Demkowicz, O., & Humphrey, N. (2019). Learning from HeadStart : Does social action help young people with emerging mental health issues ? What is HeadStart ? April Evidence Based Practice Unit.Google Scholar
Goodman, R., Meltzer, H., & Bailey, V. (1998). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A pilot study on the validity of the self-report version. International Review of Psychiatry, 15, 173177. https://doi.org/10.1080/0954026021000046137 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. W., Olchowski, A. E., & Gilreath, T. D. (2007). How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prevention Science, 8, 206213. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-007-0070-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, P. A., Reavis, R. D., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation in children’s early academic success. Journal of School Psychology, 45, 319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2006.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & Muñoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental health. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 2, 151164. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x Google Scholar
Hall-Lande, J. A., Eisenberg, M. E., Christenson, S. L., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2007). Social isolation, psychological health, and protective factors in adolescence. Adolescence, 42, 265286. https://doi.org/10.1130/B26151.1 Google ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Sciences, B., & Sciences, B. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, R. A., Qualter, P., & Robinson, S. J. (2013). Loneliness trajectories from middle childhood to pre-adolescence: Impact on perceived health and sleep disturbance. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 12951304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.12.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollenstein, T., & Lougheed, J. P. (2013). Beyond storm and stress: Typicality, transactions, timing, and temperament to Account for Adolescent Change. American Psychologist, 68, 444454. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033586 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holt-Lunstad, J., Robles, T., & Sbarra, D. A. (2017). Advancing social connection as a public health priority in the United States. American Psychologist, 72, 517530. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000103 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N., & Panayiotou, M. (2020). Bounce Back: randomised trial of a brief, school-based group intervention for children with emergent mental health difficulties. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 205210. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01612-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jindal-Snape, D., Hannah, E. F. S., Cantali, D., Barlow, W., & MacGillivray, S. (2020). Systematic literature review of primary–secondary transitions: International research. Review of Education, 8, 526566. https://doi.org/10.1002/rev3.3197 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, P. B. (2013). Adult mental health disorders and their age at onset. British Journal of Psychiatry, 202, s5s10. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.119164 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandel, D. B., Raveis, V. H., & Davies, M. (1991). Suicidal ideation in adolescence: Depression, substance use, and other risk factors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 289309. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537613 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennard, B. D., Hughes, J. L., & Foxwell, A. A. (2016). CBT for depression in children and adolescents: A guide to relapse prevention. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C. (2003). Epidemiology of women and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 74, 513. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00426-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kristensen, S. M., Danielsen, A. G., Jeno, L. M., Larsen, T. M. B., & Urke, H. B. (2021). The within-person effect of psychological distress on social self-efficacy: A random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 119. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12701 Google ScholarPubMed
Kuehner, C. (2017). Why is depression more common among women than among men? The Lancet Psychiatry, 4, 146158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30263-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwon, K., Kupzyk, K., & Benton, A. (2018). Negative emotionality, emotion regulation, and achievement: Cross-lagged relations and mediation of academic engagement. Learning and Individual Differences, 67, 3340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.07.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, G. W. (2017). Themes and theories: Perspectives on processes in family-peer relationships. In Parke, R. D. & Ladd, G. W. (Eds.), Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage (2nd ed., pp. 135). Routledge.Google Scholar
Lereya, S. T., Humphrey, N., Patalay, P., Wolpert, M., Böhnke, J. R., Macdougall, A., & Deighton, J. (2016). The Student Resilience Survey: Psychometric validation and associations with mental health. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 10, 44. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-016-0132-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, P. M., Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Seeley, J. R. (2000). Epidemiology and natural course of eating disorders in young women from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 12841292. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200010000-00016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, C. H. (2016). Confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data: Comparing robust maximum likelihood and diagonally weighted least squares. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 936949. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0619-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., Côté, S., & Beers, M. (2005). Emotion regulation abilities and the quality of social interaction. Emotion, 5, 113118. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malaquias, S., Crespo, C., & Francisco, R. (2015). How do adolescents benefit from family rituals? Links to social connectedness, depression and anxiety. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 30093017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-014-0104-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491495. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Mennin, D. S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and adolescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 544554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2011.06.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, P., Coffey, C., Romaniuk, H., Olsson, C. A., Borschmann, R., Carlin, J. B., & Patton, G. C. (2011). The natural history of self-harm from adolescence to young adulthood: A population-based cohort study. The Lancet, 379, 236243. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(11)61141-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling, 28, 638648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, S. M. (2014). Determinants of adolescent suicidal ideation: rural versus urban. The Journal of Rural Health, 30, 175185. https://doi.org/10.1111/jrh.12042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. L., Obsuth, I., Speyer, L., Murray, G., McKenzie, K., Eisner, M., & Ribeaud, D. (2021). Developmental cascades from aggression to internalizing problems via peer and teacher relationships from early to middle adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50, 663673. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-021-01396-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Lottery Community Fund. (n.d.). HeadStart. Retrieved 16 March 2022, from https://www.tnlcommunityfund.org.uk/funding/strategic-investments/headstart Google Scholar
Nickerson, A. B., & Nagle, R. J. (2005). Parent and peer attachment in late childhood and early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 25, 223249. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431604274174 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orchard, F., Pass, L., Marshall, T., & Reynolds, S. (2017). Clinical characteristics of adolescents referred for treatment of depressive disorders. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 22, 6168. https://doi.org/10.1111/camh.12178 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orchard, F., Pass, L., & Reynolds, S. (2019). ‘I Am worthless and kind’; the specificity of positive and negative self-evaluation in adolescent depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 260273. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12215 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palacios-Barrios, E. E., & Hanson, J. L. (2019). Poverty and self-regulation: Connecting psychosocial processes, neurobiology, and the risk for psychopathology. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 90, 5264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.12.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pallini, S., Baiocco, R., Schneider, B. H., Madigan, S., & Atkinson, L. (2014). Early child-parent attachment and peer relations: A meta-analysis of recent research. Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 118123. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035736 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patalay, P., & Fitzsimons, E. (2021). Psychological distress, self-harm and attempted suicide in UK 17-year olds: Prevalence and sociodemographic inequalities. British Journal of Psychiatry, 219, 437439. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.258 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, K. J., Qualter, P., & Humphrey, N. (2019). The application of latent class analysis for investigating population child mental health: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01214 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, K. V. (2009). Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue). In Parker, J., Saklofske, D., & Stough, C. (Eds.), Assessing emotional intelligence. The Springer Series on human exceptionality (pp. 103117). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88370-0 Google Scholar
Public Health England. (2021). Promoting children and young people’s emotional health and wellbeing: A whole school and college approach. https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/414908/Final_EHWB_draft_20_03_15.pdf Google Scholar
Qualter, P., Brown, S. L., Munn, P., & Rotenberg, K. J. (2010). Childhood loneliness as a predictor of adolescent depressive symptoms: An 8-year longitudinal study. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 19, 493501. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-009-0059-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qualter, P., Brown, S. L., Rotenberg, K. J., Vanhalst, J., Harris, R. A., Goossens, L., Bangee, M., & Munn, P. (2013). Trajectories of loneliness during childhood and adolescence: Predictors and health outcomes. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 12831293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qualter, P., Davis, S. K., Keefer, K. V., Parker, J. D. A., Saklofske, D. H., Wigelsworth, M., Simmons, N., & Stough, C. (2017). Emotional competency in education: Core concepts and applications. British Journal of Educational Psychology Monograph Series II: Psychological Aspects of Education–Current Trends, 12, 5171.Google Scholar
Qualter, P., Vanhalst, J., Harris, R. A., Lodder, G., Bangee, M., van Roekel, E., Verhagen, M., & Maes, M. (2015). Loneliness across the lifespan. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 250264. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615568999 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodman, A. M., Vidal Bustamante, C. M., Dennison, M. J., Flournoy, J. C., Coppersmith, D. D. L., Nook, E. C., Worthington, S., Mair, P., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2021). A year in the social life of a teenager: Within-persons fluctuations in stress, phone communication, and anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychological Science, 9, 791809. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702621991804 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, A. J., Schwartz-Mette, R. A., Glick, G. C., Smith, R. L., & Luebbe, A. M. (2014). An observational study of co-rumination in adolescent friendships. Developmental Psychology, 50, 21992209. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037465 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roza, S. J., Hofstra, M. B., Van Der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2003). Stable prediction of mood and anxiety disorders based on behavioral and emotional problems in childhood: A 14-year follow-up during childhood, adolescence, and young adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 21162121. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2116 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadler, K., Vizard, T., Ford, T., Marcheselli, F., Pearce, N., Mandalia, D., David, J., Brodie, E., Forbes, N., Goodman, A., Goodman, R., & McManus, S. (2018). Mental health of children and young people in England, 2017: Summary of key findings. https://files.digital.nhs.uk/F6/A5706C/MHCYP2017Summary.pdf Google Scholar
Sass, D. A., Schmitt, T. A., & Marsh, H. W. (2014). Evaluating model fit With ordered categorical data within a measurement invariance framework: A comparison of estimators. Structural Equation Modeling, 21, 167180. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2014.882658 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, S. M., Azzopardi, P. S., Wickremarathne, D., & Patton, G. C. (2018). The age of adolescence. The Lancet Child and Adolescent Health, 2, 223228. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schäfer, J. Ö., Naumann, E., Holmes, E. A., Tuschen-Caffier, B., & Samson, A. C. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 261276. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-016-0585-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, R. L., Arch, J. J., Landy, L. N., & Hankin, B. L. (2018). The longitudinal effect of emotion regulation strategies on anxiety levels in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 47, 978991. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2016.1157757 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz-Mette, R. A., Shankman, J., Dueweke, A. R., Borowski, S., & Rose, A. J. (2020). Relations of friendship experiences with depressive symptoms and loneliness in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 146, 664700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapero, B. G., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Emotional reactivity and internalizing symptoms; Moderating role of emotion regulation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 328340. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-015-9722-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive sympoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74, 18691880. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solmi, M., Radua, J., Olivola, M., Croce, E., Soardo, L., Salazar de Pablo, G., Il Shin, J., Kirkbride, J. B., Jones, P., Kim, J. H., Kim, J. Y., Carvalho, A. F., Seeman, M. V., Correll, C. U., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2021). Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: Large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Molecular Psychiatry. 27, 281–295. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7 Google ScholarPubMed
Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation and understanding: Implications for child psychopathology and therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 189222. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00087-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, L. B., Hankin, B. L., Gibb, B. E., & Abela, J. R. Z. (2011). Co-rumination predicts the onset of depressive disorders during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 752757. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023384 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strand, S. (2006). Comparing the predictive validity of reasoning tests and national end of Key Stage 2 tests: Which tests are the “best”? British Educational Research Journal, 32, 209225. https://doi.org/10.1080/01411920600569073 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, G. M., & Feinn, R. (2012). Using effect size - or why the p value is not enough. Journal of Graduate Medical Education, 4, 279282. https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, J., & Stewart, D. (2007). Development of population-based resilience measures in the primary school setting. Health Education, 107, 575599. https://doi.org/10.1108/MBE-09-2016-0047 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed.). Pearson Education Limited.Google Scholar
Terhaag, S., Fitzsimons, E., Daraganova, G., & Patalay, P. (2021). Sex, ethnic and socioeconomic inequalities and trajectories in child and adolescent mental health in Australia and the UK: findings from national prospective longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 62, 12551267. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13410 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (2011). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides of the developing coin. Emotion Review, 3, 5361. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073910380969 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tortella-Feliu, M., Balle, M., & Sesé Albert, A. (2010). Relationships between negative affectivity, emotion regulation, anxiety, and depressive symptoms in adolescents as examined through structural equation modeling. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 686693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.04.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umberson, D., Crosnoe, R., & Reczek, C. (2010). Social relationships and health behavior across life course. Annual Review of Sociology, 36, 139157. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-120011.Social CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Usami, S. (2021). On the differences between general cross-lagged panel models: Interpretation of cross-lagged parameters and model choice. Structural Equation Modeling, 28, 331344. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1821690 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Usami, S., Murayama, K., & Hamaker, E. L. (2019). Supplemental material for a unified framework of longitudinal models to examine reciprocal relations. Psychological Methods, 24, 637657. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000210.supp CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaillancourt, T., Brittain, H. L., McDougall, P., & Duku, E. (2013). Longitudinal links between childhood peer victimization, internalizing and externalizing problems, and academic functioning: Developmental cascades. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 12031215. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-013-9781-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadsworth, M. E., Evans, G. W., Grant, K., Carter, J. S., & Duffy, S. (2016). Poverty and the development of psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention (pp. 136179). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy404 Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2021). Adolescent health. https://www.who.int/health-topics/adolescent-health#tab=tab_1 Google Scholar
Young, K. S., Sandman, C. F., & Craske, M. G. (2019). Positive and negative emotion regulation in adolescence: links to anxiety and depression. Brain Sciences, 9, 76. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci9040076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., Crick, N. R., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Woods, K. E. (2006). The origins and development of psychopathology in females and males. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Volume one: Theory and method (2nd ed., pp. 76138). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 182194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515405 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Conceptual model for developmental cascades showing cross-lagged pathways only. Model shows cross-lagged pathways between emotional distress, emotion regulation, and family and peer connection. For simplicity we have not shown autoregressive pathways over time or concurrent relationships, or detailed statistical features (see Figure 2 for a detailed statistical model).

Figure 1

Table 1. Sample characteristics and national norms

Figure 2

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations (r)

Figure 3

Figure 2. Statistical model for main RI-CLPM developmental cascades analysis. A = autoregressive effects; B = concurrent relationships; C = within-person cross-lagged effects. Left-hand panel shows between-person differences; panels across the model show within-person components specified as latent variables; grand means used to estimate these shown in dotted lines. Some features not shown here for simplicity, including covariates and the items for emotional distress and emotion regulation.

Figure 4

Figure 3. RI-CLPM developmental cascades results. Model shows statistically significant standardized (β) autoregressive and cross-lagged pathways; concurrent relationships shown separately in Table 3. Solid lines show positive relationships and dotted lines show inverse relationships. Some features not shown for simplicity, including covariates and the modelling of random intercepts and within-person latent factors from the grand means (indicated as reminder in upper left corner). ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

Figure 5

Table 3. Concurrent Relationships between variables (includes across between-person difference random intercept factors, and between within-person variables within each timepoint; β)

Supplementary material: File

Demkowicz et al. supplementary material

Demkowicz et al. supplementary material

Download Demkowicz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 134.2 KB