Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T08:27:28.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The longitudinal association between infant negative emotionality, childhood maltreatment, and ADHD symptoms: A secondary analysis of data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2023

Dennis Golm*
Affiliation:
Centre for Innovation in Mental Health, School of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK
Valerie Brandt
Affiliation:
Centre for Innovation in Mental Health, School of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton, UK Clinic of Psychiatry, Social Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Hanover Medical School, Hanover, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Dennis Golm, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Infant temperament predicts harsh parenting, and attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms. Moreover, childhood maltreatment has consistently been associated with later ADHD symptoms. We hypothesized that infant negative emotionality predicted both ADHD symptoms and maltreatment, and that there was a bidirectional association between maltreatment experiences and ADHD symptoms.

Methods:

The study used secondary data from the longitudinal Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (N = 2860). A structural equation model was conducted, using maximum likelihood with robust standard errors. Infant negative emotionality acted as a predictor. Outcome variables were childhood maltreatment and ADHD symptoms at ages 5 and 9.

Results:

The model demonstrated good fit (root-mean-square error of approximation = .02, comparative fit index = .99, Tucker–Lewis index = .96). Infant negative emotionality positively predicted childhood maltreatment at ages 5 and 9, and ADHD symptoms at age 5. Age 5 maltreatment/ADHD symptoms predicted age 9 ADHD symptoms/maltreatment. Additionally, both childhood maltreatment and ADHD symptoms at age 5 mediated the association between negative emotionality and childhood maltreatment/ADHD symptoms at age 9.

Conclusions:

Given the bidirectional relationship between ADHD and experiences of maltreatment, it is vital to identify early shared risk factors to prevent negative downstream effects and support families at risk. Our study showed that infant negative emotionality, poses one of these risk factors.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by high levels of inattention and/or hyperactivity that interfere with everyday functioning. An inattentive, hyperactive, or combined subtype can be diagnosed (American Psychiatric Association, 2013)

According to meta-analytic evidence, the prevalence ranges between 6 and 7% depending on reporter (i.e., parent or teacher) with a higher prevalence for males than females (2.4:1). The inattentive subtype constitutes the most common subtype (Willcutt, Reference Willcutt2012).

While ADHD has a high heritability (Faraone & Larsson, Reference Faraone and Larsson2019), multiple environmental risk factors have been discussed, including experiences of childhood maltreatment (i.e., experiences of abuse or neglect) (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Polanczyk and Sonuga-Barke2020) to the extent that it was included into a recently published risk calculator for the development of young adult ADHD (Caye et al., Reference Caye, Agnew-Blais, Arseneault, Gonçalves, Kieling, Langley, Menezes, Moffitt, Passos, Rocha, Sibley, Swanson, Thapar, Wehrmeister and Rohde2019). A meta-analysis showed that in comparison to the general population, people with ADHD have 2.39 times higher odds of having experienced maltreatment with a maltreatment rate of 45.6% amongst people with an ADHD diagnosis (Clayton et al., Reference Clayton, Lee, Cheung, Theule and Henrikson2018). The relationship between experiences of adversity, (i.e., having experienced abuse, deprivation, poverty, neglect, or trauma) and ADHD seems to be bidirectional: early maltreatment experiences seem to pose a risk factor for the development of ADHD, and ADHD seems to be a risk factor for experiencing subsequent maltreatment.

Evidence for an effect of severe maltreatment on the development of ADHD symptoms and diagnosis comes from the English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) study. ERA is a longitudinal study that follows a group of Romanian adoptees who experienced severe institutional deprivation (i.e., severe neglect) in orphanages under the Ceausescu regime, and a comparison group of never-institutionalized UK adoptees. The study demonstrated a positive association between the time spent in the orphanage and ADHD symptoms at age 6 (Kreppner et al., Reference Kreppner, O’Connor and Rutter2001). Furthermore, experiences of prolonged deprivation were associated with persistently higher ADHD symptoms from childhood into young adulthood (Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Kennedy, Kumsta, Knights, Golm, Rutter, Maughan, Schlotz and Kreppner2017). In comparison to the general population, the risk of an ADHD diagnosis within the prolonged deprivation group was four times higher in adolescence (19%), and seven times higher (29%) in young adulthood (Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Kreppner, Knights, Kumsta, Maughan, Golm, Rutter, Schlotz and Sonuga-Barke2016). Due to the unique design of the study as a natural experiment, genetic confounding is unlikely (Rutter et al., Reference Rutter, Kumsta, Schlotz and Sonuga-Barke2012), and indeed, common genetic risk factors for neurodevelopmental disorders did not differ between Romanian adoptees who experienced prolonged or low levels of deprivation (Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Kennedy, Kumsta, Knights, Golm, Rutter, Maughan, Schlotz and Kreppner2017).

The effect seems to be less pronounced in general population studies with less severe but more common experiences of maltreatment. A population-based, cross-sectional, co-twin study (Dinkler et al., Reference Dinkler, Lundström, Gajwani, Lichtenstein, Gillberg and Minnis2017) including more than 8000 9-year-old twins found that the risk of having more than one neurodevelopmental disorder was seven times higher in maltreated than in non-maltreated individuals. This difference was almost completely explained by shared familial factors. However, the study found a small effect of childhood maltreatment experiences on ADHD symptoms (Dinkler et al., Reference Dinkler, Lundström, Gajwani, Lichtenstein, Gillberg and Minnis2017). This effect was replicated in a study with over 18,000 adult twins (Capusan et al., Reference Capusan, Kuja-Halkola, Bendtsen, Viding, McCrory, Marteinsdottir and Larsson2016), where maltreatment experiences predicted ADHD symptoms. The association was reduced when familial confounding was considered but remained significant. It is possible that familial confounding increases the risk of being maltreated for children with ADHD through gene–environment correlations (Dinkler et al., Reference Dinkler, Lundström, Gajwani, Lichtenstein, Gillberg and Minnis2017; Narusyte et al., Reference Narusyte, Neiderhiser, D’Onofrio, Reiss, Spotts, Ganiban and Lichtenstein2008). One possibility is that the risk for maltreatment by a parent could be increased through genetic influences on child temperamental traits (Dinkler et al., Reference Dinkler, Lundström, Gajwani, Lichtenstein, Gillberg and Minnis2017).

A recent meta-analysis showed that ADHD is indeed predicted by various temperamental traits including activity level, negative emotionality, and self-regulation (Kostyrka-Allchorne et al., Reference Kostyrka-Allchorne, Wass and Sonuga-Barke2020). Temperamental traits in turn have been shown to predict parental stress (Saisto et al., Reference Saisto, Salmela-Aro, Nurmi and HalmesmÄki2008) and harsh parenting (Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Barker, Boivin, Brendgen and Tremblay2006). In a sample of 1516 children, childhood temperament (negative emotionality) correlated moderately (r = .27) with harsh parenting at 17 months (Vitaro et al., Reference Vitaro, Barker, Boivin, Brendgen and Tremblay2006). In a longitudinal analysis of 12,474 children from the Millennium Cohort Study, high self-regulation at age 3 predicted less harsh parenting at age 5 (Baron & Malmberg, Reference Baron and Malmberg2019). The direct link between early temperament, harsh parenting or more severe forms of adverse parenting (i.e., abuse or neglect), and ADHD symptoms has, however, not been explored, yet.

By conducting a secondary analysis of data from the longitudinal Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) (Reichman et al., Reference Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel and McLanahan2001), the current study aims to assess the longitudinal relationship between infant temperament and subsequent experiences of maltreatment and ADHD symptoms. As the study will focus on temperament in infancy, only negative emotionality can be included as a predictor as activity level and self-regulation were not assessed as part of the FFCWS.

  1. 1) We hypothesize that infant negative emotionality at 12 months will predict.

    1. a) ADHD symptoms in middle childhood (age 5 and 9 years; replication of previous findings).

    2. b) The extent of child maltreatment in middle childhood (age 5 and 9 years).

  2. 2) To illustrate the bidirectional relationship between the extent of childhood maltreatment and ADHD symptoms, we hypothesize that:

    1. a) The extent of childhood maltreatment at age 5 years will predict ADHD symptoms at age 9 years.

    2. b) ADHD symptoms at age 5 years will predict childhood maltreatment extent at age 9 years.

  3. 3) The association between infant negative emotionality at 12 months and ADHD symptoms at age 9 years will be mediated by the extent of child maltreatment at age 5 years.

  4. 4) The association between infant negative emotionality at 12 months and childhood maltreatment extent at age 9 years will be mediated by ADHD symptoms at age 5 years.

Method

Sample

We analyzed data from the FFCWS (Reichman et al., Reference Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel and McLanahan2001), which follows a birth cohort of N = 4898 children born between 1998 and 2000. The study purposefully oversampled unmarried mothers (n = 3600). Participants were recruited from 75 US hospitals across 20 cities. Data is freely available from Princeton University’s Office of Population Research data archive (https://opr.princeton.edu/archive/restricted/Default.aspx). The study design has been described in detail elsewhere (Reichman et al., Reference Reichman, Teitler, Garfinkel and McLanahan2001).

The current study received ethical approval for secondary data analysis from the University of Southampton Ethics Committee (ERGO: 70317), and utilized outcome data from the 1-year (median age 12 months), 5-year (median age 5.08 years), and 9-year follow-up (median age 9.17 years). The 3-year follow-up used a different measure of ADHD symptoms and was therefore not included.

Measures

Negative emotionality in infancy

Negative emotionality was assessed with three items from the Emotionality, Activity, and Sociability Temperament Survey (Mathiesen & Tambs, Reference Mathiesen and Tambs1999) using a 5-point Likert scale (1= not at all like my child to 5 = very much like my child)- Often fusses and cries, gets upset easily, and reacts intensely when upset. The study authors’ wording differed slightly from the original version (original: 1= not characteristic or typical of your child to 5 = very characteristic or typical of your child). Data was available from N = 4316 resident mothers. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was α = .60. Inter-item correlations fell within an acceptable range (Clark & Watson, Reference Clark and Watson1995) with a mean inter-item correlation of r = .33 (range: r = .26 to r = .393).

Childhood maltreatment in middle childhood

The FFCWS assessed childhood maltreatment with selected items from the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus et al., Reference Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore and Runyan1998). This version excludes 8 items assessing severe physical maltreatment. Five items each (15 items in total) were used from the following subscales: psychological aggression (i.e., “Called him/ her dumb or lazy or some other name like that”), physical assault (i.e., “Hit him/ her on the bottom with something like a belt, hairbrush, a stick or some other hard object”), and neglect (i.e., “Was so drunk or high that they had a problem taking care of their child”). All items referred to the past year. The following response options were given: once, twice, 3–5 times, 6–10 times, 11–20 times, more than 20 times, not in the past year, but it happened before, this has never happened. Response options from once to over 20 times were coded 1 to 6. All other options were coded as 0. For the purpose of this study, a compound score was created by summing the ratings of all 15 items to reflect the extent of child maltreatment. Only complete data from the primary caregiver was included (age 5 years: N = 2874, α = .74; age 9 years: N = 2878, α = .79).

ADHD symptoms at ages 5 and 9 years

ADHD symptoms at ages 5 and 9 years were assessed with the 11-item Attention Problems subscale of the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1992) (i.e., “Can't concentrate, can't pay attention for long“; Age 5 years: N = 2758, α = .73, age 9 years: N = 3244, α = .82). The age 5 assessment included four items in the mother survey while 7 additional items were asked during the primary caregiver survey. At age 9 all items were assessed as part of the primary caregiver survey. The percentage of children above the cutoff (≥6, Skarphedinsson et al., Reference Skarphedinsson, Jarbin, Andersson and Ivarsson2021) is provided in Table 1.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Note. SD = standard deviation.

Covariates

We controlled for the influence of the following covariates on all outcome variables: sex (1 = male, 2 = female), low birthweight of under 2500 g (0 = no, 1 = yes), presence of physical disabilities (1 = yes, 2 = no), mother’s age in years, child age at time of mother interview in months, maternal depression (0 = no, 1 = yes) diagnosed with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview–short form (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Andrews, Mroczek, Ustun and Wittchen1998), and family income as a measure of socioeconomic status. We also accounted for the correlation between childhood maltreatment and ADHD symptoms at each time point.

Statistical analysis

A structural equation model was created using MPlus version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017). To account for non-normal distribution of data, the model used maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR). MLR estimates missing data on outcome variables using maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors under the missing at random assumption and requires listwise deletion of missing data on predictor variables. Model fit was established via the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA < .05 indicates good fit), comparative fit index (CFI), and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI; CFI/ TLI > .95 indicate good fit) (Geiser, Reference Geiser2012). All model coefficients represent standardized estimates (STDYX for continuous and STDY for categorical variables).

Results

Mean, standard deviation, median, and range for all variables of interest are reported in Table 1. As the estimator requires listwise deletion for predictor variables (including covariates), data was available from 2860 participants. The overall model fit was good (RMSEA = .02, CFI = .99, TLI = .96). The model explained 8.4% of variance for ADHD symptoms at age 5 years, 25.3% for ADHD symptoms at age 9 years, 4.5% of childhood maltreatment at age 5 years, and 31.5% of variance for childhood maltreatment at age 9 years. The complete model with standardized estimates (STDYX) can be found in Figure 1 and Table 2. The model specification and output can be found in Supplement 1.

Fig. 1 Coefficients represent standardized estimates (STDYX; * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001). Significant direct effects are represented by solid lines, insignificant effects are represented by dashed lines. Red lines represent significant indirect effects. Acronyms: ADHD symptoms: attention deficit/hyperactivity symptoms, Age C = Child age; Age M = Age of the mother; LBW = Low birth weight; MD = Maternal depression; PD = Physical disability; SES = Socio-economic status.

Table 2. Direct effects and correlations of variables of interests and covariates

Note. Maternal age, depression, socioeconomic status and child age were measured at the same time as the outcome variables. Correlations are between variables assessed at the same time point. Continuous predictors have been STDYX standardized, categorical predictors have been STDY standardized. SE = standard error, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Direct effects

Negative emotionality at age 12 months was associated with an increased likelihood for higher rates of childhood maltreatment at both ages 5 and 9. It was also significantly associated with increased ADHD symptoms at age 5 years, but not 9 years.

The extent of childhood maltreatment at age 5 years was associated with increased ADHD symptoms at age 9 years, and higher ADHD symptoms at age 5 years predicted higher likelihood for maltreatment at age 9 years.

In addition to cross-lagged effects, we also observed stability effects. ADHD symptoms at age 5 years predicted ADHD symptoms at age 9 years. The extent of childhood maltreatment at age 5 years predicted childhood maltreatment extent at age 9 years. It is worth noting that the stability effects were relatively larger than the cross-lagged effects.

Higher ADHD symptoms at both assessment points were associated with male sex and the presence of maternal depression. ADHD symptoms at age 5 years were additionally associated with the presence of a physical disability, lower maternal age, and lower socioeconomic status.

Across both assessment points, higher rates of maltreatment were associated with lower maternal age and presence of maternal depression. Maltreatment extent at age 5 years were additionally associated with male sex and lower socioeconomic status. Maltreatment extent at age 9 years was additionally associated with the absence of low birth weight and lower child age (see Table 2).

Indirect effects

The effect of negative emotionality at age 12 months on ADHD symptoms at age 9 years was mediated by the extent of childhood maltreatment at age 5 years. The effect of negative emotionality at age 12 months on childhood maltreatment extent at age 9 years was mediated by ADHD symptoms at age 5 years.

Negative emotionality at age 12 months further had an indirect effect on ADHD symptoms at age 9 years via ADHD symptoms at age 5 years and an indirect effect on the extent of childhood maltreatment at age 9 years via childhood maltreatment at age 5 years. Indirect effects are shown in Figure 1 and reported in Table 3.

Table 3. Indirect effects

Note. SE = standard error.

Discussion

We hypothesized a direct effect of negative emotionality on the extent of childhood maltreatment and ADHD symptoms, a bidirectional relationship between childhood maltreatment extent and ADHD symptoms, and indirect effects between negative emotionality and i) ADHD symptoms and ii) the extent of childhood maltreatment via i) childhood maltreatment extent and ii) ADHD symptoms. All our hypotheses were confirmed, apart from hypothesis 1a. Negative emotionality did predict ADHD symptoms at age 5, but not 9 years. However, the effect of negative emotionality on ADHD symptoms at 9 years was partially mediated by ADHD symptoms at 5 years.

The main finding from this study is that negative emotionality at age 12 months has an indirect effect on ADHD symptoms at age 9 that is mediated by the extent of maltreatment at age 5, i.e., children who show higher negative emotionality at age 12 months are more likely to experience higher rates of maltreatment at age 5, which in turn increases ADHD symptoms at age 9.

Moreover, infant negative emotionality predicted both ADHD symptoms and child maltreatment extent at age 5 directly, i.e., children with high negative emotionality have a higher risk of experiencing higher rates of maltreatment. Dinkler and colleagues (2017) suggested that higher maltreatment rates in children with neurodevelopmental disorders could be explained by genetically influenced traits that increase a child’s risk of maltreatment by a parent. Negative emotionality shares genetic risk with ADHD (Singh & Waldman, Reference Singh and Waldman2010) and might be an early sign or form of externalizing disorder, such as ADHD (Healey et al., Reference Healey, Marks and Halperin2011). Parent mental ill-health (Ayers et al., Reference Ayers, Bond, Webb, Miller and Bateson2019) and emotion regulation difficulties (Wang, Reference Wang2022) are associated with childhood maltreatment. It is possible that parents with ADHD (or at least high impulsivity) are more likely to use harmful conflict tactics, especially in response to a child with behavioral and emotional difficulties (e.g., negative emotionality), and their children are likely to inherit an increased vulnerability to negative emotionality and to later ADHD symptoms (Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Banaschewski, Coghill, Zheng, Biederman, Bellgrove, Newcorn, Gignac, Al Saud, Manor, Rohde, Yang, Cortese, Almagor, Stein, Albatti, Aljoudi, Alqahtani, Asherson and Wang2021; Faraone & Larsson, Reference Faraone and Larsson2019). However, while genetic risk and childhood maltreatment both increase the risk of developing ADHD independently, no interaction between the two has been found, i.e., having a high genetic risk for ADHD and experiencing childhood maltreatment does not increase the risk to develop ADHD over and above the risk posed by each of these factors (He & Li, Reference He and Li2022). The recently proposed double-jeopardy model argues that both, experiences of maltreatment and the presence of a neurodevelopmental disorder such as ADHD, increase the risk for adverse health outcomes, potentially via changes to the stress system – a shared risk pathway (Gajwani & Minnis, Reference Gajwani and Minnis2022). While the effect is small, we were able to show that infant negative emotionality has an enduring effect on childhood maltreatment extent and ADHD symptoms in middle childhood.

The cross-lagged findings show that a small, mutual influence between ADHD symptoms and maltreatment rates over time can be assumed, i.e., controlling for important other risk factors, having higher ADHD symptoms at age 5 increases the risk for a child to be maltreated at age 9 and being maltreated at age 5 leads to higher ADHD symptoms at age 9. Associations between maltreatment and a later, increased risk of ADHD have been shown repeatedly (Craig et al., Reference Craig, Bondi, O’Donnell, Pepler and Weiss2020; Kreppner et al., Reference Kreppner, O’Connor and Rutter2001; Sanderud et al., Reference Sanderud, Murphy and Elklit2016; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Kennedy, Kumsta, Knights, Golm, Rutter, Maughan, Schlotz and Kreppner2017).

Our finding regarding ADHD as a risk factor for maltreatment is consistent with results from other studies. For instance, a large longitudinal study (Lugo-Candelas et al., Reference Lugo-Candelas, Corbeil, Wall, Posner, Bird, Canino, Fisher, Suglia and Duarte2020) on 5–15-year-old Puerto Rican children (N = 2,491) showed that ADHD predicted subsequent adverse experiences, particularly parental maladjustment – an effect which was driven by inattentive symptoms. A recent follow-up from the ERA Study found that experiences of profound early neglect predicted symptoms of ADHD which in turn predicted bullying victimization (Rizeq et al., Reference Rizeq, Kennedy, Kreppner, Maughan and Sonuga-Barke2022). Taken together, it seems as if ADHD can set children up for adverse experiences within and outside of the family context.

Our results show that the relationship is mutual. The bidirectional relationship between maltreatment experiences and ADHD symptoms might pose a vicious cycle where the severity of ADHD symptoms is maintained through recent adverse experiences.

Not unexpectedly, the largest effects we found in our cross-lagged path model were that ADHD symptoms at age 5 predicted ADHD symptoms at age 9, and the same was true for the extent of maltreatment. This is in line with previous literature and the size of the effects is comparable to results from other cohort studies (Bowling et al., Reference Bowling, Tiemeier, Jaddoe, Barker and Jansen2018; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Shi, Wang, Li, Wang, Jiang and Fan2021). Previous studies have shown that at least a subset of children with ADHD symptoms has a consistent trajectory of either stable or increasing ADHD symptoms across development (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Booth, Eisner, Auyeung, Murray and Ribeaud2019; Walton et al., Reference Walton, Pingault, Cecil, Gaunt, Relton, Mill and Barker2017). High vulnerability and stability regarding ADHD have been linked to genetic (Thapar, Reference Thapar2018), epigenetic (Walton et al., Reference Walton, Pingault, Cecil, Gaunt, Relton, Mill and Barker2017), and environmental factors (Langberg et al., Reference Langberg, Epstein, Altaye, Molina, Arnold and Vitiello2008; Sasser et al., Reference Sasser, Kalvin and Bierman2016). It is therefore not surprising that ADHD scores are correlated substantially across development.

In a similar manner, parenting styles are relatively consistent over time (Dallaire & Weinraub, Reference Dallaire and Weinraub2005; Wittig & Rodriguez, Reference Wittig and Rodriguez2019), and parents who experienced abuse in their upbringing are more likely to show abusive or neglectful behavior (Greene et al., Reference Greene, Haisley, Wallace and Ford2020; van IJzendoorn et al., Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Coughlan and Reijman2020). Our results show that rates of maltreatment practices also remained stable over time. This may not be surprising, considering that a parent would have to change their approach to conflict and would have to learn and apply a different strategy to solve the conflict.

It should also be noted that the contemporaneous association between the extent of maltreatment experiences and ADHD symptoms was larger than the cross-lagged association over time. This finding is consistent with a recency model where more proximal adverse experiences are thought to have a larger impact on development (Gabard-Durnam & McLaughlin, Reference Gabard-Durnam and McLaughlin2019). This assumption has been examined on a large sample from the Avon Longitudinal Study (N = 7476). The authors tested how well different theoretical models explained the association between different types of adversity and emotional and behavioral problems. The association between abuse and psychopathology was best accounted for by a recency model rather than a sensitive developmental period model (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Crawford, Soare, Button, Raffeld, Smith, Penton-Voak and Munafò2018).

Some of the included covariates seem to pose additional robust risk factors as they increased either maltreatment or ADHD risk across both time points. For ADHD, those risk factors were maternal depression and male sex. Both factors have been included in a risk calculator for adult ADHD (Caye et al., Reference Caye, Agnew-Blais, Arseneault, Gonçalves, Kieling, Langley, Menezes, Moffitt, Passos, Rocha, Sibley, Swanson, Thapar, Wehrmeister and Rohde2019). Our data showed that these factors already predict ADHD symptom load across childhood.

In line with previous findings, younger maternal age (Dixon et al., Reference Dixon, Browne and Hamilton-Giachritsis2005; de Paúl & Domenech, Reference de Paúl and Domenech2000) and maternal depression diagnosis (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Houts, Arseneault, Pariante, Sikkema and Moffitt2019; Stith et al., Reference Stith, Liu, Davies, Boykin, Alder, Harris, Som, McPherson and Dees2009) increased the risk for maltreatment across both time points.

It would therefore be vital not only to focus on risk calculators for psychopathology (Caye et al., Reference Caye, Agnew-Blais, Arseneault, Gonçalves, Kieling, Langley, Menezes, Moffitt, Passos, Rocha, Sibley, Swanson, Thapar, Wehrmeister and Rohde2019; Meehan et al., Reference Meehan, Latham, Arseneault, Stahl, Fisher and Danese2020), but on risk calculators for transdiagnostic risk factors, such as childhood maltreatment. We were able to show that negative emotionality in infancy, ADHD symptoms in middle childhood, maternal depression, and age should be included in such a risk calculator.

Some of the included risk factors may be correlated. A study using data from the Avon Longitudinal Study showed that the presence of maternal depression was associated with the presence of other risk factors including younger maternal age and domestic violence. Maternal depression may hence increase the child’s cumulative risk exposure, i.e., maternal depression increases the vulnerability of a child to develop a mental health problem including ADHD (Barker et al., Reference Barker, Copeland, Maughan, Jaffee and Uher2012).

The current policies react to childhood maltreatment by temporarily or permanently placing children into out-of-home care (Department of Education, 2021; Drake et al., Reference Drake, Fluke, Kim, Orsi and Stubblefield2022; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Morelli, Garcia, Duong, Evans, Litrownik and Villodas2022). In addition, it would be desirable to also focus efforts on the prevention of childhood maltreatment in the first place as this would considerably reduce the global mental health burden. A meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies, for instance, was able to demonstrate that a reduction of maltreatment by 25% could reduce global cases of anxiety and depression by 80 million (Li et al., Reference Li, D’Arcy and Meng2016). Therefore, a variety of prevention and treatment policies and programs might be needed to address the issue more effectively (Magruder et al., Reference Magruder, McLaughlin and Elmore Borbon2017).

The results from our study could inform health professionals who regularly visit new parents at home (i.e., health visitors in the UK) about potential additional support needs of parents from social, educational, or health services.

For instance, screening parents for depressive symptoms and stress levels (i.e., in reaction to an infant’s temperament) might be helpful for identifying parents in need of support. Whether or not this would have an actual impact on parent behavior and/or potential risks for children would need to be evaluated in future studies though.

Our findings further highlight the bivariate relationship between maltreatment and ADHD symptoms. This supports previous findings that found an effect of childhood maltreatment on ADHD symptoms (Capusan et al., Reference Capusan, Kuja-Halkola, Bendtsen, Viding, McCrory, Marteinsdottir and Larsson2016; Dinkler et al., Reference Dinkler, Lundström, Gajwani, Lichtenstein, Gillberg and Minnis2017; Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Kreppner, Knights, Kumsta, Maughan, Golm, Rutter, Schlotz and Sonuga-Barke2016; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Kennedy, Kumsta, Knights, Golm, Rutter, Maughan, Schlotz and Kreppner2017), and findings that children with ADHD are at a higher risk for maltreatment (Hellstrom, Reference Hellstrom2019; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Agnew-Blais, Danese, Fisher, Jaffee, Matthews, Polanczyk and Arseneault2018). Overall, the results are in line with recommendations for early identification of ADHD and parenting support in maltreated children (Minnis, Reference Minnis2013) or assessments of adverse experiences as part of ADHD assessments (Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Kennedy, Kumsta, Knights, Golm, Rutter, Maughan, Schlotz and Kreppner2017). It should be emphasized however that ADHD symptoms are represented on a continuum (McLennan, Reference McLennan2016; Sonuga-Barke et al., Reference Sonuga-Barke, Becker, Bölte, Castellanos, Franke, Newcorn, Nigg, Rohde and Simonoff2022) and that higher rates of these symptoms do not equal a clinical presentation. Symptoms similar to ADHD can further be observed in trauma-related conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder including restlessness or hyperactivity (Cohen, Reference Cohen2010).

Strengths and limitations

While this study had clear strengths including the longitudinal design, large sample size, and control for relevant confounders, it had a few limitations. The FFCWS did not assess ADHD symptoms or maltreatment at the 12 months follow-up, nor did it assess ADHD symptoms or diagnoses in caregivers at any time point. Hence, it was not possible to control for these influences at the 12 months timepoint.

It stands to question however whether ADHD symptoms can be reliably assessed in infancy. Whereas a previous study reported acceptable psychometric properties in 2-year-old children (Brown & Harvey, Reference Brown and Harvey2019), comparable data on 12 months olds is missing. While it is possible to assess maltreatment in infants, the prevalence of maltreatment in children below 12 months is with 0.2% very low (Shanahan et al., Reference Shanahan, Austin and Berkoff2022). Given that the FFCWS study includes quite substantive assessments, it is understandable that less priority was given to the assessment of maltreatment in infancy.

Given the high heritability of ADHD (Faraone & Larsson, Reference Faraone and Larsson2019) it is further likely that parental ADHD affects ADHD related temperament dimensions in their offspring. Indeed, it has been shown that ADHD symptoms in mothers (Sullivan et al., Reference Sullivan, Holton, Nousen, Barling, Sullivan, Propper and Nigg2015) and fathers (Auerbach et al., Reference Auerbach, Berger, Atzaba-Poria, Arbelle, Cypin, Friedman and Landau2008) are associated with temperamental traits in infants. We therefore recommend that future birth cohort studies assess parental ADHD symptoms at baseline. In conjunction with that, maltreatment experiences of parents should be assessed as those have been identified as an antecedent of offspring maltreatment (van IJzendoorn et al., Reference van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Coughlan and Reijman2020).

Conclusion

The bidirectional relationship between ADHD and experiences of maltreatment highlights the need to identify early shared risk factors to prevent negative downstream effects of maltreatment and ADHD symptoms. Understanding these risk factors would enable social and clinical services to better support families at risk. Our study on a large longitudinal sample showed that infant negative emotionality, poses one of these shared risk factors.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000457

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare none.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the child behavior checklist/2-3 and 1992 profile. Dept. of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Pub.Google Scholar
Auerbach, J. G., Berger, A., Atzaba-Poria, N., Arbelle, S., Cypin, N., Friedman, A., & Landau, R. (2008). Temperament at 7, 12, and 25 months in children at familial risk for ADHD. Infant and Child Development, 17(4), 321338. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.579 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayers, S., Bond, R., Webb, R., Miller, P., & Bateson, K. (2019). Perinatal mental health and risk of child maltreatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 98, 104172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, E. D., Copeland, W., Maughan, B., Jaffee, S. R., & Uher, R. (2012). Relative impact of maternal depression and associated risk factors on offspring psychopathology. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 200(2), 124129.10.1192/bjp.bp.111.092346CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, A., & Malmberg, L.-E. (2019). A vicious or auspicious cycle: The reciprocal relation between harsh parental discipline and children’s self-regulation. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(3), 302317. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1399875 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowling, A. B., Tiemeier, H. W., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Barker, E. D., & Jansen, P. W. (2018). ADHD symptoms and body composition changes in childhood: A longitudinal study evaluating directionality of associations. Pediatric Obesity, 13(9), 567575. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijpo.12288 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, H. R., & Harvey, E. A. (2019). Psychometric properties of ADHD symptoms in toddlers. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 48(3), 423439. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1485105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capusan, A. J., Kuja-Halkola, R., Bendtsen, P., Viding, E., McCrory, E., Marteinsdottir, I., & Larsson, H. (2016). Childhood maltreatment and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adults: A large twin study. Psychological Medicine, 46(12), 26372646. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716001021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caye, A., Agnew-Blais, J., Arseneault, L., Gonçalves, H., Kieling, C., Langley, K., Menezes, A. M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Passos, I. C., Rocha, T. B., Sibley, M. H., Swanson, J. M., Thapar, A., Wehrmeister, F., & Rohde, L. A. (2019). A risk calculator to predict adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Generation and external validation in three birth cohorts and one clinical sample. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, e37. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796019000283 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, K. W., Houts, R., Arseneault, L., Pariante, C., Sikkema, K. J., & Moffitt, T. E. (2019). Maternal depression in the intergenerational transmission of childhood maltreatment and its sequelae: Testing postpartum effects in a longitudinal birth cohort. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 143156. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000032 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309319. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, K., Lee, J. B., Cheung, K., Theule, J., & Henrikson, B. (2018). Quantifying the relationship between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and experiences of child maltreatment: A meta-analysis. Child Abuse Review, 27(5), 361377. https://doi.org/10.1002/car.2530 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. A. (2010). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(4), 414430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2009.12.020 Google ScholarPubMed
Craig, S. G., Bondi, B. C., O’Donnell, K. A., Pepler, D. J., & Weiss, M. D. (2020). ADHD and exposure to maltreatment in children and youth: A systematic review of the past 10 years. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22(12), 79. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-01193-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallaire, D. H., & Weinraub, M. (2005). The stability of parenting behaviors over the first 6 years of life. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20(2), 201219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2005.04.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Paúl, J., & Domenech, L. (2000). Childhood history of abuse and child abuse potential in adolescent mothers: A longitudinal study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24(5), 701713. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00124-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinkler, L., Lundström, S., Gajwani, R., Lichtenstein, P., Gillberg, C., & Minnis, H. (2017). Maltreatment-associated neurodevelopmental disorders: A co-twin control analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 58(6), 691701. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12682 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, L., Browne, K., & Hamilton-Giachritsis, C. (2005). Risk factors of parents abused as children: A mediational analysis of the intergenerational continuity of child maltreatment (Part I). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(1), 4757. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00339.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, B., Fluke, J. D., Kim, H., Orsi, R., & Stubblefield, J. L. (2022). What proportion of foster care children do not have child protective services reports? A preliminary look. Child Maltreatment, 27(4), 596604. https://doi.org/10.1177/10775595211033855 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, E. C., Crawford, K. M., Soare, T. W., Button, K. S., Raffeld, M. R., Smith, A. D. A. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2018). Exposure to childhood adversity and deficits in emotion recognition: Results from a large, population-based sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 59(8), 845854. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12881 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, S. V., Banaschewski, T., Coghill, D., Zheng, Y., Biederman, J., Bellgrove, M. A., Newcorn, J. H., Gignac, M., Al Saud, N. M., Manor, I., Rohde, L. A., Yang, L., Cortese, S., Almagor, D., Stein, M. A., Albatti, T. H., Aljoudi, H. F., Alqahtani, M. M. J., Asherson, P., …, & Wang, Y. (2021). The World Federation of ADHD International Consensus Statement: 208 Evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 128, 789818, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, S. V., & Larsson, H. (2019). Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(4), Article4. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0070-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabard-Durnam, L. J., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Do sensitive periods exist for exposure to adversity? Biological Psychiatry, 85(10), 789791. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.03.975 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gajwani, R., & Minnis, H. (2022). Double jeopardy: Implications of neurodevelopmental conditions and adverse childhood experiences for child health. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-022-02081-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiser, C. (2012). Linear structural equation models. In Data analysis with Mplus. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Greene, C. A., Haisley, L., Wallace, C., & Ford, J. D. (2020). Intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review of the parenting practices of adult survivors of childhood abuse, neglect, and violence. Clinical Psychology Review, 80, 101891. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101891 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, Q., & Li, J. J. (2022). A gene-environment interaction study of polygenic scores and maltreatment on childhood ADHD. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 50(3), 309319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00873-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healey, D. M., Marks, D. J., & Halperin, J. M. (2011). Examining the interplay among negative emotionality, cognitive functioning, and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom severity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 17(3), 502510. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000294 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hellstrom, L. (2019). A systematic review of polyvictimization among children with attention deficit hyperactivity or autism spectrum disorder. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(13), 2280. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16132280 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, K., Morelli, N. M., Garcia, J., Duong, J. B., Evans, M. C., Litrownik, A. J., & Villodas, M. T. (2022). Trajectories of adolescent psychopathology among youth who were maltreated and placed in out-of-home care. Child Abuse & Neglect, 128, 105589. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105589 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, M., Kreppner, J., Knights, N., Kumsta, R., Maughan, B., Golm, D., Rutter, M., Schlotz, W., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2016). Early severe institutional deprivation is associated with a persistent variant of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Clinical presentation, developmental continuities and life circumstances in the English and Romanian Adoptees study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(10), 11131125. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12576 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Mroczek, D., Ustun, B., & Wittchen, H.-U. (1998). The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview short-form (CIDI-SF). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 7(4), 171185. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.47 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kostyrka-Allchorne, K., Wass, S. V., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2020). Research Review: Do parent ratings of infant negative emotionality and self-regulation predict psychopathology in childhood and adolescence? A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(4), 401416. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreppner, J. M., O’Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., & English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team (2001). Can inattention/overactivity be an institutional deprivation syndrome? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 513528, https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012229209190,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langberg, J. M., Epstein, J. N., Altaye, M., Molina, B. S. G., Arnold, L. E., & Vitiello, B. (2008). The transition to middle school is associated with changes in the developmental trajectory of ADHD symptomatology in young adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology: The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division, 37(3), 651663. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410802148095 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, M., D’Arcy, C., & Meng, X. (2016). Maltreatment in childhood substantially increases the risk of adult depression and anxiety in prospective cohort studies: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and proportional attributable fractions. Psychological Medicine, 46(4), 717730. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002743 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lugo-Candelas, C., Corbeil, T., Wall, M., Posner, J., Bird, H., Canino, G., Fisher, P. W., Suglia, S. F., & Duarte, C. S. (2020). ADHD and risk for subsequent adverse childhood experiences: Understanding the cycle of adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(8), 971978. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13352 and Allied Disciplines.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magruder, K. M., McLaughlin, K. A., & Elmore Borbon, D. L. (2017). Trauma is a public health issue. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 8(1), 1375338. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2017.1375338 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathiesen, K., & Tambs, K. (1999). The EAS temperament questionnaire—Factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 40(3), 431439. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00460 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLennan, J. D. (2016). Understanding attention deficit hyperactivity disorder as a continuum. Canadian Family Physician, 62(12), 979982.Google ScholarPubMed
Meehan, A. J., Latham, R. M., Arseneault, L., Stahl, D., Fisher, H. L., & Danese, A. (2020). Developing an individualized risk calculator for psychopathology among young people victimized during childhood: A population-representative cohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 262, 9098. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.10.034 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minnis, H. (2013). Maltreatment-associated psychiatric problems: An example of environmentally triggered ESSENCE? The Scientific World Journal, 2013, 15. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/148468 2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. L., Booth, T., Eisner, M., Auyeung, B., Murray, G., & Ribeaud, D. (2019). Sex differences in ADHD trajectories across childhood and adolescence. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12721. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12721 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables: User’s Guide (Version 8). Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Narusyte, J., Neiderhiser, J. M., D’Onofrio, B. M., Reiss, D., Spotts, E. L., Ganiban, J., & Lichtenstein, P. (2008). Testing different types of genotype-environment correlation: An extended children-of-twins model. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 15911603. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013911 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, J., Polanczyk, G. V., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2020). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet (London, England), 395(10222), 450462. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reichman, N. E., Teitler, J. O., Garfinkel, I., & McLanahan, S. S. (2001). Fragile families: Sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review, 23(4-5), 303326. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0190-7409(01)00141-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizeq, J., Kennedy, M., Kreppner, J., Maughan, B., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2022). Understanding the prospective associations between neuro-developmental problems, bullying victimization, and mental health: Lessons from a longitudinal study of institutional deprivation. Development and Psychopathology, 110. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942200089X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Kumsta, R., Schlotz, W., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2012). Longitudinal studies using a, natural experiment, design: The case of adoptees from Romanian Institutions. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(8), 762770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saisto, T., Salmela-Aro, K., Nurmi, J.-E., & HalmesmÄki, E. (2008). Longitudinal study on the predictors of parental stress in mothers and fathers of toddlers. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics & Gynecology, 29(3), 219228.10.1080/01674820802000467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanderud, K., Murphy, S., & Elklit, A. (2016). Child maltreatment and ADHD symptoms in a sample of young adults. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 7(1), 32061. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v7.32061 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasser, T. R., Kalvin, C. B., & Bierman, K. L. (2016). Developmental trajectories of clinically significant attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms from grade 3 through 12 in a high-risk sample: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(2), 207219. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000112 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanahan, M. E., Austin, A. E., & Berkoff, M. C. (2022). Prevalence of injuries among medicaid enrolled infants prior to child abuse and neglect. Child Maltreatment, 27(2), 218224. https://doi.org/10.1177/10775595211031651 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, A. L., & Waldman, I. D. (2010). The etiology of associations between negative emotionality and childhood externalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(2), 376388. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019342 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skarphedinsson, G., Jarbin, H., Andersson, M., & Ivarsson, T. (2021). Diagnostic efficiency and validity of the DSM-oriented Child Behavior Checklist and Youth Self-Report scales in a clinical sample of Swedish youth. PLoS One, 16(7), e0254953. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254953 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Kennedy, M., Kumsta, R., Knights, N., Golm, D., Rutter, M., Maughan, B., Schlotz, W., & Kreppner, J. (2017). Child-to-adult neurodevelopmental and mental health trajectories after early life deprivation: The young adult follow-up of the longitudinal English and Romanian Adoptees study. The Lancet, 389(10078), 15391548.10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30045-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Becker, S. P., Bölte, S., Castellanos, F. X., Franke, B., Newcorn, J. H., Nigg, J. T., Rohde, L. A., & Simonoff, E. (2022). Annual Research Review: Perspectives on progress in ADHD science - from characterization to cause. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(4), 506532. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13696 and Allied Disciplines.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, A., Agnew-Blais, J., Danese, A., Fisher, H. L., Jaffee, S. R., Matthews, T., Polanczyk, G. V., & Arseneault, L. (2018). Associations between abuse/neglect and ADHD from childhood to young adulthood: A prospective nationally-representative twin study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 81, 274285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.04.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stith, S. M., Liu, T., Davies, L. C., Boykin, E. L., Alder, M. C., Harris, J. M., Som, A., McPherson, M., & Dees, J. E. M. E. G. (2009). Risk factors in child maltreatment: A meta-analytic review of the literature. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14(1), 1329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2006.03.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Moore, D. W., & Runyan, D. (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child Conflict Tactics Scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22(4), 249270. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(97)00174-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, E. L., Holton, K. F., Nousen, E. K., Barling, A. N., Sullivan, C. A., Propper, C. B., & Nigg, J. T. (2015). Early identification of ADHD risk via infant temperament and emotion regulation: A pilot study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(9), 949957. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12426 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A. (2018). Discoveries on the genetics of ADHD in the 21st century: New findings and their implications. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(10), 943950. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18040383 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Coughlan, B., & Reijman, S. (2020). Annual Research Review: Umbrella synthesis of meta-analyses on child maltreatment antecedents and interventions: Differential susceptibility perspective on risk and resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 272290. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13147 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2006). Do early difficult temperament and harsh parenting differentially predict reactive and proactive aggression? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(5), 681691.10.1007/s10802-006-9055-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walton, E., Pingault, J.-B., Cecil, C.a M., Gaunt, T. R., Relton, C. L., Mill, J., & Barker, E. D. (2017). Epigenetic profiling of ADHD symptoms trajectories: A prospective, methylome-wide study. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(2), 250256. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2016.85 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, S., Shi, X., Wang, Z., Li, Z., Wang, A., Jiang, L., & Fan, F. (2021). Reciprocal relations between sleep and internalizing and externalizing problems: A cohort study of Chinese adolescents. Current Psychology, https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02373-4.Google Scholar
Wang, X. (2022). Intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment: The roles of parents’ emotion regulation and mentalization. Child Abuse & Neglect, 128, 104940. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2021.104940 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 490499. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittig, S. M. O., & Rodriguez, C. M. (2019). Emerging behavior problems: Bidirectional relations between maternal and paternal parenting styles with infant temperament. Developmental Psychology, 55(6), 11991210. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000707 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Coefficients represent standardized estimates (STDYX; * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001). Significant direct effects are represented by solid lines, insignificant effects are represented by dashed lines. Red lines represent significant indirect effects. Acronyms: ADHD symptoms: attention deficit/hyperactivity symptoms, Age C = Child age; Age M = Age of the mother; LBW = Low birth weight; MD = Maternal depression; PD = Physical disability; SES = Socio-economic status.

Figure 2

Table 2. Direct effects and correlations of variables of interests and covariates

Figure 3

Table 3. Indirect effects

Supplementary material: File

Golm and Brandt supplementary material

Golm and Brandt supplementary material

Download Golm and Brandt supplementary material(File)
File 27.7 KB