Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:48:36.624Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 January 2007

JON BROCK
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, UK

Abstract

Williams syndrome is a rare genetic disorder in which, it is claimed, language abilities are relatively strong despite mild to moderate mental retardation. Such claims have, in turn, been interpreted as evidence either for modular preservation of language or for atypical constraints on cognitive development. However, this review demonstrates that there is, in fact, little evidence that syntax, morphology, phonology, or pragmatics are any better than predicted by nonverbal ability, although performance on receptive vocabulary tests is relatively good. Similarly, claims of an imbalance between good phonology and impaired or atypical lexical semantics are without strong support. There is, nevertheless, consistent evidence for specific deficits in spatial language that mirror difficulties in nonverbal spatial cognition, as well as some tentative evidence that early language acquisition proceeds atypically. Implications for modular and neuroconstructivist accounts of language development are discussed.The preparation of this paper was supported by the Williams Syndrome Foundation (United Kingdom) and the Medical Research Council. I thank Courtenay Norbury and Kate Nation for comments on the manuscript and Chris Jarrold, Jill Boucher, and Teresa McCormack for many useful discussions.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., Evans, J., & Dolan, T. (2001). Theoretical perspectives on language and communication problems in mental retardation and developmental disabilities. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 7, 4555.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, E., & Spain, B. (1977). The child with spina bifida. London: Methuen.
Anderson, M. (1998). Mental retardation general intelligence and modularity. Learning and Individual Differences, 10, 159178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansari, D., Donlan, C., Thomas, M. S. C., Ewing, S. A., Peen, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). What makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 5062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, R., Yule, W., & Martin, N. (1985). The psychological characteristics of infantile hypercalcaemia: A preliminary investigation. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 27, 4959.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. (1991). Infants' contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development, 62, 875890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barisnikov, K., van der Linden, M., & Poncelet, M. (1996). Acquisition of new words and phonological working memory in Williams syndrome: A case study. Neurocase, 2, 395404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bearden, C. E., Wang, P. P., & Simon, T. J. (2002). Williams syndrome cognitive profile also characterizes velocardiofacial/DiGeorge syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 114, 689692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bello, A., Capirci, O., & Volterra, V. (2004). Lexical production in children with Williams syndrome: Spontaneous use of gesture in a naming task. Neuropsychologia, 42, 201213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 6(Suppl.), 115125.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., Lai, Z., & St. George, M. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl.), 729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Galaburda, A., & Korenberg, J. R. (1999). Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends in Neurosciences, 22, 197207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellugi, U., Marks, S., Bihrle, A., & Sabo, H. (1988). Dissociation between language and cognitive function in Williams Syndrome. In D. Bishop & K. Mogford (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
Bickerton, D. (1997). Constructivism, nativism, and explanatory adequacy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 557558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1989). Test for the reception of grammar (2nd ed.). Manchester: Chapel Press.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1997). Cognitive neuropsychology and developmental disorders: Uncomfortable bedfellows. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 899923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children's Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing the qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Böhning, M., Campbell, R., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Audiovisual speech perception in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 40, 13961406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bongiovanni, A. M., Eberlein, W. R., & Jones, I. T. (1957). Idiopathic hypercalcemia of infancy, with failure to thrive; report of three cases, with a consideration of the possible etiology. New England Journal of Medicine, 257, 951958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, J. (2002). Language and memory in Williams syndrome. Unpublished doctoral thesis. University of Warwick, Coventry, UK.
Brock, J., & Jarrold, C. (2005). Serial order reconstruction in Down syndrome: Evidence for a selective deficit in verbal short-term memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 304316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, J., Jarrold, C., Farran, E. K., & Laws, G. (2006, January). The relationship between vocabulary knowledge, age, and non-verbal ability: Evidence for contrasting profiles in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Poster presented at the Experimental Psychology Society, London.
Brock, J., McCormack, T., & Boucher, J. (2005). Probed serial recall in Williams syndrome: Lexical influences on phonological short-term memory. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, 360371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bromberg, H. S., Ullman, M., Marcus, G., Kelly, K. B., & Levine, K. (1995). A dissociation of lexical memory and grammar in Williams syndrome: Evidence from inflectional morphology. Genetic Counseling, 6, 166167.Google Scholar
Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Flanagan, T. D., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). On mosaics and melting pots: Conceptual considerations of comparison and matching strategies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 6573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Sabbadini, L., & Volterra, V. (1996). Language development in Williams syndrome: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 10171039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Clahsen, H., & Almazan, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Ring, M., & Temple, C. M. (2004). Lexical and morphological skills in English-speaking children with Williams syndrome. In S. Bartke & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 222244). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Clahsen, H., & Temple, C. M. (2003). Words and rules in children with William's syndrome. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 323352). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cornish, K. M., Munir, F., & Cross, G. (1999). Spatial cognition in males with Fragile-X syndrome: Evidence for a neuropsychological phenotype. Cortex, 35, 263271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., & Pietroski, P. (2002). Why language acquisition is a snap. The Linguistic Review, 19, 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dall'Oglio, A. M., & Milani, L. (1995). Analysis of the cognitive development in Italian children with Williams syndrome. Genetic Counseling, 6, 175176.Google Scholar
Donnai, D., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2000). Williams syndrome: From genotype through to the cognitive phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 97, 164171.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyle, T. F., Bellugi, U., Korenberg, J. R., & Graham, J. (2004). “Everybody in the world is my friend” hypersociability in young children with Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 124A, 263273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dykens, E. M. (1995). Measuring behavioral phenotypes: provocations from the “new genetics.” American Journal of Medical Genetics, 99, 522532.Google Scholar
Eckert, M. A., Hu, D., Eliez, S., Bellugi, U., Galaburda, A., Korenberg, J., et al. (2005). Evidence for superior parietal impairment in Williams syndrome. Neurology, 64, 152153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ejiri, K., & Masataka, N. (2001). Co-occurrence of preverbal vocal behavior and motor action in early infancy. Developmental Science, 4, 4048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, C. D. (1990). Differential abilities scales. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Elsabbagh, M., Cohen, H., Cohen, M., Rosen, S., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Speech perception in a population with ostensibly “intact” language. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Ewart, A. K., Morris, C. A., Atkinson, D., Jin, W. S., Sternes, K., Spallone, P., et al. (1993). Hemizygosity at the elastin locus in a developmental disorder, Williams syndrome. Nature Genetics, 5, 1116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farran, E. K., & Jarrold, C. (2003). Visuo-spatial cognition in Williams syndrome: Reviewing and accounting for strengths and weaknesses in performance. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 173200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farran, E. K., Jarrold, C., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Block design performance in the Williams syndrome phenotype: A problem with mental imagery? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 719728.Google Scholar
Farran, E. K., Jarrold, C., & Gathercole, S. E. (2003). Divided attention, selective attention and drawing: Processing preferences in Williams syndrome are dependent on the task administered. Neuropsychologia, 41, 676687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). MacArthur communicative development inventories: Technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (2000). The mind doesn't work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Galaburda, A. M., & Bellugi, U. (2000). V. Multi-level analysis of cortical neuroanatomy in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl. 1), 7488.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A. M., Holinger, D. P., Bellugi, U., & Sherman, G. F. (2002). Williams syndrome: Neuronal size and neuronal-packing density in primary visual cortex. Archives of Neurology, 59, 14611467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganger, J., & Brent, M.R. (2004). Reexamining the vocabulary spurt. Developmental Psychology, 40, 621632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge—It all depends on the nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 23, 8394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1987). The development of categorization in the second year and its relation to other cognitive and linguistic developments. Child Development, 58, 15231531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosch, A., Städing, G., & Pankau, R. (1994). Linguistic abilities in children with Williams-Beuren syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 52, 291296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, J., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Gathercole, S. A., Paterson, S., Howlin, P., Davies, M., et al. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and its relationship to language in Williams syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 8199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grant, J., Valian, V., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). A study of relative clauses in Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Langauge, 29, 403416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, V., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Funnell, E., & Tassabehji, M. (2006). In-depth analysis of spatial cognition in Williams syndrome: A critical assessment of the role of the LIMK1 gene. Neuropsychologia, 44, 679685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Györi, M., Lukács, Á., & Pléh, C. (2004). Towards the understanding of the neurogenesis of social cognition: Evidence from impaired populations. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 2, 261282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 15691579.Google Scholar
Holinger, D. P., Bellugi, U., Mills, D. L., Korenberg, J. R., Reiss, A. L., Sherman, G. F., et al. (2005). Relative sparing of primary auditory cortex in Williams Syndrome. Brain Research, 1037, 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howlin, P., Davies, M., & Udwin, O. (1998). Cognitive functioning in adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 183189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. A. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words—Evidence for a long-term-memory contribution to short-term-memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Hewes, A. K. (1998). Verbal and non-verbal abilities in the Williams syndrome phenotype: Evidence for diverging developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 511524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & Hewes, A. K. (1999). Genetically dissociated components of working memory: Evidence from Down's and Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 37, 637651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., & Brock, J. (2004). To match or not to match? Methodological issues in autism-related research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 8186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Cowan, N., Hewes, A. K., & Riby, D. M. (2004). Speech timing and verbal short-term memory: Evidence for contrasting deficits in Down syndrome and Williams syndrome. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 365380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Hartley, S. J., Phillips, C., & Baddeley, A. D. (2000). Word fluency in Williams syndrome: Evidence for unusual semantic organization. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 5, 293319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C., Purser, H. R. M., & Brock, J. (2006). Short-term memory in Down syndrome. In T. P. Alloway & S. Gathercole (Eds.), Working memory and neurodevelopmental conditions (pp. 239266). Hove: Psychology Press.
Johnson, S. C., & Carey, S. (1998). Knowledge enrichment and conceptual change in folkbiology: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognitive Psychology, 37, 156200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A., et al. (2000). Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 3046.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, W., Hesselink, J., Courchesne, E., Duncan, T., Matsuda, K., & Bellugi, U. (2002). Cerebellar abnormalities in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 688694.Google Scholar
Joseph, M. C., & Parrott, D. (1958). Severe infantile hypercalcaemia with specific reference to the facies. Archives of Diseases of Childhood, 33, 385395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D. M., & Newsome, M. (1999). The beginnings of word segmentation in English-learning infants. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 159207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff, K., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Pathways to language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 389398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., Berthoud, I., Davies, M., Howlin, P., & Udwin, O. (1997). Language and Williams syndrome: How intact is “intact”? Child Development, 68, 246262.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Scerif, G., & Thomas, M. (2002). Different approaches to relating genotype to phenotype in developmental disorders. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 311322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Thomas, M. (2003). What can developmental disorders tell us about the neurocomputational constraints that shape development? The case of Williams syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 969990.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Thomas, M., Annaz, D., Humphreys, K., Ewing, S., Brace, N., et al. (2004). Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: The importance of building developmental trajectories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 12581274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Tyler, L. K., Voice, K., Sims, K., Udwin, O., Howlin, P., et al. (1998). Linguistic dissociations in Williams syndrome: Evaluating receptive syntax in on-line and off-line tasks. Neuropsychologia, 36, 343351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kauffman, S. A. (1995). At home in the universe: The search for the laws of self-organization and complexity. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Klein, B. P., & Mervis, C. B. (1999). Contrasting patterns of cognitive abilities of 9- and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 177196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, M., & Penke, M. (2002). Inflectional morphology in German Williams syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 48, 410413.Google Scholar
Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsödl, M., Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., et al. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 5, 233246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laing, E., Grant, J., Thomas, M., Parmigiani, C., Ewing, S., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Love is … an abstract word: The influence of lexical semantics on verbal short-term memory in Williams syndrome. Cortex, 41, 169179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laing, E., Hulme, C., Grant, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Learning to read in Williams syndrome: Looking beneath the surface of atypical reading development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 729739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landau, B., & Zukowski, A. (2003). Objects, motions, and paths: Spatial language in children with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 105137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laws, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004a). A comparison of language abilities in adolescents with Down syndrome and children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 13241339.Google Scholar
Laws, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004b). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: A comparison with Down's syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 4564.Google Scholar
Levitin, D. J., Cole, K., Lincoln, A., & Bellugi, U. (2005). Aversion, awareness, and attraction: Investigating claims of hyperacusis in the Williams syndrome phenotype. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 514523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, Y. (1996). Modularity of language reconsidered. Brain and Language, 55, 240263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, Y., & Bechar, T. (2003). Cognitive, lexical and morpho-syntactic profiles of Israeli children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 39, 255271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, Y., & Hermon, S. (2003). Morphological abilities of Hebrew-speaking adolescents with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 5983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1993). The child's path to spoken language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Logie, R. H., Della Salla, S., Laiacona, M., Chambers, P., & Wynn, V. (1996). Group aggregates and individual reliability: The case of verbal short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 24, 305321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and developmental disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Lowery, M. C., Morris, C. A., Ewart, A., Brothman, L. J., Zhu, X. L., Leonard, C. O., et al. (1995). Strong correlation of elastin deletions, detected by FISH, with Williams syndrome. American Journal of Human Genetics, 57, 4953.Google Scholar
Lukács, A., Pléh, C., & Racsmány, M. (2004). Language in Hungarian children with Williams syndrome. In S. Bartke & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 187220). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Majerus, S. (2004). Phonological processing in Williams syndrome. In S. Bartke & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 125142). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Majerus, S., Barisnikov, K., Vuillemin, I., Poncelet, M., & Van der Linden, M. (2003). An investigation of verbal short-term memory and phonological processing in four children with Williams syndrome. Neurocase, 9, 390401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majerus, S., & Van der Linden, M. (2003). Long-term memory effects on verbal short-term memory: A replication study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 303310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marriage, J. E. (1996). Hyperacusis in Williams syndrome. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Manchester, UK.
Masataka, N. (2001). Why early linguistic milestones are delayed in children with Williams syndrome: Late onset of hand banging as a possible rate-limiting constraint on the emergence of canonical babbling. Developmental Science, 4, 158164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattys, S., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2001). Phonotactic cues for segmentation of fluent speech by infants. Cognition, 78, 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1994). Acquisition of the novel name-nameless category (N3C) principle. Child Development, 65, 16461662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Bertrand, J. (1997). Developmental relations between cognition and language. In L. B. Adamson & M. A. Romski (Eds.), Communication and language acquisition: Discoveries from atypical development. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2004). Methodological issues in group-matching designs: Alpha levels for control variable comparisons and measurement characteristics of control and target variables. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Bertrand, J., & Robinson, B. F. (1999). Williams syndrome: Findings from an integrated program of research. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders: Contributions to a new framework from the cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (2000). Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: A comparison. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 111126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Bertrand, J., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B. P., & Armstrong, S. C. (2000). The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain and Cognition, 44, 604628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Robinson, B. F., Rowe, M. L., Becerra, A. M., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2003). Language abilities of individuals with Williams syndrome. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 27, 3581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messer, D. J. (1994). The development of communication: From social interaction to language. New York: Wiley.
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Mervis, C. B., Sarpal, D., Koch, P., Steele, S., Kohn, P., et al. (2005). Functional, structural, and metabolic abnormalities of the hippocampal formation in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 115, 18881895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyerson, M. D., & Frank, R. A. (1987). Language, speech and hearing in Williams syndrome: Intervention approaches and research needs. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 258270.Google Scholar
Morris, C. A. (2006). The dysmorphology, genetics, and natural history of Williams–Beuren syndrome. In C. A. Morris, H. M. Lenhoff, & P. P. Wang (Eds.) Williams–Beuren syndrome: Research, evaluation, and treatment (pp. 317). Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.
Morris, C. A., Demsey, S. A., Leonard, C. O., Dilts, C., & Blackburn, B. L. (1988). Natural history of Williams syndrome: physical characteristics. Journal of Pediatrics, 113, 318326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, J., & Frith, U. (1993). What lessons for dyslexia from Down's syndrome? Comments on Cossu, Rossini, and Marshall. Cognition, 48, 289296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovitch, M., & Umilta, C. (1990). Modularity and neuropsychology: Modules and central processes in attention and memory. In M. Schwartz (Ed.), Modular deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia (pp. 159). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nazzi, T., & Bertoncini, J. (2003). Before and after the vocabulary spurt: Two modes of word acquisition? Developmental Science, 6, 136142.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Gopnik, A. (2001). Linguistic and cognitive abilities in infancy: When does language become a tool for categorization? Cognition, 80, B11B20.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Gopnik, A., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Asynchrony in the cognitive and lexical development of young children with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 32, 427438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Asynchrony in the cognitive and lexical development of young children with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 32, 427438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzi, T., Paterson, S., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Word segmentation by infants with Williams syndrome. Infancy, 4, 251271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(1–2, Serial No. 149).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neville, H. J., Mills, D.L., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Effects of altered auditory sensitivity and age of language acquisition on the development of language-relevant neural systems: Preliminary studies of Williams syndrome. In S. Broman (Ed.), Cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: Implications for brain function. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Norbury, C. F. (2004). Factors supporting idiom comprehension in children with communication disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 11791193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 142171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pagon, R. A., Bennett, F. C., LaVeck, B., Stewart, K. B., & Johnson, J. (1987). Williams syndrome: Features in late childhood and adolescence. Pediatrics, 80, 8591.Google Scholar
Paterson, S. J., Brown, J. H., Gsödl, M. K., Johnson, M. H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Cognitive modularity and genetic disorders. Science, 286, 23552358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paterson, S. J., Girelli, L., Butterworth, B., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Are numerical impairments syndrome specific? Evidence from Williams syndrome and Down's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 190204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penke, M., & Krause, M. (2004). Regular and irregular inflectional morphology in German Williams syndrome. In S. Bartke & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 245270). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Phillips, C. E., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Grant, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2004). Comprehension of spatial language terms in Williams syndrome: Evidence for an interaction between domains of strength and weakness. Cortex, 40, 85101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1963). Language et opérations intellectuelles [Language and intellectual operations]. In Collectif (Ed.), Problèmes de psycholinguistique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Piatelli-Palmarini, M. (2001). Speaking of learning: How do we acquire our marvellous facility for expressing ourselves in words? Nature, 411, 887888.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: Harper Perennial.
Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. T. (2002). The past and future of the past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pléh, C., Lukács, Á., & Racsmány, M. (2003). Morphological patterns in Hungarian children with Williams syndrome and the rule debates. Brain and Language, 86, 377383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & Rende, R. (1991). Human behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 161190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 275306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1993). Coloured progressive matrices. Oxford: Information Press Ltd.
Reilly, J., Harrison, D., & Klima, E. S. (1995). Emotional talk and talk about emotions. Genetic Counseling, 6, 158159.Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, A. L., Eliez, S., Schmitt, J. E., Straus, E., Lai, Z., Jones, W., et al. (2000). Neuroanatomy of Williams syndrome: A high-resolution MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 6573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ring, M., & Clahsen, H. (2005). Distinct patterns of language impairment in Down's syndrome and Williams syndrome: The case of syntactic chains. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 479501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, B. F., Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. W. (2003). The roles of verbal short-term memory and working memory in the acquisition of grammar by children with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 1331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1994). Exceptional cases of language development in mental retardation: the relative autonomy of language as a cognitive system. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Constraints on language acquisition: Studies of atypical children (pp. 155174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rossen, M., Klima, E. S., Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., & Jones, W. (1996). Interaction between language and cognition: Evidence from Williams syndrome. In J. H. Beitchman, N. Cohen, M. Konstantareas, & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological, and clinical perspectives (pp. 367392). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Rourke, B. P. (1987). Syndrome of non-verbal learning disabilities. The final common pathway of white matter disease/dysfunction. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 1, 209234.Google Scholar
Rustioni, D. (1994). Sviluppo della comprensione linguistics nei bambini iltaliani. Presentazione di una scala evolutiva [Development of grammar comprehension in Italian children. Presentation of an evolutionary scale]. In C. Cornoldi & R. Vianello (Eds.), Handicap e apprendimento (pp. 3553). Bergamo, Italy: Junior Ed.
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarborough, H. S., Rescorla, L., Tager-Flusberg, H., Fowler, A., & Sudhalter, V. (1991). The relation of utterance length to grammatical complexity in normal and language-disordered groups. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 2345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scerif, G., Cornish, K., Wilding, J., Driver, J., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2004). Visual search in typically developing toddlers and toddlers with Fragile X or Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 7, 116130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaner-Wolles, C. (2004). Spared domain-specific cognitive capacities? In S. Bartke & J. Siegmüller (Eds.), Williams syndrome across languages (pp. 93124). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Schmitt, J. E., Eliez, S., Warsofsky, I. S., Bellugi, U., & Reiss, A. L. (2001). Corpus callosum morphology of Williams syndrome: Relation to genetics and behavior. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 155159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt, J. E., Watts, K., Eliez, S., Bellugi, U., Galaburda, A. M., & Reiss, A. L. (2002). Increased gyrification in Williams syndrome: Evidence using 3D MRI methods. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 292295.Google Scholar
Scott, P., Mervis, C. B., Bertrand, J., Klein, B. P., Armstrong, S. C., & Ford, A. L. (1995). Semantic organization and word fluency in 9- and 10-year-old children with Williams syndrome. Genetic Counseling, 6, 172173.Google Scholar
Seashore, H. E. (1951). Differences between verbal and performance IQs on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 15, 6267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semel, E., & Rosner, S. R. (2003). Understanding Williams syndrome: Behavioral patterns and interventions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Shaffer, T. M., & Ehri, L. C. (1980). Seriators' and nonseriators' comprehension of comparative adjective forms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 9, 187204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair de Zwart, H. (1967). Verbal behavior and operational deficits. Acta Neurologica et Psychiatrica Belgica, 67, 852860.Google Scholar
Singer-Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 345370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowling, M., Van Wagtendonk, B., & Stafford, C. (1988). Object-naming deficits in developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 11, 6785CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stojanovik, V., Perkins, M., & Howard, S. (2001). Language and conversational abilities in Williams syndrome: How good is good? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 234239.Google Scholar
Stojanovik, V., Perkins, M., & Howard, S. (2004). Williams syndrome and specific language impairment do not support claims for developmental double dissociations and innate modularity. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 403424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, M. (2001). Demonstrating specific cognitive deficits: A psychometric perspective. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strømme, P., Bjørnstad, P.G., & Ramstad, K. (2002). Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 269271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Can adolescents with Williams syndrome tell the difference between lies and jokes? Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 85103.Google Scholar
Swisher, L., & Pinkser, E. (1971). The language characteristics of hyperverbal hydrocephalic children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 13, 746755.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Plesa-Skwerer, D., Faja, S., & Joseph, R. M. (2003). People with Williams syndrome process faces holistically. Cognition, 89, 1124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 5990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, C. M., Almazan, M., & Sherwood, S. (2002). Lexical skills in Williams Syndrome: A cognitive neuropsychological analysis. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 463495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, C. M., & Clahsen, H. (2002). How connectionist simulations fail to account for developmental disorders in children. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 769770.Google Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C. (2005a). Constraints on language development: Insights from developmental disorders. In P. Fletcher & J. Miller (Eds.), Language disorders and developmental theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Thomas, M. S. (2005b). Characterising compensation. (Commentary on Ullman and Pierpont, “Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis”). Cortex, 41, 434442.Google Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., Dockrell, J. E., Messer, D., Parmigiani, C., Ansari, D., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). Speeded naming, frequency and the development of the lexicon in Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 721759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., Grant, J., Barham, Z., Gsödl, M. K., Laing, E., Lakusta, L., et al. (2001). Past tense formation in Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 143176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2003). Modeling language acquisition in atypical phenotypes. Psychological Review, 110, 647682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2005). Can developmental disorders reveal the component parts of the language faculty? Language Learning and Development, 1, 6592.Google Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., & Richardson, F. M. (2006). Atypical representational change: Conditions for the emergence of atypical modularity. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development: Vol. XXI. Attention and performance XXI (pp. 315347). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Thompson, P. M., Lee, A. D., Dutton, R. A., Geaga, J. A., Hayashi, K. M., Eckert, M. A., et al. (2005). Abnormal cortical complexity and thickness profiles mapped in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 41464158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, L. K., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Voice, J. K., Stevens, T., Grant, J., Udwin, O., et al. (1997). Do individuals with Williams syndrome have bizarre semantics? Evidence for lexical organization using an on-line task. Cortex, 33, 515527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Udwin, O., Davies, M., & Howlin, P. (1996). A longitudinal study of cognitive abilities and educational attainment in Williams syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 38, 10201029.Google Scholar
Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1990). Expressive language of children with Williams syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 6(Suppl.), 108114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1991). A cognitive and behavioral phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 232244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Udwin, O., Yule, W., & Martin, N. (1987). Cognitive abilities and behavioural characteristics of children with idiopathic infantile hypercalcemia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 297309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Borsel, J., Curfs, L. M. G., & Fryns, J. P. (1997). Hyperacusis in Williams syndrome: A sample survey study. Genetic Counselling, 8, 121126.Google Scholar
Vicari, S., Bates, E., Caselli, M. C., Pasqualetti, P., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F., & Volterra, V. (2004). Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: An example of a dissociation between language and cognition? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 862876.Google Scholar
Vicari, S., Carlesimo, G., Brizzolara, D., & Pezzini, G. (1996). Short term memory in children with Williams syndrome: A reduced contribution of lexical-semantic knowledge to word span. Neuropsychologia, 34, 919925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicari, S., Caselli, M. C., Gagliardi, C., Tonucci, F., & Volterra, V. (2002). Language acquisition in special populations: A comparison between Down and Williams syndromes. Neuropsychologia, 40, 24612470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Pezzini, G., Sabbadini, L., & Vicari, S. (1996). Linguistic abilities in Italian children with Williams syndrome. Cortex, 32, 663677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Von Arnim, G., & Engel, P. (1964). Mental retardation related to hypercalcaemia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 6, 366377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, P. P., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual-spatial short-term memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 16, 317322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. C., Barratt-Boyes, B. G., & Lowe, J. B. (1961). Supravalvular aortic stenosis. Circulation, 24, 13111318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (Eds.). (1990). Woodcock–Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery—Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Zukowski, A. (2001). Uncovering grammatical competence in children with Williams syndrome. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Boston University.
Zukowski, A. (2004). Investigating knowledge of complex syntax: Insights from experimental studies of Williams syndrome. In M. Rice & S. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Zukowski, A. (2005). Knowledge of constraints on compounding in children and adolescents with Williams syndrome. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, 7992.CrossRefGoogle Scholar