Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:00:15.287Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 October 2008

Peter Mundy*
Affiliation:
University of Miami
Marian Sigman
Affiliation:
University of California at Los Angeles
Connie Kasari
Affiliation:
University of California at Los Angeles
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Peter Mundy, Director of the Psychological Services Center, Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL 33124

Abstract

Recent data suggest that a disturbance in the development of joint attention skills is a specific characteristic of young autistic children. This observation may have both theoretical and clinical significance. However, many pertinent issues remain to be addressed with regard to the parameters of joint attention disturbance in children with autism. This study attempted to address several of these issues. The study examines the effects of mental age and IQ on the joint attention skills of children with autism, mental retardation, and normal development. The study also examined the relation of joint attention behaviors to the parent's report of symptoms presented by children with autism. The results suggested that, differences in IQ and mental age may be related to differences in the type of joint attention skill deficits displayed by children with autism. The results also suggested that joint attention disturbance is associated with a circumscribed, but social cluster of symptoms observed among young autistic children by their parents. The implications of these findings for developmental models of autism are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 12781289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). Joint attention deficits in autism: Cognitive or affective. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). Perceptual role-taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months?: The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J., & Sherwood, V. (1983). Thought, language and interaction in infancy. In Call, J., Galeson, E., & Tyson, R. (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (pp. 3855). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 5572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capps, L., Kasari, C., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (in press). Parental perception of emotional expres-siveness in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Cattell, P. (1960). The intelligence of infants. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 282292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. (1983). The preverbal negotiation of failed messages: Insights in the transition period. In Golinkoff, R. (Ed.), The transition from prelinguis-tic to linguistic communication (pp. 5778). Hills-dale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hannan, T. (1987). A cross sequential assessment of the occurrences of pointing in 3- to 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 1122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, S., Handleman, J., Gordon, R., Kristoff, B., & Fuentes, F. (1991). Changes in cognitive and language functioning of preschool children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 281290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S., Collins, K., & Hong, H. (1991). An audience effect on smile production in ten-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 2, 4549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affect sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krug, D., Arick, J., & Almond, P. (1979). Autism screening instrument for educational planning: Background and development. In Gilliam, J. (Ed.), Autism: Diagnosis, instruction, management and research. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 412426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A., & Happe, E. (1989). Autism and ostensive communication: The relevance of metarepresentation. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 205212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewy, A., & Dawson, A. (1992). Social stimulation and joint attention in young autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 555566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loveland, K., & Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, R., Eichorn, D., & Hogarty, P. (1977). Transitions in early mental development. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 42(3), 171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEvoy, R., Rogers, S., & Pennington, R. (1993). Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 563578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Gopnick, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and the developing theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding others minds: Perspective from autism (pp. 335366). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, F. (1979). Interobserver agreement, reliability and generalizability of data collected in observational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 376390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., & Hogan, A. (1994). Intersubjectivity, joint attention and autistic developmental pathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 5. The self and its disorder (pp. 130). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., & Sigman, P. (1989a). Theoretical implications of joint attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., & Sigman, S. (1989b). Second thoughts on the nature of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 213218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Kasari, C., & Sigman, M. (1992). Joint attention, affective sharing, and intersubjectivity. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 377381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of A utism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1993). The autistic person's theory of mind and early nonverbal joint attention deficits. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., Cohen, D., & Volmar, F. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 181201). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Yirmiya, N. (1988). Nonverbal communication skills in Down syndrome children. Child Development, 59, 235249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of nonverbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 657669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 375384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynell, J. (1977). Reynell developmental language scales. Windsor, UK: NFER Publishing.Google Scholar
Rogers, S., & Pennington, B. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 137162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., & Garmezy, N. (1983). Developmental Psychopathology. In Herington, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 775911). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schopler, E., Reichler, R., & Renner, B. (1986). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale. New York: Irvington.Google Scholar
Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The Early Social-Communication Scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3, 244245.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1984). Mental age and cognitive stage in young handicapped and at-risk children. Intelligence, 8, 1129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1986). On the specifically cognitive nature of early object-social skill domain associations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 32, 2136.Google Scholar
Sheinkopf, S., & Siegle, B. (1994). Intensive behavior treatment of young autistic children. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992). Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sigman, M., Mundy, P., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Social interactions of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children and their caregivers. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry, 27, 647656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugarman, S. (1984). The development of preverbal communication. In Schiefelbusch, R. & Pickar, J. (Eds.), The acquisition of communicative competence. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Thorndike, R. (1972). Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale: Norms and tables. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (1988). Issues in the classification of infantile autism. In Lahey, B. & Kazdin, A. (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 135). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Volkmar, F., Cohen, D., Bregman, J., Hooks, M., & Stevenson, J. (1989). An examination of social typologies in autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 8286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherby, A., & Pruning, C. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 367377.Google ScholarPubMed
Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P., (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Development, 63, 150160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed