Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T23:21:04.305Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The heart of the matter: Developing the whole child through community resources and caregiver relationships

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 May 2021

Amanda Sheffield Morris*
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, Tulsa, OK, USA
Jennifer Hays-Grudo
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, Tulsa, OK, USA
Kara L. Kerr
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, Tulsa, OK, USA
Lana O. Beasley
Affiliation:
Oklahoma State University, Tulsa, OK, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Amanda Sheffield Morris, Oklahoma State University – Tulsa, 700 N Greenwood Ave, 2116 Main Hall, Tulsa, OK 74106; E-mail: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Numerous developmental scholars have been influenced by the research, policies, and thinking of the late Edward Zigler, who was instrumental in founding Head Start and Early Head Start. In line with the research and advocacy work of Zigler, we discuss two models that support the development of the whole child. We begin by reviewing how adverse and protective experiences “get under the skin” and affect developmental trajectories and risk and resilience processes. We then present research and examples of how experiences affect the whole child, the heart and the head (social, emotional, cognitive, and physical development), and consider development within context and across domains. We discuss examples of interventions that strengthen nurturing relationships as the mechanism of change. We offer a public health perspective on promoting optimal development through nurturing relationships and access to resources during early childhood. We end with a discussion of the myth that our current society is child-focused and argue for radical, essential change to make promoting optimal development for all children the cornerstone of our society.

Type
Special Issue Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

When I was in graduate school, I vividly remember sitting around a table one evening with fellow students answering a hypothetical question, “If you could change one thing in the world, say you were king or queen for a day, what would it be?” I responded without hesitation, “Every child in the world would have access to free, quality, early childhood education and care, starting at birth.”—Amanda Sheffield Morris

I was asked by a friend who had recently learned of the devastating consequences of adverse childhood experiences on individuals and society, “But isn't this overwhelming? Is there anything realistically that can be done about it?” My reply was immediate: “Provide home visiting or other support for parents of every child born in America. It's the hardest job we do and no one knows how to do it unless they were well-parented themselves.”—Jennifer Hays-Grudo

Many developmental scholars have been influenced by the research, policies, and thinking of the late Edward Zigler, who was instrumental in founding Head Start and Early Head Start programs. It is likely that Zigler's research and advocacy stemmed from his own childhood experiences. As the child of immigrants, he attended a program in Kansas City where he received resources such as dental care, English classes, nutritional meals, and social support (what we often call mental to dental when referring to interventions today). Such programming and experiences are in line with the concept of developing the whole child, with an emphasis on promoting social and emotional skills and relationships in addition to cognitive and physical health.

In reviewing the many decades of Zigler's life work, several clear themes emerge. First, there is a focus on family and parenting, recognizing the essential role of parents in the development of the child. This is illustrated in the two-generation approach that is the foundation of Head Start programming, recognizing that children cannot thrive when parents are suffering and lack the necessary supports and resources to be parents (Turner & Zigler, Reference Turner and Zigler1987). Second, early education and care should be of high quality and focus on multiple developmental domains (social, emotional, physical, cognitive) or the “whole child” (Raver & Zigler, Reference Raver and Zigler1997; Zigler & Bishop-Josef, Reference Zigler, Bishop-Josef, Singer, Golinkoff and Hirsh-Pasek2006). Zigler was critical of care that was merely custodial in nature and argued for enriching experiences for children across development, such as the 21st Century Schools model (Zigler, Reference Zigler1970, Reference Zigler1989). Finally, Zigler was a passionate advocate for children, applying developmental science to policies and programming at a national scale (Zigler & Styfco, Reference Zigler and Styfco2000). He was an advisor to every President from Lyndon B. Johnson to Barack Obama. When asked how he could reconcile his politics as the first director of the Office of Child Development with those of the Nixon Administration, which was not known for progressive social policies, his response was simple: “My politics are children” (Provocative Child Agency Head, 1970).

In the spirit of Ed Zigler and his work, we discuss two models that support the development of the whole child. We begin with a discussion of how adverse and protective experiences “get under the skin” and affect developmental trajectories, risk, and resilience. We present research and examples of how experiences affect the whole child, the heart and the head, and consider development within context and across domains. Next, we discuss examples of interventions that target nurturing relationships as a mechanism of change. We present a public health perspective on promoting optimal development by fostering nurturing relationships and providing resources during early childhood. We end with a discussion of Zigler's assertion that the idea that American society is child-focused is a myth, and argue for radical, essential change to promote optimal early child development for all children.

How Adverse and Protective Experiences “Get Under the Skin”

Social neuroscience supports the notion that humans are wired to connect (e.g., Lee, Qu, & Telzer, Reference Lee, Qu and Telzer2018). As a species, our survival is dependent on early emotional bonds that form between caregivers and young children. Humans have evolved to promote caregiver–child attachments at the social and neurobiological levels (Hays-Grudo, Ratliff, & Morris, Reference Hays-Grudo, Ratliff, Morris and Bensonin press). Across cultures, caregivers’ neurobiological and physical responses to children's cries are universal, and neuroimaging studies indicate that the parental brain undergoes structural changes after birth that help prepare parents for the demanding task of providing care for a newborn (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Leckman, Mayes, Feldman, Wang and Swain2010). Changes in these biological systems appear to increase parents’ responsiveness to infants and ability to manage caregiving stress (Swain, Kim, & Ho, Reference Swain, Kim and Ho2011).

During positive social interactions, dopamine and norepinephrine are released, reinforcing positive social behaviors. Attachment among infants and caregivers is influenced by such hormonal responses. Dopamine and oxytocin specifically affect the reward circuitry in the striatum, promoting physiological and behavioral synchrony and attunement (Feldman, Reference Feldman2017). In Caregiver×Child interactions, studies illustrate multiple types of synchrony: behavioral synchrony (e.g., shared gaze, touch), heart rate coupling, hormonal attunement (e.g., similar cortisol levels), and brain synchrony as evidenced by electroencephalography (EEG) patterns (Feldman, Reference Feldman2017). When behavioral and physiological synchrony are interrupted, as seen in the Still Face paradigm, infants and caregivers both become distressed (Weinberg & Tronick, Reference Weinberg and Tronick1996). Over time, asynchronous interactions can have negative effects on socioemotional development and attachment, affecting subsequent interactions. Interestingly, there is evidence for father–infant behavioral and physiological synchrony (even though most studies are with mothers), although patterns differ from mother–infant synchrony (Feldman, Reference Feldman2003). There is also evidence emerging that teachers and students display neurophysiological and behavioral synchrony (e.g., Bevilacqua et al., Reference Bevilacqua, Davidesco, Wan, Chaloner, Rowland, Ding and Dikker2019).

Biologically, we are programmed to form attachment bonds with caregivers, but as is described in the next section, these processes may often go awry, as can be seen in cases of child abuse and neglect. Much of the research on early life stress and adversity has focused on maltreatment and the negative effects of adversity rather than positive experiences. Nevertheless, there is an accompanying literature on resilience in developmental science that illustrates the effects of positive experiences on child development (see Hays-Grudo & Morris, Reference Hays-Grudo and Morris2020). It is beyond the scope of this manuscript to discuss all the potential positive factors that can influence children. Thus, we focus on a model that we developed to complement the research on adverse childhood experiences (ACEs; Anda et al., Reference Anda, Felitti, Bremner, Walker, Whitfield, Perry and Giles2006) discussed in the next section. We developed the protective and compensatory experiences (PACEs) framework to delineate ten specific experiences that children need for optimal development. These PACEs are grounded in developmental science (see Kentner, Scalia, Shin, Migliore, & Rondón-Ortiz, Reference Kentner, Scalia, Shin, Migliore and Rondón-Ortiz2020; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Treat, Hays-Grudo, Chesher, Williamson, Mendez, Morris and Williamson2018) and are similar to other lists of protective and resilience-promoting factors (Masten, Reference Masten2015). PACEs can be grouped into two categories: relationships and resources, and like the ten ACEs identified in the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) study, PACEs are experiences that occur prior to age 18. The relationship factors include: unconditional love from a primary caregiver; having a best friend; volunteering in the community; being part of a group; and having a mentor. The resource factors include: having a safe, clean home with enough food; getting a good education; having a hobby; getting plenty of physical activity; and having rules and routines. We acknowledge that not all of the PACEs are of equal influence, with unconditional love from a caregiver being most important. Moreover, at different developmental periods and in different contexts some PACEs may be more influential than others. In subsequent sections of the paper, we provide more information on how PACEs affect the development of the whole child, but first we briefly review the research on ACEs and child development.

Science of adversity

Developmental scientists have long studied the negative effects of adverse conditions on mental health and well-being (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti1984; Masten, Best, & Garmezy, Reference Masten, Best and Garmezy1990; Rutter, Reference Rutter1979, Reference Rutter1987; Sameroff, Seifer, Barocas, Zax, & Greenspan, Reference Sameroff, Seifer, Barocas, Zax and Greenspan1987). More recently, epidemiologic research provides evidence of the effects of ACEs on physical as well as mental health (Anda et al., Reference Anda, Felitti, Bremner, Walker, Whitfield, Perry and Giles2006; Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998). Analyses of extensive health records from more than 17,000 adult patients in the original Kaiser Permanente ACEs sample revealed that adults exposed to abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction were significantly more likely to experience chronic illnesses, mental health problems, and engage in health-harming behaviors. ACEs were common, occurring in a majority of patients, tended to co-occur, and had a cumulative or a dose–response effect on negative outcomes (Anda et al., Reference Anda, Felitti, Bremner, Walker, Whitfield, Perry and Giles2006; Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards and Marks1998). Subsequent research has replicated these findings in other populations around the world, supporting the negative effects of childhood trauma on developmental and health outcomes (Bellis, Lowey, Leckenby, Hughes, & Harrison, Reference Bellis, Lowey, Leckenby, Hughes and Harrison2014; Merrick, Ford, Ports, & Guinn, Reference Merrick, Ford, Ports and Guinn2018).

ACEs are also associated with school-aged children's health, developmental delays, and behavioral and learning problems (Bethell, Newacheck, Hawes, & Halfon, Reference Bethell, Newacheck, Hawes and Halfon2014; Burke, Hellman, Scott, Weems, & Carrion, Reference Burke, Hellman, Scott, Weems and Carrion2011). Even during infancy and toddlerhood, ACEs have damaging effects on children's social, emotional, and cognitive development. In a national study of families enrolled in Early Head Start (McKelvey, Edge, Mesman, Whiteside-Mansell, & Bradley, Reference McKelvey, Edge, Mesman, Whiteside-Mansell and Bradley2018), ACEs were common, with 80% of children having experienced at least one ACE by age 3. Children exposed to three or more ACEs were more than five times as likely to be diagnosed with externalizing behavior problems than children with none.

Research on early-life stress using animal models suggests a number of explanatory processes by which childhood experiences exert profound and enduring biological and behavioral effects. Using the concepts of allostatic load (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998) and biological embedding (Miller, Chen, & Parker, Reference Miller, Chen and Parker2011; Slopen, McLaughlin, Dunn, & Koenen, Reference Slopen, McLaughlin, Dunn and Koenen2013) research with both humans and animal models indicates that trauma exposure initiates biological and behavioral adaptations with short-term benefits and long-term negative consequences. One such process, allostatic load, involves biological adaptations that dysregulate the stress response system, creating hyper- or hyporesponses to future sources of stress (McEwen, Reference McEwen1998, Reference McEwen2012). Childhood trauma affects neurodevelopment, altering brain structure and function (Teicher & Samson, Reference Teicher and Samson2016; Thomason & Marusak, Reference Thomason and Marusak2017) and neuroendocrine responses (Bruce, Gunnar, Pears, & Fisher, Reference Bruce, Gunnar, Pears and Fisher2013; Danese & Lewis, Reference Danese and Lewis2017). Epigenetic responses to childhood adversity link early-life stress exposure to subsequent health and behavior disorders in animals as well as humans (Blaze & Roth, Reference Blaze and Roth2015; Gröger et al., Reference Gröger, Matas, Gos, Lesse, Poeggel, Braun and Bock2016; Lester, Conradt, & Marsit, Reference Lester, Conradt and Marsit2016). Epigenetic changes may also be transmitted from one generation to the next, resulting in intergenerational cycles of trauma (Dias & Ressler, Reference Dias and Ressler2014; Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Russig, Weiss, Gräff, Linder, Michalon and Mansuy2010; Roth, Lubin, Sodhi, & Kleinman, Reference Roth, Lubin, Sodhi and Kleinman2009). As research on childhood adversity becomes increasingly interdisciplinary, we find ourselves coming back full circle, appreciating the importance of understanding the “whole child” (Zigler & Bishop-Josef, Reference Zigler, Bishop-Josef, Singer, Golinkoff and Hirsh-Pasek2006) rather than studying separate developmental systems and biological processes. While recognizing the interconnectedness of the separate systems, however, we describe the development of each separately in the following section to better organize the information around a new model focused on how PACEs can counteract the negative effects of ACEs on development.

PACEs heart model

In a recent review of the literature on how ACEs and PACEs affect developmental trajectories (Hays-Grudo et al., Reference Hays-Grudo, Morris, Beasley, Ciciolla, Shreffler and Croff2020), we argue for a model that literally turns ACEs upside down (using a heart as a symbol of love; see Figure 1) and focus on how PACEs counteract the negative impact of ACEs on neurobiology, development, and growth (Hays-Grudo & Morris, Reference Hays-Grudo and Morris2020). We place our Heart Model next to the CDC's ACEs pyramid model. Starting from the top of the heart, we argue that if children have nurturing relationships and necessary resources to grow and thrive, protective experiences will increase secure attachment and optimal neurological, socioemotional (e.g., emotion regulation), and cognitive development (e.g., executive function). In turn, successful development leads to the adoption of healthy behaviors and the maintenance of positive relationships. Children meet developmental milestones and experience health and longevity in adulthood. We acknowledge the oversimplification of these processes and that each child has a set of unique experiences and innate genetic make-up that influence risk and resilience trajectories (see Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). However, we also see value in focusing on how positive experiences can buffer adversity and lead to successful outcomes, and we ground our thinking in scores of longitudinal studies indicating that positive, nurturing relationships and enriching resources can influence developmental pathways of children starting preconception and into young adulthood (see Hays-Grudo & Morris, Reference Hays-Grudo and Morris2020). In the next section, we provide a brief overview of the research that supports the PACEs Heart Model.

Figure 1. Heart Model: protective and compensatory experiences (PACEs) influence health and well-being via relationships and resources (Hays-Grudo & Morris, Reference Hays-Grudo and Morris2020).

Optimal Child Development

Neurological development

The brain and central nervous system undergo immense changes and development from the formation of the neural tube during the third week of gestation to the newborn brain, which has nearly 100 billion neurons and a morphology similar to that of adults (LaFreniere & MacDonald, Reference LaFreniere and MacDonald2013; Stiles & Jernigan, Reference Stiles and Jernigan2010). White matter tracts connecting brain regions form before birth, and myelination proceeds rapidly during the first two years of life (Thomason, Reference Thomason2020). The brain reaches 80% of its adult size by the second year of life, consisting primarily of increases in grey matter volume (Knickmeyer et al., Reference Knickmeyer, Gouttard, Kang, Evans, Wilber, Smith and Gilmore2008). This period of rapid growth is followed by synaptic pruning (Eltokhi, Janmaat, Genedi, Haarman, & Sommer, Reference Eltokhi, Janmaat, Genedi, Haarman and Sommer2020). Significant neurodevelopmental changes also occur during adolescence, particularly with regard to brain regions underlying socioemotional functioning. Subcortical structures such as the amygdala tend to mature earlier than the prefrontal cortex, which continues maturing well into young adulthood (Mills, Goddings, Clasen, Giedd, & Blakemore, Reference Mills, Goddings, Clasen, Giedd and Blakemore2014). The relative “mismatch” between the rates of development of subcortical structures, which underlie emotion reactivity, and cortical regions, which underlie self-regulation, has led to dual system (Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2008) and imbalance (Casey, Getz, & Galvan, Reference Casey, Getz and Galvan2008) models of adolescent brain and behavior. These models posit that increased emotionality and risk-taking behaviors during adolescence are the consequence of these differential rates of development in different brain structures.

Animal models have shown that early-life stress results in altered connectivity in frontolimbic circuitry (Cohodes, Kitt, Baskin-Sommers, & Gee, Reference Cohodes, Kitt, Baskin-Sommers and Gee2020), and similar findings have been found in children who have experienced adversity. For example, ACEs are associated with alterations in neurocircuitry important for emotion regulation (Herzberg & Gunnar, Reference Herzberg and Gunnar2020). Given the importance of emotion regulation as a core feature in many psychiatric disorders (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010), these neurobiological effects likely constitute a link between ACEs and later psychopathology. ACEs occurring in early childhood have also been associated with altered volume (Luby, Barch, Whalen, Tillman, & Belden, Reference Luby, Barch, Whalen, Tillman and Belden2017) and resting-state functional connectivity (Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen, & Luby, Reference Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen and Luby2018) of the inferior frontal gyrus, and these alterations predict poor mental and physical health outcomes in adolescents. It is important to note that the timing and type of adversity influence its effects on brain-related outcomes (Hambrick, Brawner, & Perry, Reference Hambrick, Brawner and Perry2019; Nelson & Gabard-Durnam, Reference Nelson and Gabard-Durnam2020). Advances in neuroimaging methods and analyses, including motion correction techniques useful when scanning infants and toddlers, will allow future longitudinal studies to more directly test these effects across different stages of development in humans.

While ACEs have detrimental effects on neurodevelopment, positive experiences may serve to counteract or protect against these effects to promote resilience. Animal models have found that exposure to enriched environments, which incorporate novelty, stimulation, and social opportunities, serve to limit or reverse the effects of early adversity on the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Francis, Diorio, Plotsky, & Meaney, Reference Francis, Diorio, Plotsky and Meaney2002; Kentner et al., Reference Kentner, Scalia, Shin, Migliore and Rondón-Ortiz2018; Koe, Ashokan, & Mitra, Reference Koe, Ashokan and Mitra2016). Relatively little research, however, has examined the neurobiological effects of protective experiences in humans with a history of childhood adversity. Studies of foster care as an intervention for children previously institutionalized have found that children placed in foster care exhibited more normative white matter development as compared to children who remained institutionalized (Bick et al., Reference Bick, Zhu, Stamoulis, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2015, Reference Bick, Fox, Zeanah and Nelson2017), although a similar study found persistent alterations in brain structure in adulthood despite exposure to enriched adoptive environments (Mackes et al., Reference Mackes, Golm, Sarkar, Kumsta, Rutter, Fairchild and Sonuga-Barke2020). Additional longitudinal neuroimaging studies of children are needed to understand the role of protective factors in the neurobiological effects of ACEs.

Social, emotional, and cognitive development

Numerous studies document that nurturing environments promote children's socioemotional and cognitive development. Caregiver sensitivity forms the basis of infants’ secure attachment (De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, Reference De Wolff and van IJzendoorn1997; Lucassen et al., Reference Lucassen, Tharner, Van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Volling, Verhulst and Tiemeier2011), which in turn relates to a variety of positive socioemotional outcomes, including positive peer relations (Groh et al., Reference Groh, Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Steele and Roisman2014, Reference Groh, Fearon, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg and Roisman2017; Schneider, Atkinson, & Tardif, Reference Schneider, Atkinson and Tardif2001) and emotional understanding (Cooke, Stuart-Parrigon, Movahed-Abtahi, Koehn, & Kerns, Reference Cooke, Stuart-Parrigon, Movahed-Abtahi, Koehn and Kerns2016). In early childhood, the family environment provides an important context that influences children's emerging emotion regulation abilities (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007). As children grow and develop, emotion regulation contributes to success in school (Kwon, Hanrahan, & Kupzyk, Reference Kwon, Hanrahan and Kupzyk2017) and socioemotional functioning (Eisenberg & Fabes, Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Blater and Tamis-LeMonda2006).

Childhood adversity has profound effects on children's socioemotional and cognitive development. Maltreatment has been consistently linked with emotion dysregulation in children and adolescents (Gruhn & Compas, Reference Gruhn and Compas2020), and emotion dysregulation mediates the relationship between ACEs and later psychopathology (Weissman et al., Reference Weissman, Bitran, Miller, Schaefer, Sheridan and McLaughlin2019). These effects likely reflect the impact of ACEs on the developing brain. The brain systems underlying emotion regulation are also important for broader cognitive skills, such as executive function. Executive function includes cognitive processes, such as working memory and flexibility, that are important for maintaining goal-directed behaviors and are related to outcomes such as academic performance (Cortés Pascual, Moyano Muñoz, & Quílez Robres, Reference Cortés Pascual, Moyano Muñoz and Quílez Robres2019). ACEs are associated with lower executive functioning skills in children and adults (Hawkins et al., Reference Hawkins, Ciciolla, Colaizzi, Keirns, Smith, Stout and Erato2020; Kalia & Knauft, Reference Kalia and Knauft2020; Kopetz et al., Reference Kopetz, Woerner, MacPherson, Lejuez, Nelson, Zeanah and Fox2019; Lambert, King, Monahan, & McLaughlin, Reference Lambert, King, Monahan and McLaughlin2017), as well as altered functioning in brain regions related to cognitive control (Hallowell et al., Reference Hallowell, Oshri, Liebel, Liu, Duda, Clark and Sweet2019; Lu et al., Reference Lu, Pan, Gao, Wei, Wang, Hu and Li2017; Philip et al., Reference Philip, Sweet, Tyrka, Price, Carpenter, Kuras and Niaura2013, Reference Philip, Sweet, Tyrka, Carpenter, Albright, Price and Carpenter2016).

Fortunately, positive experiences can promote resilience in children exposed to adversity. Family factors appear to be especially powerful in counteracting the effects of early adversity. A recent systematic review found that factors such as family cohesion, extended family support, positive parenting, and parental involvement protect against the development of psychopathology in adolescents and young adults with ACEs (Fritz, de Graaff, Caisley, van Harmelen, & Wilkinson, Reference Fritz, de Graaff, Caisley, van Harmelen and Wilkinson2018). Given the importance of familial context in supporting children's emotion regulation (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers and Robinson2007), it is likely that these positive family factors serve to promote children's emotion regulation, which in turn lessens the risk of psychopathology (Alink, Cicchetti, Kim, & Rogosch, Reference Alink, Cicchetti, Kim and Rogosch2009). Relationships outside of the family can serve as protective factors as well. For example, teacher–child closeness is associated with executive function in kindergarten children exposed to adversity (Suntheimer & Wolf, Reference Suntheimer and Wolf2020). In addition, as peer relationships become increasingly important in adolescence, positive peer relationships can also protect against the development of psychopathology in this age group (Collishaw et al., Reference Collishaw, Pickles, Messer, Rutter, Shearer and Maughan2007). Thus, evidence suggests that positive socioemotional experiences can promote resilience in children who have faced adversity.

Health behaviors and developmental milestones

ACEs affect neurodevelopment by violating the expected environment (Nelson & Gabard-Durnam, Reference Nelson and Gabard-Durnam2020), and these alterations in neurodevelopment can have lifelong consequences for socioemotional functioning, including that adoption of negative healthy behaviors and even early death (Anda et al., Reference Anda, Felitti, Bremner, Walker, Whitfield, Perry and Giles2006). Childhood adversity has particularly detrimental effects on the development of emotion regulation, as mentioned previously. However, protective factors, particularly positive parenting and a supportive family environment, are beneficial and may counteract the effects of ACEs on children's socioemotional development, supporting the achievement of developmental milestones and maintaining a healthy lifestyle during adulthood. Interventions aimed at building a warm and nurturing environment for children are critical for healthy development and positive developmental trajectories in the context of childhood adversity, as we discuss later in this chapter.

Interventions to Support Children and Families

In his insistence that programming for Head Start and other educational curricula focus on social and emotional development as well as cognitive development, Zigler pioneered a major shift in developmental science and education from rigorous laboratory research to more applied implementation science, and from narrowly defined models to multilevel systems models. In a sense, the research of the 21st century, with its advances in neuroimaging, molecular biology, and computational power, has validated and extended his 20th-century assertion that children are best viewed as developing systems affected by biology, behavior, families, and society. Many intervention programs that exist today are built on the concept of the whole child, and are designed to affect children's social, cognitive, and physiological regulation in the context of caregiving relationships. A number of interventions to support children's social–emotional and cognitive development have been developed that focus on the child and family system. Interventions with empirical evidence of effectiveness include home-based support for parents, center or group-based interventions for high-risk parents, and multilevel interventions for parents and other caregivers. Next, we provide some examples of such interventions

Home-based parenting programs

Home-based parenting programs are an empirically supported approach to support families (Barth, Reference Barth2009; Howard & Brooks-Gunn, Reference Howard and Brooks-Gunn2009). Parenting programs aim to help parents master their caregiving role by helping them access informal and formal support and teaching parenting behaviors, such as warmth, responsiveness, sensitivity, and appropriate discipline that encourage child–parent attachment (Berger & Font, Reference Berger and Font2015). Evidence shows that by engaging families in services early in the child's life, programs and providers are able to improve family functioning through enhancing parenting skills, social support, coping skills, and linkage to community resources (Filene, Kaminski, Valle, & Cachat, Reference Filene, Kaminski, Valle and Cachat2013; Guterman, Reference Guterman2001). Further, it is a promising approach to improve children's cognitive and social–emotional outcomes (Berger & Font, Reference Berger and Font2015). Overall, home-based parenting programs have been found to be beneficial in providing high-risk families interventions and resources to improve family functioning (Filene et al., Reference Filene, Kaminski, Valle and Cachat2013; Guterman, Reference Guterman2001).

There are numerous empirically supported home-based parenting programs, with many of these programs focusing on young children and families. These include, but are not limited to, programs such as Attachment and Biobehavioral Catchup (ABC), Nurse Family Partnership (NFP), Parents as Teachers (PAT), and SafeCare (SC). ABC is a short-term home-based parenting program (10 sessions) focused on supporting families of young children that have experienced adversity by improving positive Parent×Child interactions and reducing intrusive parenting behaviors (Dozier, Meade, & Bernard, Reference Dozier, Meade, Bernard, Timmer and Urquiza2013). Specifically, the ABC model centers around three core areas of support through live coaching. These three areas include: (a) providing nurturance to the child in times of distress, (b) following the lead of the child, and (c) eliminating behaviors that are frightful to the child. Research indicates that the ABC model is effective at enhancing parental sensitivity, children's attachment, children's biobehavioral regulatory capacity (Dozier & Bernard, Reference Dozier and Bernard2017) and parental responsiveness to infants (Bick & Dozier, Reference Bick and Dozier2013).

NFP is a home-based parenting program focused on supporting first-time mothers early in their pregnancy throughout the first year of their child's life. The NFP program is administered by nurses and centers on three goals: (a) improving prenatal health to positively influence birth outcomes, (b) improving parental care through supporting sensitive and proficient care of the child, and (c) improving parental life outcomes through family planning, educational goal attainment, and supporting parental employment (Olds, Reference Olds2006). Research regarding the effectiveness of NFP has indicated improved prenatal care of the child as seen through decreased injuries, improved emotional and language development, increased parental pregnancy planning, and increased economic self-sufficiency (Eckenrode et al., Reference Eckenrode, Campa, Morris, Henderson, Bolger, Kitzman and Olds2017; Olds, Reference Olds2006)

The PAT program is based on the belief that parents are the child's first and arguably the most influential teachers of the child (Zigler, Pfannenestiel, & Seitz, Reference Zigler, Pfannenestiel and Seitz2008). It involves home visits conducted by parent educators and is focused on supporting parents in strengthening their parenting skills, increasing child development knowledge, and preparing young children for school (Wagner & Clayton, Reference Wagner and Clayton1999). The program enrolls families with children from birth to up to three years of age, with parent educators working with families until the child is up to five years of age. The PAT program focuses on teaching families principles based on child development, positive modeling through activities, and promoting access to resources and supports. Research has indicated that the PAT program improves school readiness through improved parenting practices, increased reading to the child, and an improved likelihood that families will enroll the child in a preschool program (Pfannenstiel, Seitz, & Zigler, Reference Pfannenstiel, Seitz and Zigler2002; Zigler et al., Reference Zigler, Pfannenestiel and Seitz2008).

The SC program is an evidence-based parenting program designed to support high-risk families and reduce child abuse and neglect (Lutzker & Bigelow, Reference Lutzker and Bigelow2002). SC focuses on the family ecology in which child maltreatment transpires through understanding that maltreatment occurs from multiple factors including parental dynamics, interactions between the parent and child, family influences, and other factors related to society and culture (Dore & Lee, Reference Dore and Lee1999). The SC model focuses on key areas including: (a) child safety specific to home hazards and cleanliness, (b) child health through effective healthcare decision making, and (c) improvement of parent–infant and Parent × Child interactions. Research has shown significant improvements in key areas of child health, home safety, and Parent × Child interaction (Gershater-Molko, Lutzker, & Wesch, Reference Gershater-Molko, Lutzker and Wesch2003) and reduction in child maltreatment recidivism (Chaffin, Hecht, Bard, Silovsky, & Beasley, Reference Chaffin, Hecht, Bard, Silovsky and Beasley2012).

Although home-based parenting programs can be focused on a variety of key areas, the common theme is providing family support to reduce negative child and family outcomes. Further, home-based programs increase family engagement through providing services in the comfort and familiarity of the family home.

Center or group-based interventions

A number of center and group-based interventions have been designed to support young children and families, reducing early adversity and improving optimal child development. Circle of Security (COS) is an early intervention based on decades of attachment research. COS was created with the goal of positively influencing insecure and disorganized attachment styles in young children (Cooper, Hoffman, Powell, & Marvin, Reference Cooper, Hoffman, Powell, Marvin, Berlin, Ziv, Amaya-Jackson and Greenberg2005; Marvin, Cooper, Hoffman, & Powell, Reference Marvin, Cooper, Hoffman and Powell2002). The 20-week version of the COS program is a group-based intervention that focuses on providing parent education and therapy based on attachment theory. Specifically, the COS program introduces attachment theory, and parents watch video clips of their children in the Strange Situation to allow parents to reflect on their caregiving approach. Research has supported that the COS intervention positively influences insecure attachment patterns among toddlers (Hoffman, Marvin, Cooper, & Powell, Reference Hoffman, Marvin, Cooper and Powell2006), increases maternal sensitivity (Cassidy et al., Reference Cassidy, Ziv, Stupica, Sherman, Butler, Karfgin and Powell2010), and improves child attachment security (Yaholkoski, Hurl, & Theule, Reference Yaholkoski, Hurl and Theule2016).

The Incredible Years (IY) program is designed to be utilized by parents and teachers to work toward reducing challenging child behaviors and increasing social emotional growth and self-control. The IY program specifically works to support children in regulating emotions, improving social skills, and improving academic success (Webster-Stratton, Reference Webster-Stratton2000). Within a group setting, key intervention areas focus on setting goals, utilizing role play and self-reflection, receiving feedback from facilitators, and home or classroom activities to support positive outcomes. In terms of effectiveness, the IY program has been recognized by the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention as an effective conduct disorder prevention and treatment program for young children (Webster-Stratton, Reference Webster-Stratton2000).

The CDC's Legacy for Children™ is an excellent example of a program that supports the development of the whole child by assisting mothers and infants living in poverty. Legacy is a group-based parenting program that targets sensitive and responsive parenting, nurturing caregiving, and maternal self-efficacy. Social support is a key component of Legacy and is accomplished through peer and group leader support. Mothers are empowered to help one another by giving advice and providing support outside of the meetings. Groups targeting similar-aged children start during pregnancy or infancy and can last until children are 3 or 5 years old, depending on the curricula (see Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Hartwig, Smith, Lee, Forbes, Perou, Morris and Williamson2018). Legacy is built on a public health model that views parents as children's front-line public health workers. The focus is on preventing problems among children at risk due to poverty, and group interactions and lessons are ideal for modeling nurturing behavior and early learning through opportunities for play and social interaction (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Robinson, Hays-Grudo, Claussen, Hartwig and Treat2017). Evidence for the effectiveness of Legacy is strong. A randomized control trial found positive gains in socio-emotional and cognitive domains 3 to 6 years post-intervention (Perou et al., Reference Perou, Elliott, Visser, Claussen, Scott, Beckwith and Smith2012). Group-based interventions such as Legacy, COS, and IY are often conducted at Head Start and Early Head Start Centers. Legacy groups specifically are often held in community-focused organizations such as Catholic Charities and pediatricians’ offices (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Hartwig, Smith, Lee, Forbes, Perou, Morris and Williamson2018).

Multilevel Interventions and Integrative Models

In a model that we developed for a special issue in Child Development on maximizing resilience among children at risk for maladjustment (Luthar & Eisenberg, Reference Luthar and Eisenberg2017), we argue that promoting nurturing relationships through early intervention efforts is a key leverage point for change (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Robinson, Hays-Grudo, Claussen, Hartwig and Treat2017). In this model, the Building Early Relationships Model of Change (see Figure 2), we illustrate how family, parent, and child characteristics influence intervention implementation and subsequent parent and child outcomes through two primary mechanisms: (a) strengthening social support, and (b) increasing positive Parent × Child interactions. Specifically, we argue that there is ample scientific evidence to indicate that programs that promote supportive and nurturing relationships between caregivers and children, and caregivers and other adults (e.g., group leaders, mental health advocates) influence both parents’ and children's physical and mental health outcomes overtime (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Robinson, Hays-Grudo, Claussen, Hartwig and Treat2017). Programs that target social support can help mothers with basic caregiving needs and can empower mothers to be role models and give advice to their peers. This type of support is greatly needed in many communities where mothers are isolated and lack necessary social and material supports for basic caregiving (see Luthar & Ciciolla, Reference Luthar and Ciciolla2015; Luthar, Curlee, Tye, Engelman, & Stonnington, Reference Luthar, Curlee, Tye, Engelman and Stonnington2017). The programs described above are examples of interventions that target these two mechanisms, all with a dual-generation focus. We next discuss an example of an integrated intervention that brought together experts across disciplines to target multiple systems of care for young children.

Figure 2. Building early relationships model of change (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Robinson, Hays-Grudo, Claussen, Hartwig and Treat2017).

Tulsa Children's Project

Since its inception in the 1960s, Head Start has held fast the importance of nurturing parents as well as children. The Tulsa Children's Project (TCP) was designed, implemented, and evaluated as a highly integrated set of interventions to reduce the effects of adversity and poverty on the development of young children enrolled in the three Tulsa Educare sites (Hays-Grudo, Slocum, Root, Bosler, & Morris, Reference Hays-Grudo, Slocum, Root, Bosler, Morris, Morris and Williamson2018). Beginning in the late 2000s as a collaboration between Harvard's Center for the Developing Child, Tulsa-based researchers at OSU-Tulsa and OU-Tulsa, community partners at Tulsa Educare, and the George Kaiser Family Foundation, the multiyear project focused on increasing access to resources and strengthening relationships between parents, teachers, and staff. The TCP model included multiple components: a workforce training program for parents, curriculum and classroom support for teachers, and health promoting activities for families and staff. Enhancing mental health was woven throughout each of the components and provided a common theme.

The most intensive program for parents was a menu of workforce training programs known as EduCareers. Options for parents ranged from English-language learning classes and Graduate Equivalency Degree (GED) preparation to nursing coursework, and all included financial support for tuition, books, fees, and other expenses; life-skills training; and social support groups. Parents in the nurse training program were successful in completing the 15-week coursework and passing exams to become certified nursing assistants (CNAs). However, the success rate for those who elected to continue with Licensed Practical Nurse training was less than 20%, even with increased tutoring and other support. Conversations with participants revealed difficulties related to executive function skills, which hindered retention of information, effective planning and problem-solving, and the management of stress and negative emotions. Based on these observations and more structured assessments of executive functioning, ACEs, and current levels of stress and adversity, additional programming was added to address the cognitive and socioemotional deficits associated with parents’ own histories of abuse, neglect, and family dysfunction. These interventions included Mindfulness Based Stress Reducation (MBSR) which has been shown to have positive effects on brain regions associated with memory and self-regulation (Hölzel et al., Reference Hölzel, Carmody, Evans, Hoge, Dusek, Morgan and Lazar2010; Short, Mazmanian, Oinonen, & Mushquash, Reference Short, Mazmanian, Oinonen and Mushquash2016). Follow-up data documented significant gains in mindfulness, self-compassion, and perceived stress (Hays-Grudo et al., Reference Hays-Grudo, Slocum, Root, Bosler, Morris, Morris and Williamson2018).

Data collected from a larger sample of Educare parents also supported the need for acknowledging and addressing parents’ own history of trauma and adversity when attempting to improve children's outcomes in early childhood settings. In this study, parents completed a family health and stress survey and provided consent for the collection of salivary cortisol from children after morning drop-off and prior to afternoon pick-up (n = 200; M age = 2.57 years; SD = 1.13; range = 2 months to 5 years). The sample of children reflected the diversity of the Educare population (47% Latinx, 30% African American, 14% Caucasian, 9% other or multiple races/ethnicities). Based on previous research showing the effects of early life stress on the HPA axis (Loman & Gunnar, Reference Loman and Gunnar2010), we hypothesized that parents with higher ACE scores would exhibit more mental health symptoms, report more parent–child conflict, and have children with dysregulated cortisol patterns (Miller, Chen, & Zhou, Reference Miller, Chen and Zhou2007). As expected, parent ACE scores were positively correlated with their depressive symptoms and perceived stress, but not with parent–child conflict. However, structural equation modeling revealed that parents with higher ACE scores reported more depressive symptoms and current stress, both of which positively predicted parent–child conflict, which predicted the flatter cortisol slopes from morning to afternoon (Hays-Grudo et al., Reference Hays-Grudo, Bosler, Teague, Castle, Guss, Miller-Cribbs and Morris2016), a pattern often observed in children with histories of trauma (Bernard, Dozier, Bick, & Gordon, Reference Bernard, Dozier, Bick and Gordon2015). These data provide evidence that helping parents recover from childhood trauma is likely to yield benefits to the parent in terms of reduced mental health symptomatology, and that decreasing conflict in the parent–child relationship is key to reducing the biobehavioral dysregulation of stress associated with adversity.

The Tulsa Children's Project achieved its best results for children, parents, and teachers when each group was actively engaged in the process with researchers and interventionists from many institutions and disciplines (Hays-Grudo et al., Reference Hays-Grudo, Slocum, Root, Bosler, Morris, Morris and Williamson2018). The cultural and conceptual divides were not always easily bridged, sometimes creating conflict and confusion. However, those very conflicts provided the source for new insights into the process of applying research-driven knowledge while simultaneously listening to and learning from the parents and teachers engaged in the very real struggle to access and provide to children the nurturing relationships and resources necessary to survive and thrive in challenging environments.

Adopting a listening, reflective, relationship-building approach is precisely how researchers and clinicians alike can more meaningfully and respectfully help families and caregivers in a wide array of community environments. In a recent review of the many evidence-based interventions for high-risk youth and families, two essential elements distinguished programs that increased children's ability to rise above conditions of adversity: psychological and emotional support for their caregivers and programs that address specific dysfunctional parenting behaviors (Luthar & Eisenberg, Reference Luthar and Eisenberg2017). These themes are also consistent with the evidence-based programs identified in the recent National Research Council of the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine report on addressing adversity across a number of service sectors that help children and families (National Research Council, 2019). It is increasingly clear that the antidote to the toxic stress of ACEs is having the unconditional love of nurturing and responsive adults (Chen, Miller, Kobor, & Cole, Reference Chen, Miller, Kobor and Cole2010; Coatsworth et al., Reference Coatsworth, Duncan, Nix, Greenberg, Gayles, Bamberger and Demi2015; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Treat, Hays-Grudo, Chesher, Williamson, Mendez, Morris and Williamson2018). For parents with a history of trauma in their own childhoods, however, this is often a challenging task requiring the assistance and support of professionals and the community of caregivers, as we saw in the Tulsa Children's Project.

Becoming a Child-Oriented Society – “Are we there yet?”

In this paper, we presented two models that we believe hold promise for thinking about the future of research and intervention programming with young children and families. In the PACEs Heart Model, we illustrated how PACEs can counteract the negative impact of ACEs across development, emphasizing the need for nurturing relationships and enriching experiences to promote optimal child development across domains. In the Building Early Relationships Model of Change we described how improving nurturing relationships (by increasing positive Parent × Child interactions and social support for caregivers) can lead to positive outcomes among young children and parents living in poverty. We also presented examples of interventions that support these models and align with a public health perspective of improving the lives of children through scalable, multilevel, wide-reaching prevention efforts across communities in need (see Morris et al., Reference Morris, Robinson, Hays-Grudo, Claussen, Hartwig and Treat2017). Over the last several decades, we have certainly made progress in understanding how to best help children and families, but we still have a long way to go before our knowledge is actually used to reach all children and families in need.

In Reference Zigler1976, Zigler wrote in the New York Times that “the single greatest impediment to improving the lives of America's children is the myth that the United States is a child-oriented society.” He listed a “litany of national failures” and “massive shortcomings in regard to children” that included lack of access to prenatal and pediatric care, child hunger, school systems more focused on warehousing and punishing children than educating and inspiring them, and a welfare system that lacked the resources to help parents learn to parent and keep their children safely within the family. The lack of quality daycare was identified as a problem for millions of families who had few options other than leaving children alone and unattended, with older siblings, or in poor-quality settings.

In reading this indictment of 20th-century America's unwillingness to make meaningful investments in children, one is struck by the similarity to the challenges facing 21st-century children and families. The effects of the coronavirus pandemic and associated hardships that have stemmed from the mandates to quarantine children and nonessential workers, including social isolation, job losses, and the economic collapse of many service industries, have magnified the difficulties experienced by low- to middle-income American families. As we write this, millions of parents are without work, wondering how they will house and feed their children as the social and economic safety net woven for less urgent conditions begins to unravel. Millions more are faced with the choice of sending children to schools (many already overcrowded) that were not designed for physically distancing students, teachers, and staff, or trying to become teachers themselves while juggling their own work demands. Affordable, safe, and quality childcare is an urgent and unmet need for millions of families. Lack of access to medical care continues to be an issue, as the pandemic increasingly overwhelms state and local medical systems. It is not surprising that deaths due to COVID-19 have disproportionately affected racial and ethnic minority individuals, who are more likely to have comorbid conditions, such as diabetes, hypertension, obesity; live in more crowded conditions; lack access to testing or health care; and be employed in jobs without paid sick leave (Hooper, Nápoles, & Pérez-Stable, Reference Hooper, Nápoles and Pérez-Stable2020; Millett et al., Reference Millett, Jones, Benkeser, Baral, Mercer, Beyrer and Sherwood2020). The social isolation and economic hardships resulting from the pandemic have also increased rates of parental mental health problems and substance use, which are likely to result in increased rates of family violence, child abuse, and neglect (Humphreys, Myint, & Zeanah, Reference Humphreys, Myint and Zeanah2020). The coronavirus pandemic has exposed the cracks in the foundations of the institutions charged with helping families raise the next generation.

Yet, in spite of the historical and current lack of investments our society has made to ensure that its youngest citizens survive and thrive in spite of adversity, like Ed Zigler, we remain optimistic. We believe that change is possible and may be within our grasp. Cultural norms and attitudes are shifting, as evidenced by majority support for the Black Lives Matter movement and universal health care. As more public discourse and policies reflect the need to end institutionalized racism, opportunities increase to address human rights violations that affect children and families who have been denied basic opportunities because of race, ethnicity, sexual orientation, religion, disabilities, or gender. As recognition dawns that society as a whole, including its more affluent members, suffers when a large proportion of that society is denied access to care, popular support increases for funding of mental health, childcare, schools, and other safety net supports. COVID-19, like disasters and adverse events in the past, is drawing attention to the necessity of the social connections and institutional systems that support our children and families (Masten & Motti-Stefanidi, Reference Masten and Motti-Stefanidi2020). As we strive to respond to the challenges facing families today, we draw courage from Zigler's unstinting advocacy and tireless perseverance to improve the lives of children.

Acknowledgment

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Financial Statement

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (JH-G, grant number P20GM109097) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (ASM, grant number R34DA050343-01S2).

Conflicts of Interest

None.

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alink, L. R. A., Cicchetti, D., Kim, J., & Rogosch, F. A. (2009). Mediating and moderating processes in the relation between maltreatment and psychopathology: Mother–child relationship quality and emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 831843. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9314-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Bremner, J. D., Walker, J. D., Whitfield, C. H., Perry, B. D., … Giles, W. H. (2006). The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256, 174186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Belden, A. C., Tillman, R., Whalen, D., & Luby, J. L. (2018). Early childhood adverse experiences, inferior frontal gyrus connectivity, and the trajectory of externalizing psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57, 183190. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2017.12.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barth, R. P. (2009). Preventing child abuse and neglect with parent training: Evidence and opportunities. The Future of Children, 19, 95118. doi:10.1353/foc.0.0031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellis, M. A., Lowey, H., Leckenby, N., Hughes, K., & Harrison, D. (2014). Adverse childhood experiences: Retrospective study to determine their impact on adult health behaviours and health outcomes in a UK population. Journal of Public Health, 36, 8191. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdt038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, L. M., & Font, S. A. (2015). The role of the family and family-centered programs and policies. The Future of Children, 25, 155176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernard, K., Dozier, M., Bick, J., & Gordon, M. (2015). Intervening to enhance cortisol regulation among children at risk for neglect: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 829841. doi:10.1017/S095457941400073XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bethell, C. D., Newacheck, P., Hawes, E., & Halfon, N. (2014). Adverse childhood experiences: Assessing the impact on health and school engagement and the mitigating role of resilience. Health Affairs, 33, 21062115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bevilacqua, D., Davidesco, I., Wan, L., Chaloner, K., Rowland, J., Ding, M., … Dikker, S. (2019). Brain-to-brain synchrony and learning outcomes vary by student–teacher dynamics: Evidence from a real-world classroom electroencephalography study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31, 401411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bick, J., & Dozier, M. (2013). The effectiveness of an attachment-based intervention in promoting foster mothers’ sensitivity toward foster infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34, 116. doi:10.1002/imhj.21373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bick, J., Fox, N., Zeanah, C., & Nelson, C. A. (2017). Early deprivation, atypical brain development, and internalizing symptoms in late childhood. Neuroscience, 342, 140153. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.09.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bick, J., Zhu, T., Stamoulis, C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C., & Nelson, C. A. (2015). Effect of early institutionalization and foster care on long-term white matter development a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 169, 211219. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaze, J., & Roth, T. L. (2015). Evidence from clinical and animal model studies of the long-term and transgenerational impact of stress on DNA methylation. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology, 43, 7684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, J., Gunnar, M. R., Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2013). Early adverse care, stress neurobiology, and prevention science: Lessons learned. Prevention Science, 14, 247256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, N. J., Hellman, J. L., Scott, B. G., Weems, C. F., & Carrion, V. G. (2011). The impact of adverse childhood experiences on an urban pediatric population. Child Abuse & Neglect, 35, 408413. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2011.02.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Getz, S., & Galvan, A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Developmental Review, 28, 6277. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Stupica, B., Sherman, L. J., Butler, H., Karfgin, A., & Powell, B. (2010). Enhancing attachment security in the infants of women in a jail-diversion program. Attachment and Human Development, 12, 333353. doi:10.1080/14616730903416955CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaffin, M., Hecht, D., Bard, D., Silovsky, J., & Beasley, W. H. (2012). A statewide trial of the safe care home-based services model with parents in child protective services. Pediatrics, 129, 509515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, E., Miller, G. E., Kobor, M. S., & Cole, S. W. (2010). Maternal warmth buffers the effects of low early-life socioeconomic status on pro-inflammatory signaling in adulthood. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 729. doi:10.1038/mp.2010.53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17. doi:10.2307/1129830CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coatsworth, J. D., Duncan, L. G., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Gayles, J. G., Bamberger, K. T., … Demi, M. A. (2015). Integrating mindfulness with parent training: Effects of the mindfulness-enhanced strengthening families program. Developmental Psychology, 51, 2635. doi:10.1037/a0038212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohodes, E. M., Kitt, E. R., Baskin-Sommers, A., & Gee, D. G. (2020). Influences of early-life stress on frontolimbic circuitry: Harnessing a dimensional approach to elucidate the effects of heterogeneity in stress exposure. Developmental Psychobiology, 120. doi:10.1002/dev.21969Google ScholarPubMed
Collishaw, S., Pickles, A., Messer, J., Rutter, M., Shearer, C., & Maughan, B. (2007). Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: Evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse and Neglect, 31, 211229. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, J. E., Stuart-Parrigon, K. L., Movahed-Abtahi, M., Koehn, A. J., & Kerns, K. A. (2016). Children's emotion understanding and mother–child attachment. Emotion, 16, 11021106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., Powell, B., & Marvin, R. (2005). The circle of security intervention: Differential diagnosis and differential treatment. In Berlin, L. J., Ziv, Y., Amaya-Jackson, L. & Greenberg, M. T. (Eds.), Enhancing early attachments: Theory, research, intervention, and policy (pp. 127151). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cortés Pascual, A., Moyano Muñoz, N., & Quílez Robres, A. (2019). The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: Review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1582. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01582CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., & Lewis, S. J. (2017). Psychoneuroimmunology of early-life stress: The hidden wounds of childhood trauma? Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 99114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Wolff, M. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571591. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1997.tb04218.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dias, B. G., & Ressler, K. J. (2014). Parental olfactory experience influences behavior and neural structure in subsequent generations. Nature Neuroscience, 17, 89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dore, M. M., & Lee, J. M. (1999). The role of parent training with abusive and neglectful parents. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 48, 313325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dozier, M., & Bernard, K. (2017). Attachment and biobehavioral catch-up: Addressing the needs of infants and toddlers exposed to inadequate or problematic caregiving. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 111117. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dozier, M., Meade, E., & Bernard, K. (2013). Attachment and biobehavioral catch-up: An intervention for parents at risk of maltreating their infants and toddlers. In Timmer, S. & Urquiza, A. (Eds.), Evidence-based approaches for the treatment of maltreated children (pp. 4360). New York: Springer. doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7404-9_4Google Scholar
Eckenrode, J., Campa, M. I., Morris, P. A., Henderson, C. R., Bolger, K. E., Kitzman, H., & Olds, D. L. (2017). The prevention of child maltreatment through the nurse family partnership program: Mediating effects in a long-term follow-up study. Child Maltreatment, 22, 9299. doi.org/10.1177/1077559516685185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (2006). Emotion regulation and children's socioemotional competence. In Blater, L. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (Eds.), Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (pp. 357381). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Eltokhi, A., Janmaat, I. E., Genedi, M., Haarman, B. C. M., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2020). Dysregulation of synaptic pruning as a possible link between intestinal microbiota dysbiosis and neuropsychiatric disorders. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 98, 13351369. doi:10.1002/jnr.24616CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldman, R. (2003). Infant–mother and infant–father synchrony: The coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, R. (2017). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 8099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The adverse childhood experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 245258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filene, J. H., Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., & Cachat, P. (2013). Components associated with home visiting program outcomes: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 132, S100S109. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-1021HCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (2002). Environmental enrichment reverses the effects of maternal separation on stress reactivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 78407843. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.22-18-07840.2002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, T. B., Russig, H., Weiss, I. C., Gräff, J., Linder, N., Michalon, A., … Mansuy, I. M. (2010). Epigenetic transmission of the impact of early stress across generations. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 408415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fritz, J., de Graaff, A. M., Caisley, H., van Harmelen, A.-L., & Wilkinson, P. O. (2018). A systematic review of amenable resilience factors that moderate and/or mediate the relationship between childhood adversity and mental health in young people. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershater-Molko, R. M., Lutzker, J. R., & Wesch, D. (2003). Project SafeCare: Improving health, safety, and parenting skills in families reported for, and at-risk for child maltreatment. Journal of Family Violence, 18, 377386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gröger, N., Matas, E., Gos, T., Lesse, A., Poeggel, G., Braun, K., & Bock, J. (2016). The transgenerational transmission of childhood adversity: Behavioral, cellular, and epigenetic correlates. Journal of Neural Transmission, 123, 10371052. doi:10.1007/s00702-016-1570-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Steele, R. D., & Roisman, G. I. (2014). The significance of attachment security for children's social competence with peers: A meta-analytic study. Attachment & Human Development, 16, 103136. doi:10.1080/14616734.2014.883636CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groh, A. M., Fearon, R. M. P., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Roisman, G. I. (2017). Attachment in the early life course: Meta-analytic evidence for its role in socioemotional development. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 7076. doi:10.1111/cdep.12213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruhn, M. A., & Compas, B. E. (2020). Effects of maltreatment on coping and emotion regulation in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Child Abuse and Neglect, 103. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104446CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guterman, N. B. (2001). Sage sourcebooks for the Human Services Series: Stopping child maltreatment before it starts: Emerging horizons in early home visitation services. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. doi:10.4135/9781452220635Google Scholar
Hallowell, E. S., Oshri, A., Liebel, S. W., Liu, S., Duda, B., Clark, U. S., & Sweet, L. H. (2019). The mediating role of neural activity on the relationship between childhood maltreatment and impulsivity. Child Maltreatment, 24, 389399. doi:10.1177/1077559519835975CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hambrick, E. P., Brawner, T. W., & Perry, B. D. (2019). Timing of early-life stress and the development of brain-related capacities. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 13, 114. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, M., Ciciolla, L., Colaizzi, J., Keirns, N., Smith, C., Stout, M., … Erato, G. (2020). Adverse childhood experiences and cognitive function among adults with excess adiposity. Obesity Science & Practice, 6, 4756. doi:10.1002/osp4.385CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hays-Grudo, J., Bosler, C. D., Teague, T. K., Castle, S., Guss, S., Miller-Cribbs, J., … Morris, A. (2016). Intergenerational transmission of risk: Parents’ ACEs and young children's regulation of stress. Poster presented at the 15th World Congress of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, Prague, Czech Republic.Google Scholar
Hays-Grudo, J., & Morris, A. S. (2020). Adverse and protective childhood experiences: A developmental perspective. Washington, DC: APA Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hays-Grudo, J., Morris, A. S., Beasley, L., Ciciolla, L., Shreffler, K., & Croff, J. (2020). Integrating and synthesizing adversity and resilience knowledge and action: The ICARE model. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Hays-Grudo, J., Ratliff, E., & Morris, A. M. (in press). Adverse childhood experiences: A new framework for infant mental health. In Benson, J. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development (2nd ed., pp. 110). Cambridge, MA: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hays-Grudo, J., Slocum, R., Root, J. D., Bosler, C., & Morris, A. S. (2018). Tulsa Children's Project: Applying evidence-based interventions in early childhood settings. In Morris, A. S., & Williamson, A. C. (Eds.), Building early social and emotional relationships with infants and toddlers (pp. 277303). Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzberg, M. P., & Gunnar, M. R. (2020). Early life stress and brain function: Activity and connectivity associated with processing emotion and reward. NeuroImage, 209, 116493. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, K. T., Marvin, R. S., Cooper, G., & Powell, B. (2006). Changing toddlers’ and preschoolers’ attachment classifications: The circle of security intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 10171026. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hölzel, B., Carmody, J., Evans, K., Hoge, E., Dusek, J., Morgan, L., … Lazar, S. (2010). Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 1117. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, M. W., Nápoles, A. M., & Pérez-Stable, E. J. (2020). COVID-19 and racial/ethnic disparities. JAMA, 323, 24662467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howard, K. S., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2009). The role of home-visiting programs in preventing child abuse and neglect. The Future of Children, 19, 119146. doi:10.1353/foc.0.0032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., Myint, M. T., & Zeanah, C. H. (2020). Increased risk for family violence during the COVID-19 pandemic. Pediatrics, 145, e20200982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalia, V., & Knauft, K. (2020). Emotion regulation strategies modulate the effect of adverse childhood experiences on perceived chronic stress with implications for cognitive flexibility. PLoS One, 15, e0235412. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0235412CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kentner, A. C., Scalia, S., Shin, J., Migliore, M. M., & Rondón-Ortiz, A. N. (2018). Targeted sensory enrichment interventions protect against behavioral and neuroendocrine consequences of early life stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 98, 7485. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.07.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P., Leckman, J. F., Mayes, L. C., Feldman, R., Wang, X., & Swain, J. E. (2010). The plasticity of human maternal brain: Longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behavioral Neuroscience, 124, 695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knickmeyer, R. C., Gouttard, S., Kang, C., Evans, D., Wilber, K., Smith, J. K., … Gilmore, J. H. (2008). A structural MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 1217612182. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3479-08.2008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koe, A. S., Ashokan, A., & Mitra, R. (2016). Short environmental enrichment in adulthood reverses anxiety and basolateral amygdala hypertrophy induced by maternal separation. Translational Psychiatry, 6, e729. doi:10.1038/tp.2015.217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopetz, C., Woerner, J. I., MacPherson, L., Lejuez, C. W., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Fox, N. A. (2019). Early psychosocial deprivation and adolescent risk-taking: The role of motivation and executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 148, 388399. doi:10.1037/xge0000486CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwon, K., Hanrahan, A. R., & Kupzyk, K. A. (2017). Emotional expressivity and emotion regulation: Relation to academic functioning among elementary school children. School Psychology Quarterly, 32, 7588. doi:10.1037/spq0000166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaFreniere, P., & MacDonald, K. (2013). A post-genomic view of behavioral development and adaptation to the environment. Developmental Review, 33, 89109. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2013.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambert, H. K., King, K. M., Monahan, K. C., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2017). Differential associations of threat and deprivation with emotion regulation and cognitive control in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 929940. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000584CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T. H., Qu, Y., & Telzer, E. H. (2018). Dyadic neural similarity during stress in mother–child dyads. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 28, 121133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lester, B. M., Conradt, E., & Marsit, C. (2016). Introduction to the special section on epigenetics. Child Development, 87, 2937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loman, M. M., & Gunnar, M. R. (2010). Early experience and the development of stress reactivity and regulation in children. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 867876. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, S., Pan, F., Gao, W., Wei, Z., Wang, D., Hu, S., … Li, L. (2017). Neural correlates of childhood trauma with executive function in young healthy adults. Oncotarget, 8, 7984379853. doi:10.18632/oncotarget.20051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Barch, D., Whalen, D., Tillman, R., & Belden, A. (2017). Association between early life adversity and risk for poor emotional and physical health in adolescence a putative mechanistic neurodevelopmental pathway. JAMA Pediatrics, 171, 11681175. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.3009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucassen, N., Tharner, A., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Volling, B. L., Verhulst, F. C., … Tiemeier, H. (2011). The association between paternal sensitivity and infant-father attachment security: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Family Psychology, 25, 986992. doi:10.1037/a0025855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., & Ciciolla, L. (2015). Who mothers mommy? Factors that contribute to mothers’ well-being. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., Curlee, A., Tye, S. J., Engelman, J. C., & Stonnington, C. M. (2017). Fostering resilience among mothers under stress: “Authentic Connections Groups” for medical professionals. Women's Health Issues, 27, 382390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luthar, S. S., & Eisenberg, N. (2017). Resilient adaptation among at-risk children: Harnessing science toward maximizing salutary environments. Child Development, 88, 337349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutzker, J. R., & Bigelow, K. M. (2002). Treatment manuals for practitioners. Reducing child maltreatment: A guidebook for parent services. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mackes, N. K., Golm, D., Sarkar, S., Kumsta, R., Rutter, M., Fairchild, G., … Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2020). Early childhood deprivation is associated with alterations in adult brain structure despite subsequent environmental enrichment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117, 641649. doi:10.1073/pnas.1911264116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marvin, R., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Powell, B. (2002). The circle of security project: Attachment-based intervention with caregiver-pre-school child dyads. Attachment & Human Development, 4, 107124. doi:10.1080/ 14616730252982491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S. (2015). Ordinary magic: Resilience in development. New York: Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 425444. doi:10.1017/S0954579400005812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., & Motti-Stefanidi, F. (2020). Multisystem resilience for children and youth in disaster: Reflections in the context of COVID-19. Adversity and Resilience Science, 2, 112.Google Scholar
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 171179. doi:10.1056/NEJM199801153380307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (2012). Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1718017185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKelvey, L. M., Edge, N. C., Mesman, G. R., Whiteside-Mansell, L., & Bradley, R. H. (2018). Adverse experiences in infancy and toddlerhood: Relations to adaptive behavior and academic status in middle childhood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 82, 168177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merrick, M. T., Ford, D. C., Ports, K. A., & Guinn, A. S. (2018). Prevalence of adverse childhood experiences from the 2011–2014 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System in 23 states. JAMA Pediatrics, 172, 10381044. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.2537CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Parker, K. J. (2011). Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Zhou, E. S. (2007). If it goes up, must it come down? Chronic stress and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in humans. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 2545. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.25CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millett, G. A., Jones, A. T., Benkeser, D., Baral, S., Mercer, L., Beyrer, C., … Sherwood, J. (2020). Assessing differential impacts of COVID-19 on Black communities. Annals of Epidemiology, 47, 3744. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2020.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, K. L., Goddings, A. L., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., & Blakemore, S. J. (2014). The developmental mismatch in structural brain maturation during adolescence. Developmental Neuroscience, 36, 147160. doi:10.1159/000362328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Robinson, L. R., Hays-Grudo, J., Claussen, A. H., Hartwig, S. A., & Treat, A. E. (2017). Targeting parenting in early childhood: A public health approach to improve outcomes for children living in poverty. Child Development, 88, 388397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 361388. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x.TheCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, A. S., Treat, A., Hays-Grudo, J., Chesher, T., Williamson, A. C., & Mendez, J. (2018). Integrating research and theory on early relationships to guide intervention and prevention. In Morris, A. S. & Williamson, A. (Eds.), Building early social and emotional relationships with infants and toddlers (pp. 125). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Research Council. (2019). Vibrant and healthy kids: Aligning science, practice, and policy to advance health equity. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. doi:10.17226/25466Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & Gabard-Durnam, L. J. (2020). Early adversity and critical periods: Neurodevelopmental consequences of violating the expectable environment. Trends in Neurosciences, 43, 133143. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2020.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olds, D. (2006). The nurse-family partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27, 525. doi:10.1002/imhj.20077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perou, R., Elliott, M. N., Visser, S. N., Claussen, A. H., Scott, K. G., Beckwith, L. H., … Smith, D. C. (2012). Legacy for Children™: A pair of randomized controlled trials of a public health model to improve developmental outcomes among children in poverty. BMC Public Health, 12, 691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfannenstiel, J. C., Seitz, V., & Zigler, E. (2002). Promoting school readiness: The role of the Parents as Teachers program. NHSA Dialog, 6, 7186. doi:10.1207/s19309325nhsa0601_6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philip, N. S., Sweet, L. H., Tyrka, A. R., Carpenter, S. L., Albright, S. E., Price, L. H., & Carpenter, L. L. (2016). Exposure to childhood trauma is associated with altered n-back activation and performance in healthy adults: Implications for a commonly used working memory task. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10, 124135. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9373-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philip, N. S., Sweet, L. H., Tyrka, A. R., Price, L. H., Carpenter, L. L., Kuras, Y. I., … Niaura, R. S. (2013). Early life stress is associated with greater default network deactivation during working memory in healthy controls: A preliminary report. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 7, 204212. doi:10.1007/s11682-012-9216-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provocative Child Agency Head. (1970). New York Times. Retrieved from https://timesmachine.nytimes.com/timesmachine/1970/06/27/355647032.html?pageNumber=13Google Scholar
Raver, C. C., & Zigler, E. F. (1997). Social competence: An untapped dimension in evaluating Head Start's success. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 363385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, L. R., Hartwig, S. A., Smith, D. C., Lee, A. H., Forbes, L. W., & Perou, R.. (2018). Supporting early social and emotional relationships through a public health parenting program: The Legacy for Children™ intervention. In Morris, A. S., & Williamson, A. C. (Eds.), Building early social and emotional relationships with infants and toddlers (pp. 183211). Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, T. L., Lubin, F. D., Sodhi, M., & Kleinman, J. E. (2009). Epigenetic mechanisms in schizophrenia. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta, 1790, 869877. doi:10.1016/j.bbagen.2009.06.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1979). Fifteen thousand hours: Secondary schools and their effects on children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 316331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., Barocas, R., Zax, M., & Greenspan, S. (1987). Intelligence quotient scores of 4-year-old children: Social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics, 79, 343350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child–parent attachment and children's peer relations: A quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 37, 86100. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.1.86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Short, M., Mazmanian, D., Oinonen, K., & Mushquash, C. J. (2016). Executive function and self-regulation mediate dispositional mindfulness and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 97103. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slopen, N., McLaughlin, K. A., Dunn, E. C., & Koenen, K. C. (2013). Childhood adversity and cell-mediated immunity in young adulthood: Does type and timing matter? Brain, Behavior, Immunity, 28, 6371. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78106. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiles, J., & Jernigan, T. L. (2010). The basics of brain development. Neuropsychology Review, 20, 327348. doi:10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suntheimer, N. M., & Wolf, S. (2020). Cumulative risk, teacher–child closeness, executive function and early academic skills in kindergarten children. Journal of School Psychology, 78, 2337. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2019.11.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swain, J. E., Kim, P., & Ho, S. S. (2011). Neuroendocrinology of parental response to baby-cry. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23, 10361041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., & Samson, J. A. (2016). Annual research review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 241266. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomason, M. E. (2020). Development of brain networks in utero: Relevance for common neural disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 88, 4050. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.02.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomason, M. E., & Marusak, H. A. (2017). Toward understanding the impact of trauma on the early developing human brain. Neuroscience, 342, 5567. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, P. H., & Zigler, E. (1987). Parents and day care: The search for an alliance.Google Scholar
Wagner, M. M., & Clayton, S. L. (1999). The parents as teachers program: Results from two demonstrations. Future of Children, 9, 91115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webster-Stratton, C. (2000). The incredible years training series. Juvenile justice bulletin (pp. 123). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.Google Scholar
Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z. (1996). Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face. Child Development, 67, 905914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weissman, D. G., Bitran, D., Miller, A. B., Schaefer, J. D., Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Difficulties with emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic mechanism linking child maltreatment with the emergence of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 31, 899915. doi:10.1017/S0954579419000348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yaholkoski, A., Hurl, K., & Theule, J. (2016). Efficacy of the Circle of Security intervention: A meta-analysis. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 15, 95103. doi:10.1080/15289168.2016.1163161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E. (1970). The environmental mystique: Training the intellect versus development of the child. Childhood Education, 46, 402412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E. (1976). Children – First or Last? New York Times. Retrieved from https://timesmachine.nytimes.com/timesmachine/1976/04/25/issue.htmlGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E. F. (1989). Addressing the nation's child care crisis: The school of the twenty-first century. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 484491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zigler, E. F., & Bishop-Josef, S. J. (2006). The cognitive child versus the whole child: Lessons from 40 years of Head Start. In Singer, D. G., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (Eds.), Play = learning: How play motivates and enhances children's cognitive and social-emotional growth (pp. 1535). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E., Pfannenestiel, J. C., & Seitz, V. (2008). The parents as teachers program and school success: A replication and extension. Journal of Primary Prevention, 29, 103120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E., & Styfco, S. J. (2000). Preventing child abuse through quality early care and education: A plea for policy makers to act. UMKC Law Review, 69, 15.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Heart Model: protective and compensatory experiences (PACEs) influence health and well-being via relationships and resources (Hays-Grudo & Morris, 2020).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Building early relationships model of change (Morris et al., 2017).